1. Introduction
Symmetry and tilings have been used in art and architecture throughout human history [
1,
2,
3]. Two-dimensional periodic patterns can be classified into 17 different wallpaper groups based on the Euclidean plane isometries (translations, reflections, rotations and glides) that they possess. Islamic, Moorish and Egyptian architecture (see
Figure 1) and the art of M. C. Escher are rich with ornate examples. The Alhambra palace contains examples of most, if not all, of the wallpaper groups [
4,
5]. These patterns are also used throughout everyday Western architecture (see
Figure 2 for some examples).
The reasons why symmetries in repeating patterns present such compelling impressions to human vision have been the subject of research over many decades. Gestalt theory recognised that symmetry contributes to the “goodness” of a form, promoting its emergence through the grouping of image elements. Subsequent experimental investigations have defined goodness (also termed salience) behaviourally, in terms of accuracy and speed of discriminating symmetrical forms from non-symmetrical ones, and resistance of discrimination to degradation of symmetry by noise. In dot patterns and geometrical forms, key findings have been that mirror symmetry (around a reflection axis) is more quickly and accurately detected than either translation or rotational symmetry [
6,
7]. Mirror symmetry is detected more quickly and accurately if a pattern is reflected in a vertical axis than in a horizontal one, and less well again if reflected in oblique axes [
7]. A variety of theories have been proposed to explain these and other findings, in terms of the properties of either multiple processes of symmetry detection, or the properties of the representations of images that they generate (for a review, see [
8], and for recent models see [
9,
10,
11,
12]).
In this paper, we will not address questions about the detection of symmetries in images, but instead will investigate how symmetries affect the appearance of two-dimensional patterns. The measurement of visual appearance is important in applied contexts as a means to determine how people describe and classify objects, materials and surfaces. Our understanding of the physical basis of visual appearance is greatest in the case of colour [
13], whereas other properties have been less well explored. One of these is texture; fine-scale variation in surface height and reflectance that has constant statistical properties from one patch of a surface to another. The main perceptual dimensions along which people classify images of textures have been identified [
14,
15], and include roughness, directionality and regularity. However, progress has only been made in identifying the mathematical properties of surfaces that underlie these dimensions in a few specific cases, such as roughness in random-phase surfaces [
16], and the effects of phase randomisation on the appearance of phase-rich surfaces [
17].
Since the visual textures used in the creation or decoration of artificial surfaces often contain periodically repeating elements, it is interesting to ask how the symmetries present in such textures influence human perception of their appearance. Previous work on the appearance of patterns containing symmetries has tested their effects on aesthetic impressions, showing that there is a correlation between symmetric patterns and judgements of “beauty” in a pattern [
18], or on perceived complexity. In the second case, [
19] used “band” (or “frieze”) patterns consisting of a simple geometric form repeated (translated) along a single axis, with combinations of reflections, rotations and glide reflections defined by the seven symmetry groups that are possible for such a pattern. They concluded that processes of grouping these elements into larger perceived “motifs” partially overrode the effects of symmetry on judgements of complexity.
Here, we aim to use two-dimensional periodic patterns made up from patches of random-dot noise to measure the perceived similarity of exemplars from each of the wallpaper groups to other exemplars of the same group, and to exemplars of the other groups. To this end, a free-sorting experiment will be described in which observers were asked to sort a set of 85 patterns (
i.e., 5 examples from each of the 17 groups) into subsets of similar appearance. Similarity matrices obtained by this sorting method have previously been used to identify the dimensions underlying human classification of visual textures (e.g., [
14,
15]). it is important to emphasise that observers in our experiment were not instructed to classify the stimulus patterns into subsets according to the symmetries present in them, and nor was their attention drawn to the fact that the patterns contained multiple symmetries or that they were relevant to the experiment. Computer vision models that can extract a pattern’s symmetry group from an image have been developed [
20,
21,
22], but this is likely to be a highly skilled task for humans, completely unlike the judgements of visual similarity that we sought to obtain.
4. Results
Participants took between approximately 15 and 30 min to sort the images into subsets.
Figure 3 shows the number of subsets made by different participants, and the distribution of the number of images in subsets of different sizes. The overall similarity matrix is shown in
Figure 4.
The first question we want to ask is whether participants classified images from the same wallpaper group as being similar to each other.
Figure 5 shows how often images from a group were classed with other images from the same group. This was further analysed as follows: for each image
I, the similarity matrix was used to retrieve the
nth most similar images. A score,
was given by the number of images belonging to the same wallpaper group as
I among the
n most similar (with a maximum of 4 as there were 5 instances of each wallpaper group in the set of images). The mean results for each wallpaper group are shown in
Figure 6. As can be seen, images from the same wallpaper group are retrieved more often than would be expected by chance.
However, we can clearly see from
Figure 4 and
Figure 6 that the participants did not use only an image’s wallpaper group when making subsets, and there is considerable overlap between the different groups. The perceived similarity between wallpaper groups is shown in
Figure 7. This was constructed by collapsing the similarity matrix to obtain a value for each group, rather than each individual image. A group’s self-similarity was calculated as the mean number of times that a participant classified an image from a group with other images from the same group, divided by 4, giving a value in the range 0–1. In the figure, a group’s self-similarity is represented by the area of its shape symbol, with groups whose exemplars were consistently classified as similar to one another, such as
and
, represented by large shapes. Groups with a low degree of self-similarity, such as
and
, are represented by small shapes. The similarity of one group to another was calculated as the mean number of times that a participant classified an image from one group with an image from another, divided by 5 (since each image has five opportunities to be classified with an image in another group). In
Figure 7, similarities between groups are represented by the thicknesses of the lines connecting their shape symbols. We can see that images from
,
and
were perceived as very similar to one another. Likewise,
,
,
and
formed a coherent set of perceptually similar images.
The role of individual symmetries in determining perceived similarity was further explored with a multi-linear regression model. Each symmetry group was described using a feature vector where , and are the number of rotations, reflections and glides contained in the wallpaper group. These were then used to explore whether there is a link between the number of symmetries in a wallpaper group and its degree of self similarity. The results from the regression analysis suggest that this is not the case (, ).
A separate regression was performed on each inter-group comparison, giving 136 data points, and the dependent variables were , and for comparing wallpaper group i with group j. The results from this linear regression give a of only 0.181 (, ). Although the relationship is significant, the low value of implies that human observers do not decide whether to classify two images together or apart simply on the basis of the number of symmetries that they contain. Of the three independent variables, only was found to have a significant effect, with , .
5. Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of symmetry on texture similarity judgements. In order to minimise the effect of texton similarity, we used patches of white noise as textons. The reasoning behind this was that any perceived structure arising from the properties of the wallpaper groups would be more apparent in observers’ classifications. The results show that images from the same group were classified together by human observers more often than would be expected by chance. The similarity relationships shown in
Figure 7, and the results of the regression analyses, further indicate that the number of rotational symmetries in an image has a stronger influence on its classification than the numbers of either reflections or glides that are present. However, it is also clear from the results that images were not classified by wallpaper group, or number of rotational symmetries, alone, and that observers must have taken other visual properties into account in making classifications.
An important question in interpreting these results is their possible sensitivity to the particular random dot patterns that were repeated to create the images. It is well known that if stochastic patterns of textons such as dots are reflected around one or more two-fold symmetry axes, the groupings of textons that are perceived depend strongly on their initial arrangement. Observers’ classifications of symmetric patterns of this kind by visual similarity would clearly be dominated by specific arrangements of textons. However, the stimuli used here differed from those previously used in experiments on symmetry in containing many more repetitions of a texton (the random dot patch used as a seed), and in the fine scale at which the random dot texture was viewed (see examples of stimuli in
Figure 11,
Figure 12,
Figure 13,
Figure 14,
Figure 15,
Figure 16,
Figure 17,
Figure 18,
Figure 19,
Figure 20,
Figure 21,
Figure 22,
Figure 23,
Figure 24,
Figure 25,
Figure 26 and
Figure 27). For two reasons, we argue that the classifications of these patterns would be robust to variation in the random dot seeds.
First, the similarity matrix (see
Figure 4) shows that values in the 5 × 5 blocks of cells lying along the diagonal (ignoring cells representing self-similarities of single images) are higher than the average similarity value for pairs of images, implying that images drawn from the same wallpaper group were more often classified together in the same subset than randomly chosen pairs of images. If similarity was determined by random dot seeds alone, this pattern would not be observed.
Second, inspection of the sets of images in each wallpaper group shows that similar large-scale geometric structure emerges in all five exemplars, despite variations in detail caused by the random seed.
Figure 8,
Figure 9 and
Figure 10 show examples from groups
,
and
. Particular groups are characterised by structures such as grid patterns (e.g., PMM), striations (e.g., P2), or large geometric forms with characteristic triangular, circular or elliptical shapes (e.g., P6M). These structures are comparable to the “motifs” that [
19] argued account in part for judgements of complexity in band patterns with different symmetry properties. It is probable that the influence of wallpaper group on classification that we have observed does not operate directly through recognition of symmetries, but instead through perceptual classification of the characteristic scales and shapes of the motifs that each group generates in images, whatever random dot seed is used.
The results obtained in our experiment cannot be compared directly to those in the literature on symmetry detection, without knowing to what extent observers based their classification of images on their goodness, or salience, of the symmetries that they contained. This may well have been one factor underlying similarity judgements but, as we have seen, the motifs perceived in images were probably also important (
cf. [
19]). A further problem in comparing results is that observers had unlimited time to make classifications, and were free to move images between subsets until they were satisfied with them. Measures of reaction time from symmetry detection experiments may be relevant to any classification decisions that were made quickly, but not to those made more slowly and deliberately. For these reasons, the present data cannot be used to test models of symmetry detection. Even so, some tentative comparisons can be made with existing data on the salience of different symmetries.
Strother
et al. [
19] found that glide symmetry in band patterns made little contribution to judgements of complexity, and it appears also to have little effect on similarity judgements. The only difference between groups P1 and PG is that the latter contains a glide symmetry (neither contains reflection or rotation symmetries), and images from the two groups are frequently classified together (see
Figure 7), consistent with the hypothesis that glide symmetry is not readily visible. However, the relative importance of reflection and rotation in determining image classification is not consistent with previous results on the salience of symmetries. It is well established that reflection is more salient in dot arrays or geometrical forms than rotation (e.g., [
7]), and the same difference is seen in the perceived complexity of one-dimensional band patterns ([
19]). However, we found that the self-similarity of
(which contains only a rotation) is greater than that of
(which contains only a reflection). Furthermore, images from these two groups were very frequently classified together (see
Figure 7). Treating the results more globally, we have already noted above the negative correlation between similarity of images and difference between them in number of rotation axes, and the absence of a correlation in the case of similarity axes.
The effect of reflection and rotation symmetries on the appearance of the images that we used therefore cannot be predicted from their salience in simpler patterns, and we hypothesise instead that they are based on the distinct types of large-scale motifs characteristic of groups with different numbers of rotational symmetry axes. Those with none (, , , ) have the appearance of fine meshes or grids; those with one (, , , ) of more conspicuous grids, striations or zigzags; those with two (, , ) of larger, more conspicuous geometrical forms arranged in square arrays; those with three (, , ) of similar geometrical forms but in triangular arrays; and those with six (, ) of arrays of conspicuously large circular or hexagonal forms. Varying the number of reflection axes does not have equally consistent effects on large-scale structure.
Another feature of our results is more consistent with previous data on salience of symmetries. Wenderoth
et al. [
26] varied the number of reflection symmetry axes in a pattern, and found that symmetry in patterns with four axes was detected more quickly and accurately than in ones with two or three axes, which in turn gave superior performance to patterns with a single axis. Although, as noted above, we find an effect of number of rotation axes, our results are consistent with the more general hypothesis that perceived similarity of patterns, like their salience, is influenced by the number of symmetry axes present. It is interesting to note in this respect that if the total numbers of rotation and reflection axes in our images are considered, the five groups that contain six in all (
i.e.,
,
,
,
and
) form a tightly interconnected subset in
Figure 7.
The experiment reported here is the first to investigate the effect of symmetry properties on the perceived appearance of patterns constructed from repeated patches of random dots, using all the 17 wallpaper groups that define possible symmetries in two dimensional patterns. We have shown that images with the same symmetry properties are classified together as similar more often than would be expected by chance, and therefore that these properties have an effect on the appearance of images. We have also shown that the number of axes of rotational symmetry has a significant effect on the classification of images, but the number of reflection or glide axes does not. At this stage, it is not possible to conclude to what extent the goodness, or salience, of the different symmetries in images like these influences their perceptual classification. Further experiments that controlled the presentation time of images could resolve this question by restricting decisions to the most salient symmetries present. It will also be interesting to explore the contribution of texton similarity and scale to the perceptual classification of symmetrical patterns.