Next Article in Journal
Screening and Characterization of Phenolic Compounds and Their Antioxidant Capacity in Different Fruit Peels
Next Article in Special Issue
Identification of the Volatile Compounds and Sensory Attributes of Long-Term Aging Vin Santo Wine from Malvasia di Candia Aromatic Grapes
Previous Article in Journal
Prediction of Walnut Deterioration Using Kernel Oxidative Stability
Previous Article in Special Issue
Pre-Fermentation Water Addition to High-Sugar Shiraz Must: Effects on Wine Composition and Sensory Properties
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impact of Bottle Aging on the Composition and Sensory Properties of Flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz Wines

School of Agriculture, Food and Wine, Waite Campus, The University of Adelaide, PMB 1, Glen Osmond, SA 5064, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2020, 9(9), 1208; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9091208
Submission received: 13 August 2020 / Revised: 25 August 2020 / Accepted: 27 August 2020 / Published: 1 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Chemical and Sensory Analysis of Alcoholic Beverages)

Abstract

:
Natural flavorings could potentially be used to enhance the intensity of wine aroma and flavor; albeit since flavor additives are not legally permitted winemaking aids, flavored wines would need to be labeled as wine products. In this study, changes in the composition and sensory profiles of flavored Chardonnay (n = 2) and Shiraz (n = 2) wines were compared at bottling, and then again after 12 months of bottle aging. Flavorings and flavored wines were also analyzed by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) to determine the key constituents responsible for changes to aroma and flavor profiles. However, many of the volatile compounds identified in flavor additives were not detected at appreciably higher concentrations in flavored wines, which was attributed to the very small quantities of flavorings that were added to base wines. The sensory profiles of control and flavored wines were determined by descriptive analysis, and the addition of flavorings to base wines significantly influenced the perception of some sensory attributes. Flavored Chardonnay wines exhibited enhanced fruit aromas and flavors, while fruit and developed attributes were enhanced in flavored Shiraz wines. Differences in sensory profiles were less apparent in Chardonnay wines following bottle aging, but depending on the flavorings added, flavored Shiraz wines could still be discriminated from their corresponding control wines after bottle aging. Results from this study demonstrate the potential for flavor additives to be used to enhance desirable attributes and/or mitigate wine sensory deficiencies.

1. Introduction

Aroma and flavor intensity are important indicators of wine quality, and can be attributed to the presence of volatile compounds derived from grapes, primary and secondary fermentation, maturation and/or aging [1,2]. Whereas, flavor additives can be used to moderate aroma and flavor intensity in food and beverage production [3], regulations governing the use of additives and processing aids in most wine-producing countries prevent their use during winemaking. In Australia, the addition of flavorings to wine renders it a ‘wine product’, according to the Australian and New Zealand Food Standard Code [4]. However, research suggests a significant proportion of consumers (up to 50%) do not understand the meaning of this term, and they may, therefore, be misled by ‘wine product’ labeling [5]. Whilst it is unlikely that flavor additives would ever be used in the production of premium quality wines, they might offer winemakers the ability to moderate the sensory properties of commodity wines or address wines with sensory deficiencies. For example, flavorings could be used to: (i) Enhance the aroma and flavor intensity of wines affected by adverse weather conditions; (ii) mask undesirable ‘green’ (unripe) characters; or (iii) introduce oak-related characters to wine, without the investment in time or capital associated with traditional barrel maturation. Recent studies have, therefore, explored consumer attitudes towards the addition of flavorings to wine [6] and the potential for flavor additives to influence the sensory profiles and consumer acceptance of wine [7].
An online survey (with 1031 participants) concerning attitudes towards the use of additives in food and wine production found Australian wine consumers were generally more accepting of the addition of natural flavorings to wine, than many of the (legally permitted) additives currently used in winemaking (e.g., tartaric acid and sulfur dioxide) [6]. A subsequent study evaluated consumer liking of control (unflavored) and flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz wines, and identified consumer segments who accepted, and in some cases preferred, the flavored wines [7]. The flavorings added to wines were chosen according to findings from previous studies on wine consumers’ flavor preferences [8,9], with flavor combinations optimized via bench-top trials, then refined using feedback obtained from consumers via focus panels [7].
The composition of wine evolves with time, as a consequence of chemical transformations that occur after bottling [10]. These compositional changes can be either desirable or undesirable. For example, hydrolysis of esters during aging of wine can result in the loss of varietal expression, i.e., a decrease in the intensity of fruity, floral characters [11,12], while the formation of phenylacetaldehyde and methional, due to oxidative effects can give rise to over-ripe fruit or cooked vegetable notes [13]. In other cases, pleasant toasty, biscuit, honey, nutty, and/or toffee characters may arise with the aging of white wine [14,15,16]. Similarly, temporal changes in the volatile compounds present in red wines can result in primary fruit characters giving way to caramel, savory, truffle, leather, chocolate, cedar, and/or coffee developed notes [17,18]. Aging of red wine is also associated with modifications in the wine color and mouthfeel properties, due to reactions of polyphenolic compounds [19]. Whereas, some wine styles benefit from extended aging in the bottle, many inexpensive (commodity) wines are intended to be consumed soon after bottling, when varietal expression is at its peak. The stability of flavor additives in the acidic wine medium has not yet been investigated, but the sensory perception of flavorings would be expected to diminish with time. As such, this study sought to determine the impact of bottle aging on the composition and sensory profiles of flavored wines, using a combination of gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) and descriptive sensory analysis.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Flavorings and Reagents

Flavor additives were sourced from the Product Makers Pty. Ltd. (Melbourne, VIC, Australia; chocolate1039, cinnamon1525, custard1989, orange1883, raspberry228 and vanilla1729) and FlavorSense Corporation (San Rafael, CA, USA; apricotWW3, berry8819, butter10-1206, honey, oak and passion fruit77116). Analytical grade reagents, solvents, and standards used in GC-MS analysis were purchased from Sigma Aldrich (Castle Hill, NSW, Australia), CDN Isotopes (Pointe-Claire, QC, Canada) and Chem-Supply (Gillman, SA, Australia).

2.2. Preparation and Aging of Wines

The preparation of flavored wines has previously been reported in full [7]. Briefly, four inexpensive commercial wines (retailing at ≤ AUD$10 per 750 mL bottle), two 2011 Chardonnay wines from the Riverland and South Eastern Australia (hereafter CH1 and CH2) and two 2011 Shiraz wines, both from South Eastern Australia (hereafter SH1 and SH2), were sourced from Australian wineries and spiked with different combinations of flavorings (Table S1) to generate two flavored versions of each wine (on a 20 L scale). Flavor combinations were selected and optimized based on consumer surveys, bench-top trials, and focus panels described previously [6,7]. Control and flavored wines were then bottled (in 375 mL dark green colored glass bottles) under metal screwcap closures, with minimal ullage (<1–2 mL) and carbon dioxide blanketing, and cellared (in an upright position), in darkness at 15 °C.

2.3. Basic Wine Composition

The pH, titratable acidity (TA, as g/L of tartaric acid) alcohol (% v/v), residual sugar (as g/L of glucose and fructose) and volatile acidity (VA, as g/L of acetic acid) of wines were measured (in duplicate) according to published methodology [20]. Results from chemical analyses performed five weeks after bottling (hereafter ‘t = 0’) were reported previously [7]; in the current study, analyses were repeated following 12 months bottle aging of wines (hereafter ‘t = 1’).

2.4. Volatile Composition of Flavorings and Wines

2.4.1. Sample Preparation

For analysis of flavorings, flavor additives (2–3 drops, approx. 0.1 g) were added to 20 mL screw-cap autosampler vials (Sigma Aldrich), together with Milli-Q water (5 mL) and sodium chloride (2.0 g). Vials were sealed and thoroughly mixed with a vortex mixer prior to GC-MS analysis. For analysis of flavored wines, wine (0.5 mL) was placed in a 20 mL screw-cap autosampler vial containing sodium chloride (2.0 g) and Milli-Q water (4.5 mL) and 2-octanol (10 µL, 50 mg/L in ethanol) added as an internal standard. Vials were sealed and thoroughly mixed using a vortex mixer prior to GC-MS analysis. Flavorings were analyzed five weeks after bottling (i.e., at t = 0), whereas wines were analyzed five weeks after bottling, then again following 12 months of bottle aging (i.e., at t = 0 and t = 1).

2.4.2. GC-MS Instrumentation

Samples were analyzed by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS), with a 7890A GC coupled to a 5975C inert XL mass selective detector (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) and equipped with a Gerstel MPS2 Multipurpose autosampler (Gerstel, Mülheim an der Ruhr, Germany). Instrument control and data analysis were performed with Agilent ChemStation software (E.02.02.1431, Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) and Gerstel MASter software (version 1.3, Lasersan Australiasia Pty. Ltd., Robina, QLD, Australia). Samples were incubated with agitation for 10 min at 50 °C, prior to headspace solid-phase micro-extraction (HS-SPME) for 30 min at 50 °C (with agitation) using a Supelco 50/30 µm divinylbenzene/carboxen/polydimethylsiloxane 1 cm SPME fiber. The SPME fiber was desorbed in the GC inlet containing an ultra-inert glass SPME liner (straight taper with 0.75 mm i.d.), operating in splitless mode at a temperature of 240 °C. The SPME fiber remained in the inlet for 10 min, but with a purge, flow to split vent of 20 mL/min after 3 min. Separation of volatile compounds was achieved using an Agilent J&W DB-WAX capillary column (60 m × 0.25 mm i.d. × 0.25 µm) with ultrapure helium (Coregas, Cavan, SA, Australia) as the carrier gas at a constant flow rate of 1.5 mL/min. The oven program was as follows: 40 °C (held for 5 min), increased to 210 °C at 2 °C/min (held for 5 min), and then to 240 °C at 5 °C/min (held for 10 min), giving a total runtime of 111 min. The MS was operated using positive ion electron impact at 70 eV in either full scan mode (m/z 35–350) or select ion monitoring (SIM), with MS source and quad temperatures of 230 °C and 150 °C, respectively. The MS transfer line was held at 240 °C. SIM parameters were as follows: Group 1 (Start time 0.00 min) m/z 43.1, 70.1, 71.1, 86.0, 88.1, 101.1 and 116.1; Group 2 (start time 18.01 min) m/z 68.1, 79.1, 93.0 and 136.1; Group 3 (start time 30.00 min) m/z 39.1, 41.1, 55.1, 57.1, 67.1, 70.1, 71.1, 82.1, 83.1, 84.1, 89.1, 93.1, 95.0, 96.0, 105.0, 106.0, 121.1, 129.1 and 136.1; Group 4 (start time 48.00 min) m/z 59.1, 65.1, 69.1, 91.1, 93.1, 104.1, 121.1, 123.1, 136.1, 138.1, 156.1, 163.0, 164.1 and 192.1; Group 5 (start time of 62.00 min) m/z 43.1, 55.1, 57.1, 65.1, 77.1, 85.0, 91.1, 92.1, 93.1, 103.1, 104.1, 121.1, 122.1, 128.1, 131.1, 132.1, 135.1, 136.1, 147.1, 176.1, 177.1, and 192.1. Ions in groups 1 and 2 had a dwell time of 100 ms, while those in groups 3, 4, and 5 had a dwell time of 50 ms. Compound identification was achieved using the National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST) 05 Mass Spectral library database and by comparing retention times and mass spectra with those of reference standards (Table S2), when available. Compound peak areas were corrected relative to 2-octanol.

2.5. Sensory Analysis of Wines

The sensory profiles of control and flavored wines were determined by descriptive analysis (DA). The sensory analyses performed five weeks after bottling (i.e., at ‘t = 0’) have previously been described in full [7]; in the current study, analyses were repeated following 12 months bottle aging of wines (i.e., at ‘t = 1’).
A DA panel comprising twelve panelists (seven females and five males, aged between 22 and 60 years), all of whom had previous DA experience and six of whom participated in DA at t = 0, was assembled. Panelists underwent five training sessions (1 × 2 h session per week, held over five consecutive weeks). During training sessions, the panel evaluated the aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of wines, according to standard DA protocol [21], and were introduced to the tasting booths in which formal evaluations would be held (i.e., under controlled ventilation, light conditions, and temperature, being 22–23 °C). The panel generated twelve aroma, seven flavor, and five taste and mouthfeel descriptors for Chardonnay wines and eleven aroma, eight flavor, and five taste and mouthfeel descriptors for Shiraz wines (Table S3); these included the same aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes used for DA of wines at t = 0, but several additional descriptors were generated for the bottle-aged wines. Reference standards were developed during early training sessions and were freshly prepared (in covered, opaque black glasses) for use at subsequent training sessions and throughout formal evaluations. Examples of taste and mouthfeel attributes (from low to high) were also provided and comprised creaminess (low-fat milk to full cream milk), acidity (base wine spiked with 0.5 to 2 g/L tartaric acid), bitterness (base wine spiked with 5 to 20 mg/L quinine sulfate), and astringency (felt material to sandpaper). The aftertaste was defined as the length of time for which fruit and/or phenolic attributes were perceived after expectoration.
During training, panelists practiced rating the intensity of each descriptor, and their performance was evaluated using SENPAQ (version 5.01, Qi Statistics, Reading, UK). Further training was provided to panelists for any attributes with the significant judge by sample interactions. Panel performance was considered to be satisfactory once interactions were minimized, after which, formal evaluations commenced. Four formal evaluation sessions were held (two each for Chardonnay and Shiraz wines), with nine wines presented per session, such that three replicates of each wine were assessed. Wines (30 mL) were assigned random three-digit codes and served in XL5 (ISO standard) 215 mL wine glasses covered with plastic lids, using a randomized presentation order, with wines presented in brackets of four or five samples. Chardonnay wines were served at 14–16 °C and Shiraz wines were served at 22–24 °C. Panelists evaluated wines and recorded the intensity of each sensory attribute using FIZZ data acquisition software (Version 2.47b, Biosystèms, Couternon, France) on 15 cm unstructured line scales with anchor points of ‘low’ and ‘high’ placed at 0% and 100% on the scale, respectively. Between samples, panelists cleansed their palate with filtered water and unsalted crackers during a one min break. Panelists were required to have five min breaks after each bracket.

2.6. Data Analysis

Sensory data were analyzed using a mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with wine sample and replicate as fixed factors and panelists as random factors, including two-way interactions (Tables S4–S7). Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) was applied as post-hoc comparison p < 0.05. Data analyses were performed with XLSTAT (version 2020.1., Addinsoft, New York, NY, USA).

2.7. Ethical Statement

DA panelists gave informed consent before participating in the study, which was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The University of Adelaide (Project No. H-174-2011).

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Influence of Flavoring and Aging on Basic Wine Composition

The pH, TA, alcohol, residual sugar, and VA of control and flavored wines were measured after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1), to investigate compositional differences amongst wines attributable to the addition of natural flavorings (Table S8). As expected, compositional differences were observed between the four wines, but no significant differences were observed between control wines and their corresponding flavored wines. Nor were there significant differences between wines after bottling and bottle aging (i.e., at t = 0 and t = 1, data not shown). Neither the addition of natural flavorings nor bottle aging significantly influenced the basic wine parameters that were measured.

3.2. Volatile Composition of Flavor Additives and Flavored Wines

The composition of flavor additives was analyzed by GC-MS in an attempt to identify the key volatile compounds responsible for their characteristic aromas and flavors. The complexity of flavorings varied considerably, with some flavor additives comprising relatively few volatile compounds, e.g., the raspberry flavor additive (Figure 1a), while others contained an array of constituents; around 20, in the case of the passion fruit flavoring (Figure 1b). The most abundant flavoring constituents were isoprenoids, furans, esters, alcohols and volatile phenols (Table 1), all of which have previously been identified as constituents of grapes and/or wine [22,23,24,25].
Control and flavored wines were also analyzed by GC-MS (at both t = 0 and t = 1), to determine compositional changes attributable to the addition of flavorings and/or bottle aging. However, flavorings were added to wines in such small quantities, i.e., as 1% solutions prepared from ≤3.0 g/L standards of flavor additives (Table S1), that many of the volatile compounds identified as constituents of flavor additives were either not detected in flavored wines or were present at similar concentrations to those of corresponding control wines (data not shown); irrespective of whether samples were analyzed using full scan mode or following the development of SIM methods to improve selectivity and sensitivity. However, there were some notable exceptions (Table 2).
Similar levels of cis-3-hexenyl butyrate were found in CH1 and CH1 + PF at t = 0, but almost 30-fold higher concentrations were observed in CH1 + PF, than in CH1, at t = 1. Comparable results were obtained following the addition of passion fruit flavoring to CH2; approximately 50-fold higher cis-3-hexenyl butyrate concentrations were found in CH2 + PF, than in CH2 at t = 1. Although similar levels of linalool were found in CH1 and CH1 + PF at t = 0, CH1+PF contained approximately double the linalool content of CH1 at t = 1. The linalool and limonene concentrations of SH1 + C and SH1 + R were similarly found to increase (relative to SH1) following bottle aging. Significant quantities of 2-ethyl hexanol were detected in all control and flavored SH1 wines; levels were higher in SH1 + C than SH1 at t = 0, but lower in SH1 + C (than SH1) at t = 1. The addition of berry flavoring to SH2 resulted in significantly higher concentrations of linalool and α- and β-ionone in SH2 + B (approximately 55%, 1900% and 360% higher levels, respectively, at t = 0). Linalool levels remained similar for control and flavored SH2 wines following bottle aging, but the α- and β-ionone content of SH2 + B increased (by an additional 50–100%) during bottle aging. Approximately two-fold higher concentrations of phenethyl acetate were found in CH2 + H than in CH2. In some instances, compositional differences between control and flavored wines were directly attributable to the addition of flavor additives, but changes observed after bottle aging likely reflect chemical transformations of wine and/or flavor constituents [10].
The detection of volatile compounds derived from flavor additives could be improved through various method development strategies, for example, through the extraction of larger volumes of flavored wine, different sampling methods, and/or the use of more specific standards (i.e., isotopically labeled internal standards). In this study, the impact of flavor addition and bottle aging was instead assessed via sensory analysis. However, it should be acknowledged that challenges associated with detecting flavor constituents in wine could have implications for policing the use of flavor additives by industry; i.e., where their use is legally prohibited, flavorings can seemingly impact wine aroma and flavor at concentrations that cannot be readily detected.

3.3. Sensory Profiles of Control and Flavored Wines

Results from descriptive analysis of control and flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz wines performed at t = 0 and t = 1 were compared to determine the impact of flavor addition and bottle aging on wine sensory profiles. As previously reported, the addition of flavorings influenced the sensory profiles of Chardonnay and Shiraz wines [7], but bottle aging also influenced wine aroma, flavor, and/or mouthfeel attributes. The addition of apricot and passion fruit flavorings to CH1 enhanced the intensity of selected fruit and/or floral characters, and diminished the perception of astringency (Table 3). However, after 12 months of bottle aging, differences between control and flavored wines were less apparent; only melon aroma and caramel-lolly flavor were found to be significantly higher in flavored CH1 wines at t = 1. The intensity of other sensory attributes increased for CH1, CH1 + A, and CH1 + PF alike. This likely reflects the development of some complexity, due to aging, i.e., increases in the intensity of vanilla, butter, mixed spice, caramel, and oak characters, as well as the occurrence of dried fruit, toast and green notes (Table 3). However, it should be acknowledged that this could also reflect differences in the composition and/or performance of the DA panels between t = 0 and t = 1. LSD values were higher at t = 1 compared to t = 0 (data not shown), despite each panel undergoing training. Increased LSD values might also reflect the DA panel’s broader use of the intensity scales at t = 1.
Less favorable results were achieved following the addition of flavor additives to Chardonnay 2 (Table 4). The honey flavoring had little impact on wine aroma or flavor; the honey aroma was not significantly enhanced, and only the perception of oak flavor increased. The addition of passion fruit flavoring surprisingly resulted in less intense fruit characters (including passion fruit aroma), while bitterness, acidity, and astringency were perceived to be more prominent. This may reflect cross-modal interactions [26], as previous studies have shown certain aromas can enhance taste perceptions without directly imparting taste properties [27,28,29]. Certainly, the enhanced acidity perceived in CH2+PF at t = 0 was not indicative of any differences in pH or TA (data not shown).
Bottle aging of CH2 gave similar outcomes to those observed for CH1; i.e., more intense vanilla, butter, orange blossom, mixed spice, caramel-lolly, and oak characters, together with dried fruit, toast and green vegetable notes (Table 4). Interestingly, the bitterness, acidity, and astringency observed in CH2 + PF at t = 0 were no longer prominent after bottle aging. At t = 1, control and flavored CH2 wines had quite similar sensory profiles, albeit CH2 + H exhibited more intense honey and oak notes, than CH2 (at t = 1). Again, the emergence of additional attributes, dried fruit and toast, in particular (Table 4), was consistent with the developed notes associated with bottle age in white wine.
The addition of flavorings to SH1 significantly increased the perception of confectionary and chocolate-vanilla characters, and diminished the earthy aroma of SH1 + R, but there were few statistically significant sensory differences between SH1 and SH1 + C (Table 5). This was surprising given the chocolate flavoring was intended to enhance chocolate notes, i.e., to mimic oak characters. In the case of SH1 + R, sensory differences were attributed to the butter and custard flavor additives present in the raspberry flavoring. This combination of flavors (i.e., butter, orange, custard, and raspberry) seemingly maintained its influence on wine aroma and flavor during bottle aging, and the intensity of confectionary and chocolate-vanilla characters were still significantly different from SH1 at t = 1. Again, additional attributes were observed at t = 1, i.e., plum, licorice and dried herb aromas, and cherry and green vegetable flavors, possibly due to aging (Table 5). The sensory profiles of SH1 and SH1 + C were still similar. These results suggest the raspberry flavoring that was added to SH1 had greater persistence than any of the flavorings added to the Chardonnay base wines.
The addition of berry flavoring, which comprised berry, custard, and butter flavor additives, to SH2 enhanced the perception of confectionary flavor and diminished the perceived intensity of oak flavor at t = 0 (Table 6). After bottle aging, the intensity of red berry and confectionary aromas and chocolate-vanilla flavor were still significantly higher than in SH2. In contrast, the raspberry flavoring did not significantly influence wine aroma or flavor, at either t = 0 or t = 1. This suggests a higher dose of flavor additives might have been needed to modify wine sensory properties.
A key aim of this study was to determine the impact of bottle aging on the sensory profiles of flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz wines, and the persistence of sensory qualities imparted by flavorings. In some, but not all cases, the addition of flavor additives did modify the perception of wine aroma and/or flavor. Where flavorings had limited impact on wine sensory profiles, the addition of more concentrated flavorings, or flavorings comprised of different combinations of flavor additives, might achieve more apparent sensory outcomes. Regardless, bottle aging seemingly influenced flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz wines differently. The differences observed between the sensory profiles of control and flavored Chardonnay wines after bottling (i.e., at t = 0) were not as apparent after 12 months bottle aging (i.e., at t = 1); which may have reflected the development of secondary vanilla, butter, spice, caramel and/or honey characters in both control and flavored wines. In contrast, sensory differences observed between some control and flavored Shiraz wines, SH1+R in particular, persisted during bottle aging, such that the sensory impact of flavor additives on wine aroma and/or flavor was still apparent. These results demonstrate that flavorings could be used to influence the sensory profiles of wine, but the optimization of the concentration and composition of flavorings would be needed to achieve lasting sensory outcomes.
Although flavor additives are routinely used in many food and beverage industries, they are not legally permitted winemaking aids [4], so their use in wine is currently prohibited. It is unlikely that flavorings would ever be used in the production of premium quality wines, for which winemakers and consumers alike value traditional approaches to winemaking. However, this study demonstrates the potential for flavorings to be used to mitigate sensory deficiencies in lower quality and/or commodity wines, if the regulations governing winemaking additives were reviewed. For example, flavorings could be used to enhance the aroma and/or flavor of wines which lack intensity (e.g., due to adverse weather conditions, such as unusually cool seasons, or prolonged drought or heat), or to mask the presence of undesirable characters (e.g., green or earthy notes) or even mild faults or taints (e.g., reductive characters or mustiness). Given the current (global) trend towards producing wines of lower alcohol content [30], there might also be opportunities for flavor additives to be used in conjunction with various alcohol adjustment strategies, which can also affect wine aroma, flavor, and body. Additionally, flavorings could be used to introduce oak characters to wine (e.g., vanilla, coconut, or spice notes), without the investment in time or capital associated with traditional barrel maturation. Findings could also be applied in the production of wine products, i.e., wine made with the addition of flavorings, legally defined in Australia as ‘food containing no less than 700 mL/L of wine which has been formulated, processed, modified or mixed with other foods’ [4].
As indicated above, differences in the composition and/or performance of the DA panel between t = 0 and t = 1 are acknowledged as an inherent limitation of the study. The differences observed in the sensory profiles of control and flavored wines between time points may, in part, have been attributable to the DA panel. Nonetheless, significant differences were still observed between the sensory properties of some control and flavored wines at each time point. The DA panels identified several new attributes in bottle-aged wines, some of which were consistent with descriptors associated with bottle aging, i.e., dried fruit and toast for white wine and licorice and plum for red wine. Most importantly, there was no evidence to suggest that any chemical transformation of natural flavorings that might have occurred resulted in the formation of off-odors during bottle aging; i.e., at t = 1, the flavor additives had not negatively impacted wine sensory profiles.

4. Conclusions

The natural flavorings used in this study were found to contain volatile compounds previously identified in grapes and/or wine, but their addition to base wines did not always significantly impact wine composition; i.e., many of the volatile compounds identified as constituents of flavor additives were not detected at appreciably higher concentrations in flavored wines, which likely reflects the extremely small quantities of flavorings added to base wines. However, the addition of flavorings significantly modified the sensory profiles of wines, with flavored wines, CH1 + A, CH1 + PF, CH2 + PF, SH1 + R and SH2 + B in particular, exhibiting enhanced fruit and/or developed aromas and flavors, as a consequence of the use of flavor additives. In the case of Chardonnay wines, the variation in sensory properties resulting from the addition of flavorings diminished with time. However, the sensory impact arising from the addition of selected flavorings to Shiraz persisted after 12 months of bottle aging. This demonstrates the potential for flavor additives to be used to enhance desirable sensory attributes and/or mitigate sensory deficiencies.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2304-8158/9/9/1208/s1, Table S1: Flavorings added to Chardonnay and Shiraz wines. Table S2: Aroma descriptors and GC-MS method characteristics (retention times and ions) of key constituents of flavor additives. Table S3: Attributes and standards used in the descriptive analysis of Chardonnay and Shiraz wines. Table S4: Analysis of variance p values for sensory attributes of Chardonnay 1 (CH1) wines after bottling (at t = 0) and after 12 months bottle aging (at t = 1). Table S5: Analysis of variance p values for sensory attributes of Chardonnay 2 (CH2) wines after bottling (at t = 0) and after 12 months bottle aging (at t = 1). Table S6: Analysis of variance p values for sensory attributes of Shiraz 1 (SH1) wines after bottling (at t = 0) and after 12 months bottle aging (at t = 1). Table S7: Analysis of variance p values for sensory attributes of Shiraz 2 (SH2) wines after bottling (at t = 0) and after 12 months bottle aging (at t = 1). Table S8: pH, titratable acidity (TA), alcohol, residual sugar, and volatile acidity (VA) of control and flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz wines after 12 months bottle aging (t = 1).

Author Contributions

All authors were involved in conceptualization; formal analysis, Y.S., J.A.C., T.E.J. and R.R.; investigation, Y.S.; resources, S.E.P.B.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.S. and K.L.W.; writing—review and editing, J.A.C., T.E.J., R.R. and S.E.P.B.; supervision, K.L.W. and S.E.P.B.; project administration, S.E.P.B.; funding acquisition, Y.S., K.L.W. and S.E.P.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Wine Australia and Y.S. held a Wine Australia PhD scholarship (GWR Ph1102).

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge: the industry partners who contributed wine; FlavorSense and The Product Makers for the provision of flavor additives; Laura Fassier for assistance with wine analysis; and the University of Adelaide staff and students involved in DA.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Fischer, U. Wine aroma. In Flavours and Fragrances; Berger, R.G., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2007; pp. 241–267. [Google Scholar]
  2. Rapp, A.; Mandery, H. Wine aroma. Experientia 1986, 42, 873–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sinki, G.S.; Gordon, R.J. Flavoring agents. In Food Additives, 2nd ed.; Branen, A.L., Davidson, P.M., Salminen, S., Thorngate, J.H., Eds.; Marcel Dekker: New York, NY, USA, 2001; pp. 349–408. [Google Scholar]
  4. Food Standards Australia New Zealand. Food Standards Codes 2.7.4 and 4.5.1. Available online: http://www.foodstandards.gov.au/ (accessed on 1 August 2020).
  5. Mueller, S.C.; Umberger, W.J. Are consumers indeed misled? Congruency in consumers’ attitudes towards wine labeling information versus revealed preferences from a choice experiment. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Agricultural and Applied Economics Association. In Proceedings of the Agricultural and Applied Economics Association, Denver, CO, USA, 25–27 July 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Saltman, Y.; Johnson, T.E.; Wilkinson, K.L.; Bastian, S.E.P. Australian wine consumers’ acceptance of and attitudes toward the use of additives in wine and food production. Int. J. Wine Res. 2015, 7, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Saltman, Y.; Johnson, T.; Wilkinson, K.L.; Ristic, R.; Norris, L.M.; Bastian, S.E.P. Natural flavor additives influence the sensoy perception and consumer liking of Australian Chardonnay and Shiraz wines. Am. J. Enol. Vitic. 2017, 68, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lattey, K.A.; Bramley, B.R.; Francis, I.L.; Herderich, M.J.; Pretorius, S. Wine quality and consumer preferences: Understanding consumer needs. Aust. N. Z. Wine Ind. J. 2007, 22, 31–39. [Google Scholar]
  9. Lattey, K.A.; Bramley, B.R.; Francis, I.L. Consumer acceptability, sensory properties and expert quality judgements of Australian Cabernet Sauvignon and Shiraz wines. Aust. J. Grape Wine Res. 2010, 16, 189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rapp, A. Volatile flavour of wine: Correlation between instrumental analysis and sensory perception. Die Nahrung 1998, 42, 351–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ramey, D.D.; Ough, C.S. Volatile ester hydrolysis or formation during storage of model solutions and wines. J. Agric. Food Chem. 1980, 28, 928–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pérez-Coello, M.; González-Viñas, M.; Garcıa-Romero, E.; Dıaz-Maroto, M.; Cabezudo, M. Influence of storage temperature on the volatile compounds of young white wines. Food Control 2003, 14, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bueno, M.; Culleré, L.; Cacho, J.; Ferreira, V. Chemical and sensory characterization of oxidative behavior in different wines. Food Res. Int. 2010, 43, 1423–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Recamales, A.F.; Sayago, A.; González-Miret, M.L.; Hernanz, D. The effect of time and storage conditions on the phenolic composition and colour of white wine. Food Res. Int. 2006, 39, 220–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hernanz, D.; Gallo, V.; Recamales, Á.F.; Meléndez-Martínez, A.J.; González-Miret, M.L.; Heredia, F.J. Effect of storage on the phenolic content, volatile composition and colour of white wines from the varieties Zalema and Colombard. Food Chem. 2009, 113, 530–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Rapp, A.; Marais, J. Shelf life of wine: Changes in aroma substances during storage and ageing of white wines. In Shelf Life Studies of Foods and Beverages; Charakanbous, G., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993; pp. 891–921. [Google Scholar]
  17. Perez-Prieto, L.; López-Roca, J.; Gómez-Plaza, E. Differences in major volatile compounds of red wines according to storage length and storage conditions. J. Food Comp. Anal. 2003, 16, 697–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Picard, M.; Tempere, S.; de Revel, G.; Marchand, S. A sensory study of the ageing bouquet of red Bordeaux wines: A three-step approach for exploring a complex olfactory concept. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 42, 110–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Monagas, M.; Bartolomé, B.; Gómez-Cordovés, C. Evolution of polyphenols in red wines from Vitis vinifera L. during aging in the bottle. Eur. Food Res. Technol. 2005, 220, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Iland, P.; Bruer, N.; Edwards, G.; Weeks, S.; Wilkes, E. Chemical Analysis of Grapes and Wine: Techniques and Concepts; Patrick Iland Wine Promotions: Campbelltown, Australia, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  21. Lawless, H.T.; Heymann, H. Descriptive analysis. In Sensory Evaluation of Food: Principles and Practices; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 1999; pp. 341–378. [Google Scholar]
  22. Wang, J.; Capone, D.L.; Wilkinson, K.L.; Jeffery, D.W. Chemical and sensory profiles of rosé wines from Australia. Food Chem. 2016, 196, 682–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Smyth, H.E. The Compositional Basis of the Aroma of Riesling and Unwooded Chardonnay Wine. Ph.D. Thesis, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  24. Francis, I.; Newton, J. Determining wine aroma from compositional data. Aust. J. Grape Wine Res. 2005, 11, 114–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Robinson, A.L.; Boss, P.K.; Solomon, P.S.; Trengove, R.D.; Heymann, H.; Ebeler, S.E. Origins of grape and wine aroma. Part 1. Chemical components and viticultural impacts. Am. J. Enol. Vitic. 2014, 65, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Auvray, M.; Spence, C. The multisensory perception of flavor. Conscious Cogn. 2008, 17, 1016–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Clark, C.C.; Lawless, H.T. Limiting response alternatives in time-intensity scaling: An examination of the halo dumping effect. Chem. Senses 1994, 19, 583–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Stevenson, R.J.; Boakes, R.A.; Prescott, J. Changes in odor sweetness resulting from implicit learning of a simultaneous odor-sweetness association: An example of learned synesthesia. Learn Motiv. 1998, 29, 113–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Niimi, J.; Liu, M.; Bastian, S.E.P. Flavour-tactile cross-modal sensory interactions: The case for astringency. Food. Qual. Pref. 2017, 62, 106–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Pham, D.-T.; Stockdale, V.J.; Wollan, D.; Jeffery, D.W.; Wilkinson, K.L. Compositional consequences of partial dealcoholization of red wine by reverse osmosis-evaporative perstraction. Molecules 2019, 24, 1404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Chromatograms of (a) raspberry and (b) passion fruit flavor additives.
Figure 1. Chromatograms of (a) raspberry and (b) passion fruit flavor additives.
Foods 09 01208 g001
Table 1. Volatiles identified as abundant constituents of natural flavor additives by GC-MS analysis.
Table 1. Volatiles identified as abundant constituents of natural flavor additives by GC-MS analysis.
Flavor AdditivesVolatile Compounds
apricotlinalool, hexyl butanoate
berrylinalool, α-terpineol, α-ionone
butterethyl butanoate
chocolate2-ethyl-1-hexanol, ethyl butanoate
cinnamoncinnamaldehyde, ethyl cinnamate, benzaldehyde
custardethyl butanoate
honey2-phenylethyl acetate, ethyl acetate
oak2-phenylethyl alcohol, furfural
orangelinalool, ethyl butanoate, limonene
passion fruit2-phenethyl isovalerate, cis-3-hexenyl butyrate
raspberryβ-ionone, linalool
vanillavanillin, 2-ethyl-1-hexanol
Table 2. Peak areas for selected volatile constituents of control and flavored Chardonnay (CH1 and CH2) and Shiraz (SH1 and SH2) wines, after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Table 2. Peak areas for selected volatile constituents of control and flavored Chardonnay (CH1 and CH2) and Shiraz (SH1 and SH2) wines, after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Flavor TargetCompoundWine Composition
berrylinaloolSH2 (t = 0): 77,936
SH2 + B (t = 0): 120,887
SH2 (t = 1): 74,909
SH2 + B (t = 1): 110,252
α-iononeSH2 (t = 0): 1797
SH2 + B (t = 0): 35,769
SH2 (t = 1): 1842
SH2 + B (t = 1): 53,500
β-iononeSH2 (t = 0): 2686
SH2 + B (t = 0): 12,429
SH2 (t = 1): 2416
SH2 + B (t = 1): 27,276
chocolate2-ethyl-1-hexanolSH1 (t = 0): 693,470
SH1 + C (t = 0): 880,936
SH1 (t = 1): 903,129
SH1 + C (t = 1): 783,384
linaloolSH1 (t = 0): 65,153
SH1 + C (t = 0): 67,702
SH1 (t = 1): 66,725
SH1 + C (t = 1): 430,975
limoneneSH1 (t = 0): 14,347
SH1 + C (t = 0): 18,966
SH1 (t = 1): 18,458
SH1 + C (t = 1): 50,955
honey2-phenylethyl acetateCH2 (t = 0): 327,217
CH2 + H (t = 0): 734,763
CH2 (t = 1): 325,031
CH2 + H (t = 1): 511,441
passion fruitcis-3-hexenyl butyrateCH1 (t = 0): 4309
CH1 + PF (t = 0): 3232
CH1 (t = 1): 3272
CH1 + PF (t = 1): 87,350
CH2 (t = 0): 3531
CH2 + PF (t = 0): 2990
CH2 (t = 1): 3400
CH2 + PF (t = 1): 161,211
linaloolCH1 (t = 0): 46,231
CH1 + PF (t = 0): 43,152
CH1 (t = 1): 45,623
CH1 + PF (t = 1): 95,449
raspberrylinaloolSH1 (t = 0): 65,153
SH1 + R (t = 0): 62,545
SH1 (t = 1): 66,725
SH1 + R (t = 1): 282,633
limoneneSH1 (t = 0): 14,347
SH1 + R (t = 0): 19,719
SH1 (t = 1): 18,458
SH1 + R (t = 1): 41,333
Peak areas were corrected against the internal standard (i.e., 2-octanol).
Table 3. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Chardonnay 1 (CH1) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Table 3. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Chardonnay 1 (CH1) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
AttributesCH1
t = 0
CH1 + A
t = 0
CH1 + PF
t = 0
pCH1
t = 1
CH1 + A
t = 1
CH1 + PF
t = 1
p
Aroma
 passion fruit7.47.48.9ns10.211.810.4ns
tropical fruit8.69.89.7ns9.59.49.0ns
stone fruit7.3 b9.1 a9.6 a0.0178.510.39.2ns
citrus3.6 b6.3 a5.8 a0.0016.76.35.9ns
green2.31.92.0ns2.91.51.0ns
honey5.15.34.1ns7.79.68.7ns
vanilla4.44.44.5ns7.510.58.1ns
butter3.8 a3.2 a2.1 b0.0028.09.38.6ns
orange blossom3.1 b5.7 a4.0 b0.0107.58.38.1ns
dried stone fruit a----8.510.610.3ns
Melon a----4.0 b5.3 ab6.5 a0.009
Toast a----6.46.95.8ns
Flavor
passion fruit9.59.29.2ns11.110.69.6ns
stone fruit9.09.59.6ns10.29.410.1ns
mixed spice4.6 b5.9 a3.6 c<0.0018.88.28.8ns
caramel-lolly4.3 b5.3 a3.7 b0.0056.8 b10.0 a9.5 a0.017
oak3.3 b4.8 a3.3 b0.0138.79.99.1ns
dried stone fruit a----10.012.511.7ns
green vegetable a----3.11.92.7ns
Taste and mouthfeel
bitterness5.55.65.2ns6.46.96.0ns
acidity8.07.98.3ns9.09.78.9ns
astringency6.1 a3.9 c4.9 b<0.00018.47.17.2ns
creaminess5.8 a3.8 b4.0 b<0.00016.3 b9.5 a7.5 ab0.044
aftertaste10.9 a6.4 b7.1 b<0.000110.010.810.1ns
Values are mean scores from 4 replicates per treatment, determined by 11 judges at t = 0 and from 3 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 1. Mean values followed by a different letter within a row (by treatment for each time point) are significantly different (p ≤ 0.05, one way ANOVA, Fisher’s LSD post hoc); ns = not significant. a Attributes associated with aged wines only.
Table 4. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Chardonnay 2 (CH2) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Table 4. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Chardonnay 2 (CH2) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
AttributesCH2
t = 0
CH2 + H
t = 0
CH2 + PF
t = 0
pCH2
t = 1
CH2 + H
t = 1
CH2 + PF
t = 1
p
Aroma
passion fruit10.0 a9.5 a7.6 b<0.00111.710.411.3ns
tropical fruit10.510.510.1ns10.28.810.8ns
stone fruit6.5 ab8.2 a6.0 bns8.0 b8.9 ab10.8 a0.016
citrus6.2 a3.3 b2.6 b<0.00017.77.18.7ns
green1.7 a1.0 b1.0 b0.0123.32.13.0ns
honey5.1 a5.2 a3.2 b0.0296.1 b9.5 a7.3 b0.010
vanilla5.54.15.3ns7.08.96.5ns
butter3.5 a2.9 a1.7 b<0.0018.18.07.6ns
orange blossom2.42.12.7ns7.38.19.3ns
dried stone fruit a----8.410.28.3ns
Melon a----5.65.66.1ns
Toast a----5.2 ab6.6 a4.5 b0.026
Flavor
passion fruit9.810.89.5ns12.0 a10.1 b12.1 a0.036
stone fruit7.78.87.1ns10.09.611.6ns
mixed spice2.5 b3.3 b2.4 bns5.76.37.0ns
caramel-lolly3.13.53.2ns6.29.07.0ns
oak2.3 b3.7 a2.9 b0.0086.6 b9.3 a6.0 b0.004
dried stone fruit a----9.611.09.9ns
green vegetable a----3.52.04.1ns
Taste and mouthfeel
bitterness3.0 c5.3 b9.7 a<0.00016.14.84.9ns
acidity7.2 b6.7 b11.2 a<0.000110.09.811.3ns
astringency5.4 b5.0 b9.3 a<0.00016.66.67.4ns
creaminess7.3 ab6.4 b8.5 a0.0067.27.86.3ns
aftertaste7.9 b9.6 ab10.7 a<0.0018.9 b11.2 a10.0 ab0.0003
Values are mean scores from 4 replicates per treatment, determined by 11 judges at t = 0 and from 3 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 1. Mean values followed by a different letter within a row (by treatment for each time point) are significantly different (p ≤ 0.05, one way ANOVA, Fisher’s LSD post hoc); ns = not significant. a Attributes associated with aged wines only.
Table 5. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Shiraz 1 (SH1) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Table 5. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Shiraz 1 (SH1) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
AttributesSH1
t = 0
SH1 + C
t = 0
SH1 + R
t = 0
pSH1
t = 1
SH1 + C
t = 1
SH1 + R
t = 1
p
Aroma
red berry5.26.46.1ns4.96.25.0ns
dark berry7.78.17.6ns6.96.47.1ns
confectionary5.4 b5.8 b8.3 a<0.0013.6 b4.6 b6.5 a<0.0001
chocolate-vanilla5.4 b5.3 b8.4 a<0.00014.7 b4.8 b7.8 a<0.0001
mixed spice6.46.15.5ns4.44.84.9ns
earthy2.2 b3.0 a1.7 b0.0034.83.54.2ns
green2.12.81.6ns4.6 a3.4 ab2.5 b0.006
black pepper4.85.24.6ns5.74.84.3ns
Plum a----5.24.84.9ns
Licorice a----4.14.04.0ns
dried herbs a----3.53.03.5ns
Flavor
red berry6.76.47.0ns6.56.36.7ns
dark berry9.28.79.3ns7.77.17.2ns
confectionary5.2 ab4.8 b6.6 a0.0434.4 b5.1 b6.5 a0.003
mixed spice6.56.06.3ns5.2 ab4.7 b6.0 a0.021
chocolate-vanilla4.2 b3.9 b6.4 a<0.00015.0 b5.2 b7.1 a<0.001
oak7.47.07.9ns5.8 b5.6 b6.8 a0.020
Cherry a----6.4 a4.8 b6.8 a0.013
green vegetable a----4.23.42.8ns
Taste and mouthfeel
bitterness6.56.15.20.0065.96.34.80.045
acidity7.47.97.5ns6.06.06.2ns
astringency7.97.88.4ns7.88.37.5ns
alcohol8.47.77.7ns7.37.46.60.040
length10.610.410.9ns7.07.17.5ns
Values are mean scores from 4 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 0 and from 3 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 1. Mean values followed by a different letter within a row (by treatment for each time point) are significantly different (p ≤ 0.05, one way ANOVA, Fisher’s LSD post hoc); ns = not significant. a Attributes associated with aged wines only.
Table 6. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Shiraz 2 (SH2) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
Table 6. Mean intensity ratings for aroma, flavor, taste, and mouthfeel attributes of control and flavored Shiraz 2 (SH2) wines after bottling (t = 0) and then after 12 months of bottle aging (t = 1).
AttributesSH2
t = 0
SH2 + B
t = 0
SH2 + R
t = 0
pSH2
t = 1
SH2 + B
t = 1
SH2 + R
t = 1
p
Aroma
red berry6.37.06.8ns5.4 b7.7 a6.2 b<0.001
dark berry7.97.78.4ns6.06.67.1ns
confectionary6.48.0 6.7ns4.1 b6.4 a5.0 b0.002
chocolate-vanilla4.65.34.9ns3.95.24.4ns
mixed spice6.05.55.6ns3.63.74.9ns
earthy2.32.72.1ns4.7 a3.0 b4.2 a0.013
green2.42.62.8ns4.54.03.6ns
black pepper5.24.74.8ns4.35.34.5ns
Plum a----5.44.86.0ns
Licorice a----4.65.04.1ns
dried herbs a----3.12.92.9ns
Flavor
red berry7.58.77.8ns7.38.27.7ns
dark berry9.29.89.7ns7.47.67.4ns
confectionary7.5 ab8.8 a6.7 b0.0356.67.17.1ns
mixed spice5.45.45.5ns4.15.25.3ns
chocolate-vanilla4.85.54.4ns4.3 b5.8 a4.9 ab0.016
oak8.2 a6.2 b7.6 a0.0024.94.94.9ns
Cherry a----6.76.94.9ns
green vegetable a----3.73.72.9ns
Taste and mouthfeel
bitterness5.34.65.0ns5.3 a4.4 b4.4 b0.050
acidity6.87.37.3ns6.25.86.4ns
astringency7.97.97.9ns7.06.06.6ns
alcohol7.1 b8.1 a7.9 a0.0406.76.46.9ns
length9.810.310.1ns7.17.57.4ns
Values are mean scores from 4 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 0 and from 3 replicates per treatment, determined by 12 judges at t = 1. Mean values followed by a different letter within a row (by treatment for each time point) are significantly different (p ≤ 0.05, one way ANOVA, Fisher’s LSD post hoc); ns = not significant. a Attributes associated with aged wines only.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Saltman, Y.; Culbert, J.A.; Johnson, T.E.; Ristic, R.; Wilkinson, K.L.; Bastian, S.E.P. Impact of Bottle Aging on the Composition and Sensory Properties of Flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz Wines. Foods 2020, 9, 1208. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9091208

AMA Style

Saltman Y, Culbert JA, Johnson TE, Ristic R, Wilkinson KL, Bastian SEP. Impact of Bottle Aging on the Composition and Sensory Properties of Flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz Wines. Foods. 2020; 9(9):1208. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9091208

Chicago/Turabian Style

Saltman, Yaelle, Julie A. Culbert, Trent E. Johnson, Renata Ristic, Kerry L. Wilkinson, and Susan E. P. Bastian. 2020. "Impact of Bottle Aging on the Composition and Sensory Properties of Flavored Chardonnay and Shiraz Wines" Foods 9, no. 9: 1208. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9091208

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop