Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Fusiform Correlates of Facial Memory in Autism
Previous Article in Journal
The Impact of Precaution and Practice on the Performance of a Risky Motor Task
Previous Article in Special Issue
Hippocampal Physiology, Structure and Function and the Neuroscience of Schizophrenia: A Unified Account of Declarative Memory Deficits, Working Memory Deficits and Schizophrenic Symptoms
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia: A Selective Review of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Studies

by
Nina V. Kraguljac
,
Annusha Srivastava
and
Adrienne C. Lahti
*
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, SC 501, 1530 3rd Ave S, Birmingham, AL 35294, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Behav. Sci. 2013, 3(3), 330-347; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030330
Submission received: 9 May 2013 / Revised: 18 June 2013 / Accepted: 20 June 2013 / Published: 27 June 2013
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Learning and Memory Deficits Related to Neuropsychiatric Disorders)

Abstract

:
Schizophrenia is a complex chronic mental illness that is characterized by positive, negative and cognitive symptoms. Cognitive deficits are most predictive of long-term outcomes, with abnormalities in memory being the most robust finding. The advent of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has allowed exploring neural correlates of memory deficits in vivo. In this article, we will give a selective review of fMRI studies probing brain regions and functional networks that are thought to be related to abnormal memory performance in two memory systems prominently affected in schizophrenia; working memory and episodic memory. We revisit the classic “hypofrontality” hypothesis of working memory deficits and explore evidence for frontotemporal dysconnectivity underlying episodic memory abnormalities. We conclude that fMRI studies of memory deficits in schizophrenia are far from universal. However, the current literature does suggest that alterations are not isolated to a few brain regions, but are characterized by abnormalities within large-scale brain networks.

1. Introduction

Schizophrenia is a chronic mental illness without racial or socioeconomic prejudice. It affects approximately one percent of the population worldwide and is a leading cause for disability and premature mortality in developed countries [1,2,3].
The disorder is characterized by three complexes of clinical features. Positive symptoms present as hallucinations, delusions and disorganization in thoughts or behaviors. Negative symptoms include lack of motivation, social withdrawal, diminished emotional expression/recognition of emotions and poverty of thinking and speech. Impaired cognition, including disturbance of memory, is present in approximately 75–85% of patients; patients typically score about one standard deviation lower on standardized assessments than would be expected for the general population [4,5,6].
Deficits are reported at illness onset and possibly even predate clinical manifestation; impairments are pervasive and may worsen with illness progression [7,8,9,10,11]. Cognitive dysfunction also is reported in high risk individuals and unaffected family members of patients with schizophrenia, suggesting significant genetic contribution [12,13,14]. If cognitive operations are disturbed, constructing something meaningful from incoming information may become inefficient [15]. Taken together, cognitive deficits may be one of the core abnormalities of schizophrenia that set the stage for later emergence of psychosis [16]. Not surprisingly, the extent of cognitive deficits is most predictive of long-term outcome and social functioning [17,18].
Cognitive deficits are most prominent in areas, such as working memory and episodic memory, attention, language comprehension, processing speed and executive function [19,20,21]; a meta-analysis reported the effect sizes for dysfunction to be largest in learning and memory [5]. Despite heterogeneity of the illness and the wide range of deficits, a number of findings appear to be consistent throughout the literature. Memory is the cognitive domain showing the most pronounced deficits; with working memory and episodic memory appearing to be primarily affected [22,23,24]. Importantly, the deficits are not just due to distractive positive symptoms, poor concentration or medication effects [25,26,27].
The underlying etiopathogenesis of memory deficits is believed to be complex and to widely affect neural circuitry. It appears that patients with schizophrenia may differ in the way in which they organize neural activity, maintain levels of activation in large-scale networks and functionally integrate neural circuits when compared to healthy controls. Abnormalities causing aberrant circuitry could be located (1) within a single region of the brain, (2) within a specific network or (3) at the level of interconnected networks [28,29]. While strictly localized pathophysiology may account for some aspects, memory deficits, abnormalities in interaction or integration of networks may hold a more sufficient explanation for the complexity of clinical presentation [30].
The advent of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has provided a new opportunity to gain insight to neuronal activity and neural circuitry in vivo without requiring the subject to ingest substances, be exposed to radiation or undergo invasive procedures. Classic fMRI paradigms measure the change in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal evoked by specific tasks. The BOLD signal is a surrogate marker of neural activity; an increase in the signal is thought to reflect higher activation in the brain [31]. More recently, low frequency fluctuations of the BOLD signal during rest, i.e., in absence of a specific task, have been employed to investigate temporal correlations across cortical areas that reflect functionally related brain networks [32,33].
To appreciate functional abnormalities that are present in patients with schizophrenia, one must first understand the general construct of memory. It is generally accepted that multiple independent, but interacting, memory systems exist [34,35]. These are conceptualized as organized structures of elementary operating components that consist of a neural substrate and its cognitive or behavioral correlates. While some components are shared by some or all systems, others are unique to specific memory systems [36]. Several steps in the process of memory formation have been identified: (1) encoding (information converted into a construct that is stored in the brain) (2) manipulation and maintenance of information and (3) retrieval (access of stored information). Neural substrates of both long- and short-term memory systems include the prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe (MTL), implicating a relationship between short-term memory and long-term memory [37].
While it is generally accepted that patients with schizophrenia often do poorly on memory tests, the exact nature of these deficits has a long history of being subject to debate [38,39]. Much of the empirical evidence comes from studies contrasting different memory systems in patients with schizophrenia to those of healthy controls [40], with an apparent pattern of working memory and episodic memory impairment, but less disruption of verbal short-term memory and procedural memory is seen in schizophrenia [41,42].
Working memory was originally defined as an active short-term process consisting of a central executive and a modality-specific slave system and can be conceptualized as a temporary store, whose contents are continually modified in response to immediate processing demands, but do not necessarily translate into long-term storage [43,44]. General consensus for the neuroanatomy of normal working memory includes the prefrontal cortex; however, it remains controversial if the hippocampal formation is contributing to working memory processes or if hippocampal activation found in some studies is because paradigms exceed the working memory span and start to involve long-term memory processes [45].
Episodic memory is a category of long-term memory that was originally defined as a record of a person’s experience that includes temporally dated information and spatio-temporal relations [46]. The neuroanatomy of episodic memory is less clearly delineated, but is thought to consist of a widely distributed network of cortical and subcortical brain regions and to be crucially dependent on the MTL, with contributions from the prefrontal cortex [47,48,49].
Importantly, these systems show similar activation patterns with tasks, suggesting that they share basic processing components [50,51,52]. Within the prefrontal cortex, the dorsolateral, ventrolateral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices are recruited in both working memory and episodic memory tasks [50,53,54]. The anterior cingulate cortex supports dynamic adjustments in cognitive control, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex is associated with item-specific information processing and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is linked to processing of relations among items [55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62]. While evidence suggests that some basic memory processes are shared across systems, system-specific activation patterns are also present [53]. For example, working memory is associated with larger dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation and episodic memory tasks elicit specific frontopolar activations [50,63,64]. When both memory systems are investigated in the same group of healthy controls, a common fronto-parieto-cerebellar network for working memory and episodic retrieval with common and specific activation of subregions in the prefrontal cortex and common activation of the MTL was identified [64].
Not surprisingly, these are the very areas that neuroimaging research of memory deficits in schizophrenia has most widely explored, but the nature of functional abnormalities during memory tasks in patients with schizophrenia remains controversial, because of inconsistencies in reports. The goal of this article is to give a selective review of fMRI studies that explore patterns of neural activation of memory processes in patients with schizophrenia. Evidence of neural abnormalities will be discussed in the context of two systems widely affected in schizophrenia; working memory and episodic memory [65,66].

2. Overview of fMRI Studies Relevant to Memory in Schizophrenia

2.1. Working Memory Deficits

The prefrontal cortex is one of the most widely explored regions in functional imaging studies of working memory. Reduced activation patterns in the DLPFC in schizophrenia in working memory tasks appear to be most widely replicated; with the extent of reduction being correlated with poorer performance [67,68]. For example, Callicott et al. report a failure of activation of the DLPFC in five of six patients with schizophrenia using a working memory task [69]. These abnormalities were later confirmed in groups of chronically ill and first episode patients, in medicated and medication naive subjects [70,71,72,73]. The failure of activity increases during task in medicated patients subsequently was reported only in high, but not in low, levels of working memory load. Not surprisingly, an association between decreased task performance and failure of activation was observed [74]. However, several studies are inconsistent with the finding of decreased prefrontal cortex activation during working memory tasks. No abnormalities in DLPFC activation patterns were found in acutely ill, medicated patients and hospitalized subjects in partial remission [75,76], and even hyperactivation was reported in mildly to moderately ill chronic patients with schizophrenia [77,78,79].
It has been suggested that the inconsistencies in the literature could be related to task requirements or may be explained by poor task performance in schizophrenia [28,80]. Many argue for the conceptualization of task related activation patterns as a non-linear, inverted U-shaped function that relates the fMRI signal to working memory load and is shifted to the left in patients with schizophrenia. Assuming an initial relative overactivation of this region with relatively low memory demands, a decline in processing capacity with increasing demand would then be accompanied by a relative underactivation [81,82,83]. A disruption in this activation-performance relationship in schizophrenia has indeed been reported [28,84]. Furthermore, a left shift of the curve was confirmed in a study of ten chronically medicated patients reaching peak activation on the working memory system at lower processing loads than healthy controls and a subsequent decline of DLPFC activity at high processing loads [85].
Honey et al. attempted to control for possible task performance confounds by recruiting 20 male patients with intact performance on a low-load verbal memory task and matched controls. In controls, functional response and behavioral performance correlated, but a de-coupling between these parameters was present in schizophrenia [86]. Accounting for performance differences on measures of cognition between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls is a challenging problem. Controlling for these differences has been handled in different ways, including limiting analyses to correct trials or blocks of trials with acceptable accuracy, using task performance as a covariate, matching groups based on performance or adjusting disparity between task presentations to individual ability levels [75,83,87,88,89,90,91].
Interestingly, it appears that even patients who are able to keep up with processing demands tend to engage greater levels of activation or a less focused cortical activity state, thus responding to demands less efficiently [92,93]. Ragland et al. report that, while healthy participants respond to increased processing demands through an increase in DLPFC activity, patients with schizophrenia did not show higher DLPFC activity, but diffusely engaged a number of cortical and subcortical regions to meet the same demands [94]. In addition to reduced activation of the right DLPFC, an abnormal correlation between the left DLPFC and the left hippocampal formation that was not present in healthy controls was seen in a group of unmedicated patients, again underscoring widespread neural abnormalities [95]. In a quantitative meta-analysis of twelve studies using a working memory paradigm, Glahn et al. report clear support for hypofrontality, but also consistently increased activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and frontal pole in patients [67]. Consistent with this, aberrant frontotemporal functional connectivity patterns, with dorsal prefrontal/anterior cingulate hypoactivity were described in working memory tasks in patients compared to healthy controls [96]. Interestingly, an increase in functional connectivity between the DLPFC and hippocampus was found in patients [97]. Taken together, these findings could be suggestive of a disruption in frontal-based top-down cognitive control function and resulting compensatory response to support alternative strategies in task performance in patients with schizophrenia. In a meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia that also included a subset of studies using working memory tasks, Minzenberg et al. report widespread abnormalities of activation patterns, including deficits in the middle frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus in patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy controls. To maintain performance, patients may increase other processes including mnemonic, attentional and performance monitoring functions that manifest as relative hyperactivation in aforementioned regions; i.e., less focused cortical activity [98].
Given the heritability of working memory deficits, several studies investigated the association between processing efficiency and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), a known schizophrenia risk gene, during a working memory task. The authors did find the Val allele to be associated with a reduced level of performance and reduced efficiency of the physiological response in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a finding that presumably is mediated by reducing signal to noise through compromising postsynaptic impact of evoked dopamine response [93,99,100,101,102,103]. When dopamine D1 receptor availability in the DLPFC was assessed in unmedicated patients, a relationship between increased receptor availability and worse working memory performance was identified, further substantiating the role of dopamine abnormalities in working memory deficits [104].
Several studies investigated effects of antipsychotic medications, which act by blocking dopamine receptors, on brain activation patterns in patients with schizophrenia. In a longitudinal study, Wolf et al. do report enhanced bilateral frontotemporal function after 7–8 weeks of multimodal antipsychotic treatment that was associated with improved accuracy in a verbal working memory task and improvement of psychotic symptoms [105]. Schlagenauf et al. report activation deficits during a working memory task in the DLPFC, but did not find significant changes in activation after switching patients from a typical antipsychotic to olanzapine. Surprisingly, they do report a normalization of frontal lobe activity when switching to aripiprazole, a partial dopamine agonist [106,107]. In another group of first-episode, medication naive patients, the baseline activation deficits in the DLPFC were unchanged after ten weeks of treatment with different second generation antipsychotics, an effect that was driven by medication non-responders. Patients who did respond to medications had DLPFC activation levels that were much more similar to that of healthy controls [108,109]. Other studies report improvement of baseline activation deficit in the DLPFC after a twelve week course of quetiapine and after 6–8 weeks of risperidone or olanzapine [81,110]. Consistent with this, Honey et al. did report improvement of functional activation by a working memory task in the right DLPFC within six weeks of switching from typical antipsychotics to risperidone and hypothesized that this finding may be due to reduced dopamine D2 receptor antagonism in the nigrostriatal system, which might enhance frontal cortical activity by reducing inhibitory outputs [111]. Contrastingly, regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in the anterior cingulate cortex were corrected in patients treated with clozapine, but not haloperidol, suggesting differential mechanisms in mediating neural activation between these drugs [112].

2.2. Episodic Memory Deficits

A substantially smaller number of functional neuroimaging studies have been conducted with the goal to elucidate neural correlates of episodic memory deficits in schizophrenia. In the prefrontal cortex, activation deficits have most commonly been reported [73,113,114,115,116], but also increased activation, especially in the frontal pole, has been reported [113,116]. In addition to prefrontal abnormalities, some studies have also reported reduced activation of the MTL [73,115], with others reporting no change [87], or increased activation in this area [114,116].
Given that abnormalities appear to involve both the prefrontal cortex and MTL, a disruption in the frontotemporal network has been proposed [22]. In 14 patients with schizophrenia, Ragland et al. found evidence of this, reporting prefrontal activation deficits and parahippocampal over activation during encoding [114]. Evidence of reduced left inferior frontal activation during encoding and reduced anterior cingulate cortex and temporal lobe activation, but no reduction of hippocampal activation was later reported in a group of stable, medicated patients [117]. Similar to this, Hofer et al. report an activation deficit in the DLPFC and anterior cingulate cortex in encoding and impairments in DLPFC activation during retrieval. When encoding and recognition were contrasted, attenuated frontotemporal activation in patients with schizophrenia was apparent, despite intact behavioral performance [118]. Later, they also reported activation failure in the frontal, posterior cingulate and retrosplenial regions during encoding and reduced activation in the DLPFC and paralimbic regions during retrieval in acutely psychotic patients [119]. When patients were given organizational strategies through levels-of processing paradigms, patients had similar behavioral effects compared to healthy controls in a word encode and retrieve task that was accompanied by increased ventrolateral prefrontal activation during encoding in both groups. Overactivation in the thalamus, hippocampus and lingual cortex were present during encoding and overactivation of the left frontal pole with decreased activation of the right prefrontal cortex during retrieval were observed in patients with schizophrenia [116]. Hippocampal activity during deep encoding along with reduced anterior cingulate cortex and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex activation led to the hypothesis that an activation deficit in the anterior cingulate could result in insufficient top-down modulation of attention, resulting in impaired encoding performance [120]. Given the association between increased hippocampal activity, positive symptoms, and impaired memory performance, it is conceivable that increased neural activity is necessary to compensate for dysfunctional mesolimbic circuitry that results in a hyperdopaminergic state [121,122,123]. Both experimental and computational evidence suggests that excitation of the hippocampus can lead to excitation of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area and, in turn, leads to increased release of dopamine in the hippocampus, possibly resulting in positive symptoms and memory impairment [121,124,125].
Meta-analytic evidence of episodic memory studies initially suggested reduced right hippocampal activation along with activation deficits of the left inferior frontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex during encoding in patients with schizophrenia and a deactivation of the hippocampus, but hyperactivation of the parahippocampal gyrus with activation deficits of the anterior cingulate cortex, the inferior frontal cortex, middle frontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex during retrieval [126]. However, a later meta-analysis investigating neuroimaging correlates of episodic memory deficits in schizophrenia did report less prefrontal activation in the frontal pole, DLPFC and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices during encoding and less DLPFC and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during retrieval, but did not confirm reduced hippocampal or surrounding medial temporal lobe activation in patients during encoding or retrieval. They did find a relative increase in activation in the parahippocampal gyrus during encoding and retrieval, which may reflect a compensatory mechanism, with overall greater distribution in activation abnormalities in retrieval than encoding [127]. Discrepancies in findings of these meta-analyses may be associated with the difference in study inclusion criteria; with the latter excluding region-of-interest studies that the former had included.
Genetic contributions to functional abnormalities with episodic memory tasks have been confirmed in a study of patients with schizophrenia and healthy siblings who demonstrated similar patterns of reduced hippocampal-parahippocampal activation with a task, suggesting a susceptibility-related phenotype [128]. In a similar approach, using a visual task, incremental increase in BOLD responses in schizophrenia compared with first-degree relatives and healthy controls in prefrontal regions, thalamus and insula were reported during retrieval, but no activation differences were noted during encoding, again highlighting a possible genetic component [129]. When directly testing effects of neuregulin, neurogranin and dystrobrevin-binding protein 1, all genes previously identified as increasing risk for schizophrenia have been demonstrated to modulate brain activation during episodic memory processing in healthy controls [130,131,132]. However, these links remain to be confirmed in patients with schizophrenia. An association that has been suggested to be relevant is the schizophrenia risk gene, COMT, that also been implicated in BOLD signal activation during working memory tasks. A genotype by diagnosis interaction has been demonstrated in investigating parahippocampal activation during encoding in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls [133]. This finding again implies an alteration in dopamine signaling that could be related to observed abnormalities in brain activation patterns.
Given this and the above discussed, evidence, albeit, not undisputed, of antipsychotic medications possibly affecting brain activation patterns in working memory, it is conceivable that these medications may also have modulating effects on activation during episodic memory tasks. While fMRI studies have enrolled subjects who were medicated [114,115,116,117,118] and unmedicated [119], there is a paucity of longitudinal investigations with the goal to elucidate changes of activation patterns induced by antipsychotic medications.

2.3. Shared Abnormalities in Working Memory and Episodic Memory

Much of the work investigating memory deficits in schizophrenia has conceptualized working memory and episodic memory as two independent constructs and studied them individually. Given the shared neural substrates of both these memory systems in healthy controls and the evidence that prefrontal cortical abnormalities contribute to both working memory and episodic memory deficits, Barch et al. chose to test the hypothesis that abnormal prefrontal cortex activation contribute to deficits in both memory systems. Indeed, the authors found impaired activation in the right DLPFC in patients with schizophrenia in both tasks. Interestingly, all functional abnormalities observed in the working memory task were also present in the episodic memory task [73]. Congruent with this, working memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia were found to account for deficits in long-term recall in a behavioral study [134], but others report that these are differential core deficits in schizophrenia [135]. In a study investigating the relationship between neural processing related to working memory and long-term memory, Ragland et al. found that the DLPFC was activated during a working memory task, but that activation was less focal in patients compared to controls and that this activation did not translate to higher success in long-term memory processes. The authors further report that patients showed disproportional impairment in recognizing familiarity for items compared to general long-term memory deficits, emphasizing the processing of relational information in working memory [94]. It is conceivable that abnormal prefrontal cortex and MTL activation patterns and aberrant functional connectivity between these regions could be related to DLPFC dysfunction with subsequent compensatory mechanisms that result in difficulties both working memory and long-term memory deficits [136,137]. However, more investigations need to be conducted to clarify if both memory systems share a common neural substrate.

3. Conclusions

Cognitive dysfunction is a critical and enduring feature of schizophrenia. Advances in neuroimaging methodology allow the study of memory processes in vivo and have made a tremendous contribution to our understanding of the functional alterations in the brain in schizophrenia.
Numerous studies have been conducted to investigate neural correlates of two major memory systems affected in schizophrenia; working memory and episodic memory. Neural substrates of working memory deficits confirmed abnormalities in prefrontal activation patterns; most widely replicated in the DLPFC. Studies addressing the dynamic range of activation patterns in context of memory load suggest a non-linear, inverted U-shaped physiological response. In episodic memory, a disruption in frontotemporal connectivity is apparent. Interestingly, it seems that some of the activation abnormalities are shared in both memory systems, suggesting common neurophysiological underpinnings of working memory and episodic memory deficits in patients with schizophrenia.
Emerging data suggests that dynamic changes occur in functional networks in response to underlying abnormalities in schizophrenia, but that these resulting networks are dysfunctional [28,92]. Compensatory mechanisms in schizophrenia may result in a limited improvement of performance through reweighting disturbed neural networks, but this circuitry is likely to be unstable and may fail to adapt to increasing cognitive demands [92].
Understanding the underlying neurobiology of memory deficits in schizophrenia remains a challenge, because functional magnetic resonance imaging studies are far from universal. Whether this is related to disease heterogeneity, differential response to pharmacologic treatment, effects of performance differences between groups, experimental design, image acquisition parameters or other factors yet to be elucidated is unclear. Clearly, brain regions are differentially affected in schizophrenia, but we lack an overarching mechanistic model of memory deficits that is inclusive of different memory systems, the relationship between memory systems and the modulatory capacity or non-memory-related neural networks [48]. It is critical to not only advance the understanding on the functional architecture of working memory dysfunction, but also to investigate the mechanisms that allow manipulation of related networks, potentially improving the treatment of schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (grant R01 MH081014, ACL).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Wildgust, H.J.; Hodgson, R.; Beary, M. The paradox of premature mortality in schizophrenia: New research questions. J. Psychopharmacol. 2010, 24, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Kilbourne, A.M.; Morden, N.E.; Austin, K.; Ilgen, M.; McCarthy, J.F.; Dalack, G.; Blow, F.C. Excess heart-disease-related mortality in a national study of patients with mental disorders: Identifying modifiable risk factors. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatr. 2009, 31, 555–563. [Google Scholar]
  3. Saha, S.; Chant, D.; McGrath, J. A systematic review of mortality in schizophrenia: Is the differential mortality gap worsening over time? Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 2007, 64, 1123–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Saykin, A.J.; Gur, R.C.; Gur, R.E.; Mozley, P.D.; Mozley, L.H.; Resnick, S.M.; Kester, D.B.; Stafiniak, P. Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 1991, 48, 618–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Heinrichs, R.W.; Zakzanis, K.K. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1998, 12, 426–445. [Google Scholar]
  6. Lee, J.; Park, S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2005, 114, 599–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Napal, O.; Ojeda, N.; Elizagarate, E.; Pena, J.; Ezcurra, J.; Gutierrez, M. The course of the schizophrenia and its impact on cognition: A review of literature. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 2012, 40, 198–220. [Google Scholar]
  8. Lesh, T.A.; Niendam, T.A.; Minzenberg, M.J.; Carter, C.S. Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: Mechanisms and meaning. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011, 36, 316–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Davidson, M.; Reichenberg, A.; Rabinowitz, J.; Weiser, M.; Kaplan, Z.; Mark, M. Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am. J. Psychiatr. 1999, 156, 1328–1335. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brewer, W.J.; Francey, S.M.; Wood, S.J.; Jackson, H.J.; Pantelis, C.; Phillips, L.J.; Yung, A.R.; Anderson, V.A.; McGorry, P.D. Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2005, 162, 71–78. [Google Scholar]
  11. Sponheim, S.R.; Jung, R.E.; Seidman, L.J.; Mesholam-Gately, R.I.; Manoach, D.S.; O'Leary, D.S.; Ho, B.C.; Andreasen, N.C.; Lauriello, J.; Schulz, S.C. Cognitive deficits in recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2010, 44, 421–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cosway, R.; Byrne, M.; Clafferty, R.; Hodges, A.; Grant, E.; Abukmeil, S.S.; Lawrie, S.M.; Miller, P.; Johnstone, E.C. Neuropsychological change in young people at high risk for schizophrenia: Results from the first two neuropsychological assessments of the edinburgh high risk study. Psychol. Med. 2000, 30, 1111–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Egan, M.F.; Goldberg, T.E.; Gscheidle, T.; Weirich, M.; Rawlings, R.; Hyde, T.M.; Bigelow, L.; Weinberger, D.R. Relative risk for cognitive impairments in siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 2001, 50, 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Simon, A.E.; Cattapan-Ludewig, K.; Zmilacher, S.; Arbach, D.; Gruber, K.; Dvorsky, D.N.; Roth, B.; Isler, E.; Zimmer, A.; Umbricht, D. Cognitive functioning in the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophr. Bull. 2007, 33, 761–771. [Google Scholar]
  15. Yates, A.J. Psychological deficit. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1966, 17, 111–144. [Google Scholar]
  16. Lewis, D.A. Cortical circuit dysfunction and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia—Implications for preemptive interventions. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 1871–1878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Green, M.F. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatr. 1996, 153, 321–330. [Google Scholar]
  18. Liddle, P.F. Schizophrenic syndromes, Cognitive performance and neurological dysfunction. Psychol. Med. 1987, 17, 49–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Heaton, R.; Paulsen, J.S.; McAdams, L.A.; Kuck, J.; Zisook, S.; Braff, D.; Harris, J.; Jeste, D.V. Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics. Relationship to age, Chronicity, And dementia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 1994, 51, 469–476. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fioravanti, M.; Carlone, O.; Vitale, B.; Cinti, M.E.; Clare, L. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2005, 15, 73–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fioravanti, M.; Bianchi, V.; Cinti, M.E. Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: An updated metanalysis of the scientific evidence. BMC Psychiatr. 2012, 12, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Weiss, A.P.; Heckers, S. Neuroimaging of declarative memory in schizophrenia. Scand. J. Psychol. 2001, 42, 239–250. [Google Scholar]
  23. Aleman, A.; Hijman, R.; de Haan, E.H.; Kahn, R.S. Memory impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatr. 1999, 156, 1358–1366. [Google Scholar]
  24. Rushe, T.M.; Woodruff, P.W.; Murray, R.M.; Morris, R.G. Episodic memory and learning in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 1999, 35, 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Goldberg, T.E.; Weinberger, D.R. Effects of neuroleptic medications on the cognition of patients with schizophrenia: A review of recent studies. J. Clin. Psychiatr. 1996, 57 (Suppl. 9), 62–65. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hoff, A.L.; Riordan, H.; O'Donnell, D.W.; Morris, L.; DeLisi, L.E. Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients. Am. J. Psychiatr. 1992, 149, 898–903. [Google Scholar]
  27. Landro, N.I. Memory function in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1994, 90, 87–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Callicott, J.H.; Mattay, V.S.; Verchinski, B.A.; Marenco, S.; Egan, M.F.; Weinberger, D.R. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: More than up or down. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003, 160, 2209–2215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Fornito, A.; Zalesky, A.; Pantelis, C.; Bullmore, E.T. Schizophrenia, Neuroimaging and connectomics. NeuroImage 2012, 62, 2296–2314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Friston, K.J.; Frith, C.D. Schizophrenia: A disconnection syndrome? Clin. Neurosci. 1995, 3, 89–97. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ogawa, S.; Lee, T.M.; Kay, A.R.; Tank, D.W. Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1990, 87, 9868–9872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Raichle, M.E.; MacLeod, A.M.; Snyder, A.Z.; Powers, W.J.; Gusnard, D.A.; Shulman, G.L. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 676–682. [Google Scholar]
  33. Biswal, B.; Yetkin, F.Z.; Haughton, V.M.; Hyde, J.S. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995, 34, 537–541. [Google Scholar]
  34. Tulving, E.; Schacter, D.L. Priming and human memory systems. Science 1990, 247, 301–306. [Google Scholar]
  35. Gabrieli, J.D. Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1998, 49, 87–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Tulving, E. How many memory systems are there? Am. Psychol. 1985, 40, 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ranganath, C.; Heller, A.; Cohen, M.X.; Brozinsky, C.J.; Rissman, J. Functional connectivity with the hippocampus during successful memory formation. Hippocampus 2005, 15, 997–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Saykin, A.J.; Shtasel, D.L.; Gur, R.E.; Kester, D.B.; Mozley, L.H.; Stafiniak, P.; Gur, R.C. Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 1994, 51, 124–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Goldberg, T.E.; Ragland, J.D.; Torrey, E.F.; Gold, J.M.; Bigelow, L.B.; Weinberger, D.R. Neuropsychological assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 1990, 47, 1066–1072. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ruiz, J.C.; Soler, M.J.; Fuentes, I.; Tomas, P. Intellectual functioning and memory deficits in schizophrenia. Compr. Psychiatr. 2007, 48, 276–282. [Google Scholar]
  41. McKenna, P.J.; Omstein, T.; Baddeley, A.D. Schizophrenia, 2nd ed.; John Wiley and Sons: West Sussex, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  42. Al-Uzri, M.M.; Reveley, M.A.; Owen, L.; Bruce, J.; Frost, S.; Mackintosh, D.; Moran, P.M. Measuring memory impairment in community-based patients with schizophrenia. Case-control study. Br. J. Psychiatry 2006, 189, 132–136. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kim, D.I.; Manoach, D.S.; Mathalon, D.H.; Turner, J.A.; Mannell, M.; Brown, G.G.; Ford, J.M.; Gollub, R.L.; White, T.; Wible, C.; et al. Dysregulation of working memory and default-mode networks in schizophrenia using independent component analysis, an fbirn and mcic study. Human Brain Mapp. 2009, 30, 3795–3811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Beddeley, A. Working Memory; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  45. Jeneson, A.; Squire, L.R. Working memory, Long-term memory, And medial temporal lobe function. Learn. Mem. 2012, 19, 15–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tulving, E. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  47. Tulving, E.; Markowitsch, H.J. Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 1998, 8, 198–204. [Google Scholar]
  48. Leavitt, V.M.; Goldberg, T.E. Episodic memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2009, 19, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Heckers, S.; Curran, T.; Goff, D.; Rauch, S.L.; Fischman, A.J.; Alpert, N.M.; Schacter, D.L. Abnormalities in the thalamus and prefrontal cortex during episodic object recognition in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 2000, 48, 651–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Nyberg, L.; Marklund, P.; Persson, J.; Cabeza, R.; Forkstam, C.; Petersson, K.M.; Ingvar, M. Common prefrontal activations during working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 371–377. [Google Scholar]
  51. Schacter, D.L.; Wagner, R.L.; Buckner, R.L. Memory Systems of 1999; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  52. Cabeza, R.; Nyberg, L. Imaging cognition ii: An empirical review of 275 pet and fmri studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000, 12, 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Nyberg, L.; Forkstam, C.; Petersson, K.M.; Cabeza, R.; Ingvar, M. Brain imaging of human memory systems: Between-systems similarities and within-system differences. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002, 13, 281–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Duncan, J.; Owen, A.M. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000, 23, 475–483. [Google Scholar]
  55. Owen, S. Devising a checklist to evaluate the non-verbal aspects of teaching skills and delivery. Nurse Educ. Today 1992, 12, 392–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fletcher, P.C.; Henson, R.N. Frontal lobes and human memory: Insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain: J. Neurol. 2001, 124, 849–881. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kerns, J.G.; Cohen, J.D.; MacDonald, A.W., 3rd; Johnson, M.K.; Stenger, V.A.; Aizenstein, H.; Carter, C.S. Decreased conflict- and error-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex in subjects with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2005, 162, 1833–1839. [Google Scholar]
  58. Botvinick, M.M.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Carter, C.S.; Cohen, J.D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 624–652. [Google Scholar]
  59. Badre, D.; Wagner, A.D. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 2883–2901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Blumenfeld, R.S.; Ranganath, C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 916–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Wagner, A.D.; Maril, A.; Bjork, R.A.; Schacter, D.L. Prefrontal contributions to executive control: Fmri evidence for functional distinctions within lateral prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage 2001, 14, 1337–1347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Carter, C.S.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Botvinick, M.M.; Noll, D.; Cohen, J.D. Anterior cingulate cortex, Error detection, And the online monitoring of performance. Science 1998, 280, 747–749. [Google Scholar]
  63. Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Kelley, W.M.; Buckner, R.L.; Cohen, N.J.; Miezin, F.M.; Snyder, A.Z.; Ollinger, J.M.; Akbudak, E.; Conturo, T.E.; et al. Direct comparison of prefrontal cortex regions engaged by working and long-term memory tasks. NeuroImage 2001, 14, 48–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Cabeza, R.; Dolcos, F.; Graham, R.; Nyberg, L. Similarities and differences in the neural correlates of episodic memory retrieval and working memory. NeuroImage 2002, 16, 317–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Ranganath, C.; Minzenberg, M.J.; Ragland, J.D. The cognitive neuroscience of memory function and dysfunction in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 2008, 64, 18–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Grasby, P.M.; Frith, C.D.; Friston, K.J.; Bench, C.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Dolan, R.J. Functional mapping of brain areas implicated in auditory—Verbal memory function. Brain 1993, 116 (Pt 1), 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  67. Glahn, D.C.; Ragland, J.D.; Abramoff, A.; Barrett, J.; Laird, A.R.; Bearden, C.E.; Velligan, D.I. Beyond hypofrontality: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 60–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Van Snellenberg, J.X.; Torres, I.J.; Thornton, A.E. Functional neuroimaging of working memory in schizophrenia: Task performance as a moderating variable. Neuropsychology 2006, 20, 497–510. [Google Scholar]
  69. Callicott, J.H.; Ramsey, N.F.; Tallent, K.; Bertolino, A.; Knable, M.B.; Coppola, R.; Goldberg, T.; van Gelderen, P.; Mattay, V.S.; Frank, J.A.; et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain mapping in psychiatry: Methodological issues illustrated in a study of working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1998, 18, 186–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Mendrek, A.; Kiehl, K.A.; Smith, A.M.; Irwin, D.; Forster, B.B.; Liddle, P.F. Dysfunction of a distributed neural circuitry in schizophrenia patients during a working-memory performance. Psychol. Med. 2005, 35, 187–196. [Google Scholar]
  71. Menon, V.; Anagnoson, R.T.; Mathalon, D.H.; Glover, G.H.; Pfefferbaum, A. Functional neuroanatomy of auditory working memory in schizophrenia: Relation to positive and negative symptoms. NeuroImage 2001, 13, 433–446. [Google Scholar]
  72. Barch, D.M.; Sheline, Y.I.; Csernansky, J.G.; Snyder, A.Z. Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: Specificity to schizophrenia compared with major depression. Biol. Psychiatr. 2003, 53, 376–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Barch, D.M.; Csernansky, J.G.; Conturo, T.; Snyder, A.Z. Working and long-term memory deficits in schizophrenia: Is there a common prefrontal mechanism? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2002, 111, 478–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Perlstein, W.M.; Carter, C.S.; Noll, D.C.; Cohen, J.D. Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2001, 158, 1105–1113. [Google Scholar]
  75. Walter, H.; Vasic, N.; Hose, A.; Spitzer, M.; Wolf, R.C. Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia compared to healthy controls and patients with depression: Evidence from event-related fmri. NeuroImage 2007, 35, 1551–1561. [Google Scholar]
  76. Walter, H.; Wunderlich, A.P.; Blankenhorn, M.; Schafer, S.; Tomczak, R.; Spitzer, M.; Gron, G. No hypofrontality, But absence of prefrontal lateralization comparing verbal and spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2003, 61, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Manoach, D.S.; Press, D.Z.; Thangaraj, V.; Searl, M.M.; Goff, D.C.; Halpern, E.; Saper, C.B.; Warach, S. Schizophrenic subjects activate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task, as measured by fmri. Biol. Psychiatr. 1999, 45, 1128–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Manoach, D.S.; Gollub, R.L.; Benson, E.S.; Searl, M.M.; Goff, D.C.; Halpern, E.; Saper, C.B.; Rauch, S.L. Schizophrenic subjects show aberrant fmri activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during working memory performance. Biol. Psychiatr. 2000, 48, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Callicott, J.H.; Bertolino, A.; Mattay, V.S.; Langheim, F.J.; Duyn, J.; Coppola, R.; Goldberg, T.E.; Weinberger, D.R. Physiological dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia revisited. Cereb. Cortex 2000, 10, 1078–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Weinberger, D.R.; Berman, K.F. Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: Confounds and controversies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1996, 351, 1495–1503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Mendrek, A.; Laurens, K.R.; Kiehl, K.A.; Ngan, E.T.; Stip, E.; Liddle, P.F. Changes in distributed neural circuitry function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry 2004, 185, 205–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Callicott, J.H.; Mattay, V.S.; Bertolino, A.; Finn, K.; Coppola, R.; Frank, J.A.; Goldberg, T.E.; Weinberger, D.R. Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional mri. Cereb. Cortex 1999, 9, 20–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Manoach, D.S. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction during working memory performance in schizophrenia: Reconciling discrepant findings. Schizophr. Res. 2003, 60, 285–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Karlsgodt, K.H.; Glahn, D.C.; van Erp, T.G.; Therman, S.; Huttunen, M.; Manninen, M.; Kaprio, J.; Cohen, M.S.; Lonnqvist, J.; Cannon, T.D. The relationship between performance and fmri signal during working memory in patients with schizophrenia, Unaffected co-twins, and control subjects. Schizophr. Res. 2007, 89, 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Jansma, J.M.; Ramsey, N.F.; van der Wee, N.J.; Kahn, R.S. Working memory capacity in schizophrenia: A parametric fmri study. Schizophr. Res. 2004, 68, 159–171. [Google Scholar]
  86. Honey, G.D.; Bullmore, E.T.; Sharma, T. De-coupling of cognitive performance and cerebral functional response during working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2002, 53, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Avsar, K.B.; Stoeckel, L.E.; Bolding, M.S.; White, D.M.; Tagamets, M.A.; Holcomb, H.H.; Lahti, A.C. Aberrant visual circuitry associated with normal spatial match-to-sample accuracy in schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Res. 2011, 193, 138–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Henseler, I.; Falkai, P.; Gruber, O. A systematic fmri investigation of the brain systems subserving different working memory components in schizophrenia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009, 30, 693–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Meda, S.A.; Stevens, M.C.; Folley, B.S.; Calhoun, V.D.; Pearlson, G.D. Evidence for anomalous network connectivity during working memory encoding in schizophrenia: An ica based analysis. PloS One 2009, 4, e7911. [Google Scholar]
  90. Potkin, S.G.; Turner, J.A.; Brown, G.G.; McCarthy, G.; Greve, D.N.; Glover, G.H.; Manoach, D.S.; Belger, A.; Diaz, M.; Wible, C.G.; et al. Working memory and dlpfc inefficiency in schizophrenia: The fbirn study. Schizophr. Bull. 2009, 35, 19–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Callicott, J.H.; Egan, M.F.; Mattay, V.S.; Bertolino, A.; Bone, A.D.; Verchinksi, B.; Weinberger, D.R. Abnormal fmri response of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitively intact siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003, 160, 709–719. [Google Scholar]
  92. Tan, H.Y.; Callicott, J.H.; Weinberger, D.R. Dysfunctional and compensatory prefrontal cortical systems, Genes and the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17 (Suppl. 1), i171–i181. [Google Scholar]
  93. Egan, M.F.; Goldberg, T.E.; Kolachana, B.S.; Callicott, J.H.; Mazzanti, C.M.; Straub, R.E.; Goldman, D.; Weinberger, D.R. Effect of comt val108/158 met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 6917–6922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ragland, J.D.; Blumenfeld, R.S.; Ramsay, I.S.; Yonelinas, A.; Yoon, J.; Solomon, M.; Carter, C.S.; Ranganath, C. Neural correlates of relational and item-specific encoding during working and long-term memory in schizophrenia. NeuroImage 2012, 59, 1719–1726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.S.; Olsen, R.K.; Kohn, P.D.; Brown, T.; Egan, M.F.; Weinberger, D.R.; Berman, K.F. Regionally specific disturbance of dorsolateral prefrontal-hippocampal functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 2005, 62, 379–386. [Google Scholar]
  96. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Poline, J.B.; Kohn, P.D.; Holt, J.L.; Egan, M.F.; Weinberger, D.R.; Berman, K.F. Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2001, 158, 1809–1817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Wolf, R.C.; Vasic, N.; Sambataro, F.; Hose, A.; Frasch, K.; Schmid, M.; Walter, H. Temporally anticorrelated brain networks during working memory performance reveal aberrant prefrontal and hippocampal connectivity in patients with schizophrenia. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatr. 2009, 33, 1464–1473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Minzenberg, M.J.; Laird, A.R.; Thelen, S.; Carter, C.S.; Glahn, D.C. Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 2009, 66, 811–822. [Google Scholar]
  99. Servan-Schreiber, D.; Printz, H.; Cohen, J.D. A network model of catecholamine effects: Gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science 1990, 249, 892–895. [Google Scholar]
  100. Goldberg, T.E.; Egan, M.F.; Gscheidle, T.; Coppola, R.; Weickert, T.; Kolachana, B.S.; Goldman, D.; Weinberger, D.R. Executive subprocesses in working memory: Relationship to catechol-o-methyltransferase val158met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatr. 2003, 60, 889–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Nichols, T.; Callicott, J.H.; Ding, J.; Kolachana, B.; Buckholtz, J.; Mattay, V.S.; Egan, M.; Weinberger, D.R. Impact of complex genetic variation in comt on human brain function. Mol. Psychiatr. 2006, 11, 867–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Kohn, P.D.; Kolachana, B.; Kippenhan, S.; McInerney-Leo, A.; Nussbaum, R.; Weinberger, D.R.; Berman, K.F. Midbrain dopamine and prefrontal function in humans: Interaction and modulation by comt genotype. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 594–596. [Google Scholar]
  103. Weinberger, D.R.; Egan, M.F.; Bertolino, A.; Callicott, J.H.; Mattay, V.S.; Lipska, B.K.; Berman, K.F.; Goldberg, T.E. Prefrontal neurons and the genetics of schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 2001, 50, 825–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Abi-Dargham, A.; Mawlawi, O.; Lombardo, I.; Gil, R.; Martinez, D.; Huang, Y.; Hwang, D.R.; Keilp, J.; Kochan, L.; Van Heertum, R.; et al. Prefrontal dopamine d1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 3708–3719. [Google Scholar]
  105. Wolf, R.C.; Vasic, N.; Hose, A.; Spitzer, M.; Walter, H. Changes over time in frontotemporal activation during a working memory task in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2007, 91, 141–150. [Google Scholar]
  106. Schlagenhauf, F.; Wustenberg, T.; Schmack, K.; Dinges, M.; Wrase, J.; Koslowski, M.; Kienast, T.; Bauer, M.; Gallinat, J.; Juckel, G.; et al. Switching schizophrenia patients from typical neuroleptics to olanzapine: Effects on bold response during attention and working memory. Eur. neuropsychopharmacol. 2008, 18, 589–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Schlagenhauf, F.; Dinges, M.; Beck, A.; Wustenberg, T.; Friedel, E.; Dembler, T.; Sarkar, R.; Wrase, J.; Gallinat, J.; Juckel, G.; et al. Switching schizophrenia patients from typical neuroleptics to aripiprazole: Effects on working memory dependent functional activation. Schizophr. Res. 2010, 118, 189–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Van Veelen, N.M.; Vink, M.; Ramsey, N.F.; van Buuren, M.; Hoogendam, J.M.; Kahn, R.S. Prefrontal lobe dysfunction predicts treatment response in medication-naive first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2011, 129, 156–162. [Google Scholar]
  109. Van Veelen, N.M.; Vink, M.; Ramsey, N.F.; Kahn, R.S. Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in medication-naive schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2010, 123, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Meisenzahl, E.M.; Scheuerecker, J.; Zipse, M.; Ufer, S.; Wiesmann, M.; Frodl, T.; Koutsouleris, N.; Zetzsche, T.; Schmitt, G.; Riedel, M.; et al. Effects of treatment with the atypical neuroleptic quetiapine on working memory function: A functional mri follow-up investigation. Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 2006, 256, 522–531. [Google Scholar]
  111. Honey, G.D.; Bullmore, E.T.; Soni, W.; Varatheesan, M.; Williams, S.C.; Sharma, T. Differences in frontal cortical activation by a working memory task after substitution of risperidone for typical antipsychotic drugs in patients with schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1999, 96, 13432–13437. [Google Scholar]
  112. Lahti, A.C.; Holcomb, H.H.; Weiler, M.A.; Medoff, D.R.; Frey, K.N.; Hardin, M.; Tamminga, C.A. Clozapine but not haloperidol re-establishes normal task-activated rcbf patterns in schizophrenia within the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004, 29, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Weiss, A.P.; Schacter, D.L.; Goff, D.C.; Rauch, S.L.; Alpert, N.M.; Fischman, A.J.; Heckers, S. Impaired hippocampal recruitment during normal modulation of memory performance in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 2003, 53, 48–55. [Google Scholar]
  114. Ragland, J.D.; Gur, R.C.; Valdez, J.; Turetsky, B.I.; Elliott, M.; Kohler, C.; Siegel, S.; Kanes, S.; Gur, R.E. Event-related fmri of frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2004, 161, 1004–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Ragland, J.D.; Gur, R.C.; Raz, J.; Schroeder, L.; Kohler, C.G.; Smith, R.J.; Alavi, A.; Gur, R.E. Effect of schizophrenia on frontotemporal activity during word encoding and recognition: A pet cerebral blood flow study. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2001, 158, 1114–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Ragland, J.D.; Gur, R.C.; Valdez, J.N.; Loughead, J.; Elliott, M.; Kohler, C.; Kanes, S.; Siegel, S.J.; Moelter, S.T.; Gur, R.E. Levels-of-processing effect on frontotemporal function in schizophrenia during word encoding and recognition. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2005, 162, 1840–1848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Hutcheson, N.L.; Reid, M.A.; White, D.M.; Kraguljac, N.V.; Avsar, K.B.; Bolding, M.S.; Knowlton, R.C.; den Hollander, J.A.; Lahti, A.C. Multimodal analysis of the hippocampus in schizophrenia using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophr. Res. 2012, 140, 136–142. [Google Scholar]
  118. Hofer, A.; Weiss, E.M.; Golaszewski, S.M.; Siedentopf, C.M.; Brinkhoff, C.; Kremser, C.; Felber, S.; Fleischhacker, W.W. An fmri study of episodic encoding and recognition of words in patients with schizophrenia in remission. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003, 160, 911–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Hofer, A.; Weiss, E.M.; Golaszewski, S.M.; Siedentopf, C.M.; Brinkhoff, C.; Kremser, C.; Felber, S.; Fleischhacker, W.W. Neural correlates of episodic encoding and recognition of words in unmedicated patients during an acute episode of schizophrenia: A functional mri study. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003, 160, 1802–1808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Zierhut, K.; Bogerts, B.; Schott, B.; Fenker, D.; Walter, M.; Albrecht, D.; Steiner, J.; Schutze, H.; Northoff, G.; Duzel, E.; et al. The role of hippocampus dysfunction in deficient memory encoding and positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Res. 2010, 183, 187–194. [Google Scholar]
  121. Lisman, J.E.; Grace, A.A. The hippocampal-vta loop: Controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron 2005, 46, 703–713. [Google Scholar]
  122. Lisman, J.E.; Coyle, J.T.; Green, R.W.; Javitt, D.C.; Benes, F.M.; Heckers, S.; Grace, A.A. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2008, 31, 234–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Lisman, J. Excitation, inhibition, local oscillations, or large-scale loops: What causes the symptoms of schizophrenia? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2012, 22, 537–544. [Google Scholar]
  124. Legault, M.; Rompre, P.P.; Wise, R.A. Chemical stimulation of the ventral hippocampus elevates nucleus accumbens dopamine by activating dopaminergic neurons of the ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 1635–1642. [Google Scholar]
  125. Lisman, J.; Grace, A.A.; Duzel, E. A neohebbian framework for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late ltp. Trends Neurosci. 2011, 34, 536–547. [Google Scholar]
  126. Achim, A.M.; Lepage, M. Episodic memory-related activation in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br. J. Psychiatry 2005, 187, 500–509. [Google Scholar]
  127. Ragland, J.D.; Laird, A.R.; Ranganath, C.; Blumenfeld, R.S.; Gonzales, S.M.; Glahn, D.C. Prefrontal activation deficits during episodic memory in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2009, 166, 863–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Di Giorgio, A.; Gelao, B.; Caforio, G.; Romano, R.; Andriola, I.; D'Ambrosio, E.; Papazacharias, A.; Elifani, F.; Bianco, L.L.; Taurisano, P.; et al. Evidence that hippocampal-parahippocampal dysfunction is related to genetic risk for schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2012, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  129. Stolz, E.; Pancholi, K.M.; Goradia, D.D.; Paul, S.; Keshavan, M.S.; Nimgaonkar, V.L.; Prasad, K.M. Brain activation patterns during visual episodic memory processing among first-degree relatives of schizophrenia subjects. NeuroImage 2012, 63, 1154–1161. [Google Scholar]
  130. Krug, A.; Krach, S.; Jansen, A.; Nieratschker, V.; Witt, S.H.; Shah, N.J.; Nothen, M.M.; Rietschel, M.; Kircher, T. The effect of neurogranin on neural correlates of episodic memory encoding and retrieval. Schizophr. Bull. 2013, 39, 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Krug, A.; Markov, V.; Krach, S.; Jansen, A.; Zerres, K.; Eggermann, T.; Stocker, T.; Shah, N.J.; Nothen, M.M.; Treutlein, J.; et al. The effect of neuregulin 1 on neural correlates of episodic memory encoding and retrieval. NeuroImage 2010, 53, 985–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Thimm, M.; Krug, A.; Markov, V.; Krach, S.; Jansen, A.; Zerres, K.; Eggermann, T.; Stocker, T.; Shah, N.J.; Nothen, M.M.; et al. The impact of dystrobrevin-binding protein 1 (dtnbp1) on neural correlates of episodic memory encoding and retrieval. Human Brain Mapp. 2010, 31, 203–209. [Google Scholar]
  133. Di Giorgio, A.; Caforio, G.; Blasi, G.; Taurisano, P.; Fazio, L.; Romano, R.; Ursini, G.; Gelao, B.; Bianco, L.L.; Papazacharias, A.; et al. Catechol-o-methyltransferase val(158)met association with parahippocampal physiology during memory encoding in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 1721–1731. [Google Scholar]
  134. Stone, M.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Stebbins, G.T.; Sullivan, E.V. Working and strategic memory deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 1998, 12, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Palmer, B.W.; Savla, G.N.; Fellows, I.E.; Twamley, E.W.; Jeste, D.V.; Lacro, J.P. Do people with schizophrenia have differential impairment in episodic memory and/or working memory relative to other cognitive abilities? Schizophr. Res. 2010, 116, 259–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Van Snellenberg, J.X. Working memory and long-term memory deficits in schizophrenia: Is there a common substrate? Psychiatr. Res. 2009, 174, 89–96. [Google Scholar]
  137. Barch, D.M.; Ceaser, A. Cognition in schizophrenia: Core psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kraguljac, N.V.; Srivastava, A.; Lahti, A.C. Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia: A Selective Review of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Studies. Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 330-347. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030330

AMA Style

Kraguljac NV, Srivastava A, Lahti AC. Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia: A Selective Review of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Studies. Behavioral Sciences. 2013; 3(3):330-347. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030330

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kraguljac, Nina V., Annusha Srivastava, and Adrienne C. Lahti. 2013. "Memory Deficits in Schizophrenia: A Selective Review of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) Studies" Behavioral Sciences 3, no. 3: 330-347. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030330

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop