Next Article in Journal
Sustainable Wheat Production and Food Security of Domestic Wheat in Tajikistan: Implications of Seed Health and Protein Quality
Previous Article in Journal
Prospective Study on Factors Associated with Referral of Patients with Opioid Maintenance Therapy from Specialized Addictive Disorders Centers to Primary Care
Previous Article in Special Issue
Higher Utilization of Social Services Is Associated with Higher Language Scores in Children from Deeply Impoverished Urban Families
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Differences in Neuropsychological Performance between Refugee and Non-Refugee Children in Palestine

by
Ahmed F. Fasfous
1,2,
María Nieves Pérez-Marfil
2,3,*,
Francisco Cruz-Quintana
2,3,
Miguel Pérez-García
2,3,
Hala R. Al-Yamani
4 and
Manuel Fernández-Alcántara
5
1
Social Sciences Department, Bethlehem University, Bethlehem 92248, Palestine
2
Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, 18010 Granada, Spain
3
Faculty of Psychology, University of Granada, 18010 Granada, Spain
4
Faculty of Education, Bethlehem University, Bethlehem 92248, Palestine
5
Department of Health Psychology, University of Alicante, 03690 Alicante, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(11), 5750; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115750
Submission received: 25 April 2021 / Revised: 21 May 2021 / Accepted: 24 May 2021 / Published: 27 May 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Risk and Protector Factors, Socioeconomic Status and Neurodevelopment)

Abstract

:
Neuropsychological studies on refugee children are scarce, but there are even less in the case of Palestinian children. This work aims to study the neuropsychological performance of Palestinian refugee children in Palestine compared to other Palestinian children living outside refugee camps. A comprehensive Neuropsychological battery was administrated to 584 Palestinian school children (464 refugees and 120 non-refugees) aged 6, 7, and 8 years old. Results showed that non-refugee children outperformed refugee children in sustained attention, verbal comprehension, verbal memory, and visual memory. This study is the first to have performed a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, based on a standardized and validated battery with the Palestinian refugee children. It supports professionals in their evaluation of neurodevelopment and neuropsychological alterations in refugee and non-refugee children in Palestine.

1. Introduction

According to the data on worldwide refugees included in the ‘Global Trends’ report [1], by the end of 2018, there were 70.8 million forcibly displaced persons worldwide: 41.3 million internal displacements, 25.9 million refugees, and 3.5 million asylum seekers. All were forced to flee due to persecution, conflict, violence, or human rights violations.
Palestinian refugees stand out among these populations, for the duration of the situation and their sheer number: they account for a quarter of the world’s refugees. June 2020 marked the 72nd year of the Israeli occupation of Palestine. Over 5.3 million Palestinian refugees (living in the Gaza Strip, the West Bank, Syria, Lebanon, and Jordan) undergo extreme restrictions of freedom of movement, affecting their access to school, employment, agricultural land, medical services, and family relationships [2]. The Gaza Strip has a population of more than 1.9 million people, of which 1.4 million are refugees. Some 2.4 million live on the West Bank area and a quarter of refugees live in 19 camps run by the United Nations Relief and Works Agency (UNRWA). The UNRWA currently provides basic health, education, and health infrastructure services to these refugee camps [3].
Palestinian refugee camps face several problems, notably: high population density, high unemployment, poverty, lack of space, parks and play areas, shortages of health and education services, as well as poor infrastructure. Children aged under 14 years make up nearly 40% of the Hebron and Bethlehem camps, and schools in these camps are immensely overcrowded [3].
The evidence of the psychopathological effects of war on children and adolescents is extensive. Various studies have analyzed the impact of exposure to war conflict on the mental health of children and adolescents [4,5,6,7,8]. It has been found that these children experience intense acute stress and many develop Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) [9,10]. Common symptoms and reactions after a traumatic event include sadness, anger, fears, numbness, nervousness, moodiness or irritability, changes in appetite, difficulty sleeping, nightmares, avoidance of situations reminiscent of the trauma and deteriorating concentration [11].
Research in Palestine has shown that as a result of the war, children living in the West Bank, Hebron, and Gaza areas often have mental health problems [10,12,13,14,15] especially PTSD, as well as anxiety and depression disorders [13,16]. It is estimated that approximately 41% of children living in Gaza experience post-traumatic stress and depression symptoms [10,12,15] and 77.4% of children have shown moderate or severe PTSD symptoms in the Hebron area, of which 20.5% meet the diagnostic criteria for PTSD [17]. The number of traumatic experiences related to conflict correlates positively with the prevalence of mental, behavioral and emotional problems. The main determining factors identified are the level and type of exposure, age, gender, socioeconomic adversity, social support, and religiosity [18]. In this line, the study conducted by Thabet and colleagues [16] in the Gaza Strip found high percentages of psychopathology in that population. A total of 59%presented a wide range of clinical PTSD symptoms, 21.9% of children presented anxiety, and 50.6% had depression. In addition, the total number of traumatic events was significantly related to the presence of PTSD, avoidance behaviors, physiological activation, anxiety, and depression [9]. Results showed that political violence due to war trauma was related to the development of PTSD and depression in Palestinian children in the Gaza Strip [16].
Although children and adolescents account for a big share of the refugee population, the neuropsychological problems of children and young people have not been sufficiently addressed in the scientific literature, unlike studies on psychopathology [19,20]. It has been suggested that traumatic experiences related to war conflicts not only increase the risk of mental health problems but may also have consequences on neurocognitive and socio-emotional development [7,8,21]. The findings suggest that war-related trauma has an impact on cognitive-affective processes that are essential to the healthy development of both children and adolescents [22]. In this way, toxic stress has been found to affect the development of certain brain structures. It occurs when the child is exposed to constant and uncontrollable unfavorable experiences that he is unable to adequately cope with [23] Toxic stress has been shown to cause a reduction in the size of areas such as the corpus callosum, hippocampus, amygdala and to attenuate the development of the left neocortex.
As indicated above, neuropsychological studies on children exposed to conflict are scarce, but there are even less in the case of Palestinian children. In a study by Qouta et al., [14] on Palestinian children exposed to traumatic experiences, the authors found that these children also suffered from concentration, attention, and memory problems. In a more recent study, Buckner et al., [24] found that conflict exposure could negatively influence the development of executive functions, which can in turn affect academic performance. Their analysis focused on a sample of 185 Young Palestinians, aged between 6 and 16 years. They concluded that students from schools that were the most exposed to political conflict achieved lower levels of executive functioning, notably planning. In short, the few studies that have been conducted indicate that Palestinian children present neuropsychological alterations.
Nevertheless, further studies are necessary to replicate these results. They should be conducted using extensive neuropsychological testing batteries to obtain a complete profile of all neuropsychological domains. Furthermore, these neuropsychological tests should be adapted to the Arab population, despite a shortage of such tests for this population [25]. In addition, studies should consider the degree of exposure to conflict [26], because the socio-economic situation and exposure to violence in Palestine may vary depending on whether children live inside or outside a refugee camp. Finally, the studies should verify whether the levels of exposure to conflict are even across different years of age or whether they shift depending on the children’s age.
Based on all the above, the objective of the present work was to compare the neuropsychological functioning of Palestinian children living in refugee camps (who are exposed to violence the most) compared to that of Palestinian children living outside the camps (who are less exposed to violence), using a standardized and validated battery on the Arab population (Child Computerized Neuropsychological Assessment Battery, BENCI) [27] which evaluates the major neuropsychological domains. In addition, to check whether exposure to violence has equal effects according to age and sex, the study was conducted on children aged 6, 7 and 8 years. These age groups were selected because they correspond to the first years of schooling in the Palestinian education system and it is at these ages that the neuropsychological problems relating to school performance can appear. In addition, several studies have stressed the importance of conducting neuropsychological studies with schoolchildren and the need to create normative data on school children. We hypothesized that Palestinian children exposed to violence (i.e., living in refugee camps) would present more extensive alterations regarding memory, attention, and executive function [14,24]. No hypotheses were advanced in any of the other domains because of the absence of any previous studies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 584 Palestinian schoolchildren (295 boys, 50.51%; 289 girls, 49.49%) aged 6, 7, and 8 voluntarily participated in the study (mean (M) = 6.99, standard deviation (SD) = 0.817). As noted above, these ages were selected because they correspond to the first years of schooling in the Palestinian education system and due to the scarcity of standard scale tests for these ages. Participants were selected from two Palestinian provinces (Hebron and Bethlehem). These two geographical areas include five refugee camps, in Bethlehem: the Dheisheh Camp (established in 1949), the Beit Jibrin Camp (established in 1950), the Aida Camp (established in 1950), and in Hebron: the Arroub Camp (established in 1949), and the Fawwar Camp (established in 1949). A total of 464 (79.45%) children were selected from schools located within five refugee camps. In addition, 120 (20.55%) children living outside refugee camps participated in the study. Table 1 presents the geographical distribution of the study sample.
Table 2 includes general information on the sample, as well as sociodemographic data on the parents.

2.2. Instruments

The computerized Battery for the Neuropsychological Evaluation of Children (BENCI) consists of 14 neuropsychological tests that measure different neuropsychological functions. They allow a comprehensive evaluation of children’s neurodevelopment. The main functions are: language (Verbal Comprehension and Phonemic Fluency), Sustained Attention (Continuous Performance Test), Visuomotor Coordination (Visuomotor test), Verbal Memory (Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall, and Recognition), Visual Memory (Immediate Recall, Delayed Recall, and Recognition), and Executive Function (Working Memory, Planning, Abstract Reasoning, Semantic Fluency, and Alternate Visuomotor Test). Further details on the tests used to measure these functions can be found in previous studies [27,28,29].
BENCI has been used in different contexts to evaluate neurodevelopment and has shown good psychometric properties [30]. In addition, this battery has been validated and adapted to Arab culture and its good reliability and validity on Arab children has been demonstrated [27].

2.3. Procedure

First, the study was approved by the ethical commissions of the University of Granada and Bethlehem University as a cooperative project between the two universities on 6 October 2014 (Reference No. 201402400001070). Once the authorization was obtained from the ethics committees, an authorization from the “Directorate of Education, Bethlehem” was obtained to conduct the study at public schools outside refugee camps. Another authorization was obtained from the office of The United Nations Relief and Works Agency for Palestine Refugees (UNURWA) in Palestine to conduct the study at schools located in Hebron and Bethlehem refugee camps. The schools were randomly selected and, applying tiered sampling to the class lists, the sample was subdivided into three levels (1st, 2nd and 3rd year) based on gender and academic level. The children were then randomly extracted from each strata. The 1st year corresponds to children aged 6 years, the 2nd year to children aged 7 years, and the 3rd grade to children aged 8 years.
All assessments were conducted in the morning, during school hours, in a room located within the school premises. In this study, the order of administration was the same for all participants, with amid-session 10-min break. Administering the battery lasted on average 75 min and the order of administration was determined following the recommendations of Lezak et al. [31].

2.4. Statistical Analysis

To start with, descriptive analyses were conducted using the main socio-demographic variables. ANOVAs were then conducted with a 2 × 2 × 3 inter subject factorial design, considering the group (living inside the refugee camp vs. outside the refugee camp), sex (male vs. female) and age (six vs. seven vs. eight years of age) as independent variables, and BENCI subtest results as dependent variables. In the case of the verbal memory subtest, an extra within factor was added to the design (immediate recall trials 1, 2, 3, delayed trials). In a similar way, an extra within factor was added for visual memory (immediate and delayed trials) as well as a go/no-go task (trial blocks 1–4). Some post hoc Bonferroni analyses were conducted when significant statistical interactions were found. Furthermore, partial eta squared was calculated to measure the effect size. The significant levels were adjusted to 0.05 and all analyses were conducted using SPSS 23.

3. Results

3.1. Language

Main effect of age was found to have an impact across all the tests used to evaluate language. Post hoc comparisons indicated that children aged 8 years performed better than children aged 7 years, and children aged 7 years performed better than children aged 6 years in the phonetic fluency test. However, no statistically significant differences were found in the verbal comprehension tests (images and figures) between children aged 7 and 8 years.
In the image comprehension test, we found main effect of area and the scores of the refugee children were lower than that of children outside the camps. In addition, another main effect was found for sex with girls performed generally better than boys in the phonetic fluency test. No interactions were found for any language subtest (see Table 3).

3.2. Sustained Attention

The Continuous Performance Tasks (CPT) results main effect for correct answer and omissions, showing that the group of refugee children was more successful than children outside the camp. However, non-refugee children made fewer mistakes than refugee children. Considering the time variable, the results showed that refugee children were slower at completing the test. Regarding main effect of sex, boys were faster at responding to stimuli. The results demonstrated a Sex × Area interaction, refugee boys and girls showing similar values, although in the case of non-refugee children, girls performed significantly better than boys in this test (see Table 3).

3.3. Visuomotor

In the visuomotor coordination test, we found main effect of age. Younger children took longer than older children to perform the task. The group of children aged 6 years scored lower than the two other groups. No statistically significant differences were found between the 7 and 8 year age groups. No interactions were found for visuomotor coordination (see Table 3).

3.4. Memory

For the recognition trials, the main effects of age were found for verbal memory and visual memory with younger children scoring lower than older ones (Table 3).
For the free recall trails and learning curve, we found the main effects of area and age in verbal and visual memory. Palestinian children living outside refugee camps performed better than refugee children in verbal memory tasks. In addition, the results presented notable differences between children based on age. Post hoc tests showed significant differences between the three age groups.
As for the verbal memory results, refugee children scored lower on visual memory tasks. Significant differences were found; however, between the children aged 6 and 7 years, the older group scored higher. They were also found between children aged 6 years and the group of children aged 8 years, with the older group scoring higher. These differences were significant between the group of children aged 7 and the group of children aged 8. No interactions were found for memory (see Table 4).

3.5. Executive Functions

The results revealed significant differences between scores in the case of 4 out of 5 executive function tests based on age (main effect). Specifically, the group of children aged 8 obtained higher scores than the group of children aged 6 and 7 year. The group of children aged 7 years scored higher than the group of children aged 6 years on working memory, abstract reasoning, and semantic fluency tasks (Table 3).
In relation to the Alternate Visuomotor task, the results showed significant differences between the three age groups and sex (main effect of age and sex). Post hoc comparisons indicated that the older groups achieved higher scores. Moreover, girls outperformed boys in this test. No interactions were found for executive functions (see Table 4).
In addition, our results showed that effect size was large in sustained attention (Area), while medium effect sizes were reported in Phonemic fluency (sex), visuomotor coordination (sex), Alternate Visuomotor (sex), Working Memory (sex), abstract reasoning (sex), and verbal memory recognition (age). Furthermore, results revealed a small effect size in the rest of the neuropsychological tests (see Table 5).

4. Discussion

The main objective of the present study was to assess the neuropsychological performance of refugee children in Palestine compared to other Palestinian children living outside refugee camps. Though several papers have examined the mental health of Palestinian children, few studies have studied neuropsychological performance [14]. This study is the first to have used a complete neuropsychological battery, including major neuropsychological domains, to assess the neuropsychological performance of Palestinian refugee and non-refugee children. Moreover, it is also the first study to have examined the impact of living in a refugee camp on neuropsychological performance during early school years.
Overall, the results showed that refugee children perform worse than non-refugee children in sustained attention, verbal comprehension, verbal memory, and visual memory. This finding is consistent with that of other prior studies that concluded that refugees tend to perform worse neuropsychologically. A recent study showed the deterioration of sustained attention during an inhibitory control task (Stroop’s task) in young people who had experienced the loss of their parents and who lived in a conflict zone [32].
These results can be explained by the socio-economic difficulties of refugee children. In addition to the marginalization and difficult living conditions within refugee camps [3], Palestinian camps are the targets of on-going incursions by Israeli occupying forces. Therefore, the probability of children experiencing traumatic events is higher, and this could explain the poor performance of children who live in refugee camps. Neuropsychological functions such as attention and memory are among the functions that are the most affected by exposure to psychological trauma [22,33,34]. In the same line, young Afghan refugees have been found to present poorer working memory capacity than a control group [6].
The absence of differences in other neuropsychological functions could be related to the similar context of Palestinian children living outside and inside refugee camps. Both Palestinian children living inside and outside refugee camps are exposed to various forms of political, economic, and social violence, as they all live under Israeli occupation control. However, children living within refugee camps are subject to worse life conditions and are also exposed to more extensive violence. These similarities and differences in the contexts in which children grow could explain the child performance similarities and differences in neuropsychological tests.
With respect to gender differences in neuropsychological performance among Palestinian children, three main functions were identified: phonetic fluency, sustained attention, visuomotor coordination. These findings coincide with that of other studies in which girls presented a better performance in language tests [35,36], visuomotor coordination [37,38] and sustained attention [39,40]. However, there is no scientific consensus regarding these differences. In our study, these differences were encountered in the case of some functions, but we must remember that both boys and girls were exposed to a general context of violence. Added to the above differences, non-refugee Palestinian girls performed better than the group of non-refugee boys in the visual memory test. However, these differences were not detected between the boys and girls in the refugee camps. The results can be explained by the higher level of stress children are subjected to when living in refugee camps. As indicated, high levels of exposure to violence experienced in refugee camps may have reduced gender differences.
The age variable had a clear impact on children’s neuropsychological performance in all BENCI tests except the sustained attention test. These results are similar to that of prior studies that used the BENCI battery to measure neurodevelopment in children in Morocco [17] and Ecuador [28]. Improvements in the performance of neuropsychological tests reflect the normal development of neuropsychological functions during childhood [41]. None of the analyses, however, showed an age interaction per group, indicating that children living inside and outside refugee camps had improved their neuropsychological performance at different ages in a similar way. Since it is not possible to study a group of Palestinian children that is “not exposed” to violence, we are unable to verify whether such patterns of performance increase with age follow an adequate or altered pattern.
The main risk factors for normal neurocognitive development are related to conditions of the prenatal stage, perinatal circumstances, nutritional, infectious and toxic factors, and child-rearing practices [42], as well as socioeconomic conditions, in especially those related to poverty, in general, and extreme poverty in particular [43] and with exposure to violence [23]. Many of these circumstances are present in the sample of this study. This work constitutes the first neuropsychological study carried out with children living in Palestinian refugee camps. It has made it possible to obtain a baseline that can be used for future longitudinal studies. The results confirm the need to generate programs to intervene in the detected deficits and implement other preventive programs from an earlier age. These results have important health implications, indicating the vulnerability and the negative effects in the neurodevelopment in these children. Given the large number of variables and factors that interact (poverty, lack of space, poor infrastructure, and education services) it is necessary to develop interventions for health promotion that can address both internal and external risks factors.
The main limitations of the study include, first of all, the fact of having included only three age groups from two provinces of Palestine. Future studies should be performed, using samples that span a broader age range to thus obtain information on neuropsychological performance in adolescent populations in these contexts. Second, given Palestine’s socio-political situation of conflict, it was not possible to include a control group of Palestinian children that was “not exposed” to violence. Future studies might consider including a control group of Palestinian children living outside Palestine, such as Jordan, who are not exposed to conflict violence. Third, we could not assess the intellectual coefficient of the children that participated in the research. Finally, the cross-sectional research design does not allow us to study the direct influence of age in the neuropsychological development of these populations. Studies using a longitudinal design are needed to test the effects of trauma in the cognitive development of refugees and non-refugees children in Palestine
Despite this, the present work is the first to have performed a comprehensive evaluation of the neuropsychological functioning, based on a standardized and validated battery with the Arab population and during the first years of schooling. This battery can be used in future research to examine further variables and to create normalized data on Palestinian children. It also supports professionals in their evaluation of neurodevelopment and neuropsychological alterations in refugee and non-refugee Palestinian children in Palestine. Future studies should also consider comparing these results with that of Palestinian children living outside Palestine to better understand the role of continuous exposure to violence.

5. Conclusions

To conclude, data from our study indicate that Palestinian children living in refugee camps present lower neuropsychological test performance than other children in sustained attention, verbal comprehension, verbal memory, and visual memory.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.F.F., M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G. and M.F.-A.; methodology, A.F.F., M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G. and M.F.-A.; formal analysis, M.P.-G. and A.F.F.; investigation, A.F.F.; resources, H.R.A.-Y.; data curation, A.F.F., M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G. and M.F.-A.; writing—original draft preparation, A.F.F., M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G. and M.F.-A.; writing—review and editing, A.F.F., M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G., H.R.A.-Y. and M.F.-A.; project administration, M.P.-G.; funding acquisition, A.F.F.; M.N.P.-M., F.C.-Q., M.P.-G. and M.F.-A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Center for Development Cooperation Initiatives (Centro de Iniciativas de Cooperación al Desarrollo—CICODE), Granada University, Spain (Reference No. C14P11_9359).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Bethlehem University and the University of Granada on 6 October 2014 (Reference No. 201402400001070).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. Global Trends: Forced Displacement in 2018; UNHCR: Geneva, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  2. Agencia de la ONU para los Refugiados (ACNUR). Informe ‘Tendencias Globales’. 2019. Available online: https://www.acnur.org/es-es/el-acnur.html (accessed on 12 March 2020).
  3. The United Nations Relief and Works Agency for Palestine Refugees in the Near East (UNRWA). Available online: http://www.unrwa.org (accessed on 10 February 2021).
  4. Alpak, G.; Unal, A.; Bulbul, F.; Sagaltici, E.; Bez, Y.; Altindag, A.; Dalkilic, A.; Savas, H.A. Post-traumatic stress disorder among Syrian refugees in Turkey: A cross-sectional study. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2014, 19, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Foster, R.P. When immigration is trauma: Guidelines for the individual and family clinician. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2001, 71, 153–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Mirabolfathi, V.; Schweizer, S.; Moradi, A.; Jobson, L. Affective working memory capacity in refugee adolescents. Psychol. Trauma 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Silveira, K.; Garcia-Barrera, M.A.; Smart, C.M. Neuropsychological Impact of Trauma-Related Mental Illnesses: A Systematic Review of Clinically Meaningful Results. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020, 30, 310–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Weinstein, C.S.; Fucetola, R.; Mollica, R. Neuropsychological issues in the assessment of refugees and victims of mass violence. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2001, 11, 131–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. El-Khodary, B.; Samara, M. The relationship between multiple exposures to violence and war trauma, and mental health and behavioural problems among Palestinian children and adolescents. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 29, 719–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Thabet, A.; Vostanis, P. Post traumatic stress disorder reactions in children of war: A longitudinal study. Child Abus. Negl. 2000, 24, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. De Jong, J.T.; Komproe, I.H.; Van Ommeren, M.; El Masri, M.; Araya, M.; Khaled, N.; van de Put, W.; Somasundaram, D. Life time events and posttraumatic stress disorder in 4 post conflict settings. JAMA 2001, 286, 555–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Khamis, V. Post-traumatic stress disorder among school age Palestinian children. Child Abus. Negl. 2005, 29, 81–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Khamis, V. Post-traumatic stress and psychiatric disorders in Palestinian adolescents following intifada-related injuries. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 67, 1199–1207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Qouta, S.; Punamäki, R.-L.; El Sarraj, E. The relations between traumatic experiences, activity, and cognitive and emotional responses among palestinian children. Int. J. Psychol. 1995, 30, 289–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Thabet, A.M.; Abed, Y.; Vostanis, P. Comorbidity of PTSD and depression among refugee children during war conflict. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 533–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Thabet, A.M.; Thabet, S.; Vostanis, P. The Relationships between Mental Health Problems and Family Coping Strategies among Palestinian in the Gaza Strip. J. Adv. Med. Med. Res. 2016, 17, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fasfous, A.F.; Peralta-Ramírez, I.; Pérez-García, M. Symptoms of PTSD among Children Living in War Zones in Same Cultural Context and Different Situations. J. Muslim Ment. Health 2013, 7, 47–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Dimitry, L. A systematic review on the mental health of children and adolescents in areas of armed conflict in the Middle East. Child Care Health Dev. 2012, 38, 153–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Llorente, A.M.; Soong, C. Psychological issues affecting unaccompanied immigrant children. In Proceedings of the Eleventh ABA/APA-Sponsored National Conference on Children and the Law, Washington, DC, USA, 21–22 October 2004. [Google Scholar]
  20. Llorente, A.M. Principles of Neuropsychological Assessment with Hispanics: Theoretical Foundations and Clinical Practice; Springer Science & Business Media: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  21. Llorente, A.; Soong, C.; Friedrich, E.; Shields, B.; Cohen, P.; Dias, E.; Lawless, S.; Steigmeyer, H. Neuropsychological and Legal Factors Affecting Unaccompanied Immigrant Children: A Review of the Literature and Case Study. J. Pediatr. Neuropsychol. 2017, 3, 170–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Mueller, S.C.; Unal, C.; Saretta, M.; Al Mughairbi, F.; Gómez-Odriozola, J.; Calvete, E.; Metin, B. Working memory and emotional interpretation bias in a sample of Syrian refugee adolescents. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  23. Shonkoff, J.P.; Garner, A.S.; Siegel, B.S.; Dobbins, M.I.; Earls, M.F.; McGuinn, L.; Pascoe, J.; Wood, D.L. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2011, 129, e232–e246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Buckner, E.; Kim, P. Mobile innovations, executive functions, and educational developments in conflict zones: A case study from Palestine. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 2012, 60, 175–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Fasfous, A.F.; Al-Joudi, H.F.; Puente, A.E.; Pérez-García, M. Neuropsychological Measures in the Arab World: A Systematic Review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2017, 27, 158–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Veliu, B.; Leathem, J. Neuropsychological assessment of refugees: Methodological and cross-cultural barriers. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 2017, 24, 481–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Fasfous, A.F.; Peralta-Ramírez, M.I.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Catena-Martínez, A.; Pérez-García, M. Reliability and validity of the Arabic version of the computerized Battery for Neuropsychological Evaluation of Children (BENCI). Child Neuropsychol. 2014, 21, 210–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Burneo-Garcés, C.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Fernández-Alcántara, M.; Fasfous, A.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N. Interaction between Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Development in Children Aged 7, 9, and 11 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2019, 44, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Roldan-Vílchez, L.M.; FernándezLópez, A.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N. Manual de la Batería de Evaluación Neuropsicológica Infantil (BENCI); Siger: Granada, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  30. García-Bermúdez, O.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Hidalgo-Ruzzante, N.; Fernández-Alcántara, M.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N. Improvement of executive functions after the application of a neuropsychological intervention program (PEFEN) in pre-term children. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2019, 98, 328–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Lezak, M.D.; Howieson, D.B.; Loring, D.W.; Fischer, J.S. Neuropsychological Assessment; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  32. Mueller, S.; Baudoncq, R.; De Schryver, M. The effect of parental loss on cognitive and affective interference in adolescent boys from a post-conflict region. J. Adolesc. 2015, 42, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Barrera-Valencia, M.; Calderón-Delgado, L.; Trejos-Castillo, E.; O’Boyle, M. Cognitive profiles of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder and depression in children and adolescents. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2017, 17, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Moradi, A.R.; Doost, H.T.; Taghavi, M.R.; Yule, W.; Dalgleish, T. Everyday memory deficits in children and adolescents with PTSD: Performance on the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 1999, 40, 357–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Ardila, A.; Rosselli, M.; Matute, E.; Inozemtseva, O. Gender differences in cognitive development. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 984–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Weiss, E.M.; Ragland, J.D.; Brensinger, C.M.; Bilker, W.B.; Deisenhammer, E.A.; Delazer, M. Sex differences in clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2006, 12, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Llorente, A.M. Children’s Color Trails Test: Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  38. Valtr, L.; Psotta, R.; Abdollahipour, R. Gender differences in performance of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children—2nd edition test in adolescents. Acta Gymnica 2016, 46, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Balart, P.; Oosterveen, M. Females show more sustained performance during test-taking than males. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 3798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Sobeh, J.; Spijkers, W. Development of neuropsychological functions of attention in two cultures: A cross-cultural study of attentional performances of Syrian and German children of pre-school and school age. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 10, 318–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Waber, D.P.; De Moor, C.; Forbes, P.W.; Almli, C.R.; Botteron, K.N.; Leonard, G.; Milovan, D.; Paus, T.; Rumsey, J.; the Brain Development Cooperative Group. The NIH MRI study of normal brain development: Performance of a population based sample of healthy children aged 6 to 18 years on a neuropsychological battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2007, 13, 729–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Garcia, O.F.; Serra, E.; Zacares, J.J.; Calafat, A.; Garcia, F. Alcohol use and abuse and motivations for drinking and non-drinking among Spanish adolescents: Do we know enough when we know parenting style? Psychol. Health 2020, 35, 645–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Pérez-Marfil, M.N.; Fernández-Alcántara, M.; Fasfous, A.F.; Burneo-Garcés, C.; Pérez-García, M.; Cruz-Quintana, F. Influence of Socio-Economic Status on Psychopathology in Ecuadorian Children. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Geographical distribution of the study sample.
Table 1. Geographical distribution of the study sample.
ProvinceBoys/GirlsRefugeesNon-Refugees6 Years7 Years8 Years
Hebron114/1172310737880
Bethlehem181/172233120123117113
Total295/289464120196195194
Table 2. General information on the sample and sociodemographic data of the parents.
Table 2. General information on the sample and sociodemographic data of the parents.
Refugee
M (SD) or n (%)
Non-Refugee
M (SD) or n (%)
p
Number of people they live with5.77 (1.91)5.57 (1.91)0.295
Number sisters and brothers3.9 (2.02)4 (2.2)0.297
Hours of sleep9.49 (1.27)10.48 (1.94)0.000
Chronic health problem
 Yes
 No

137 (29.6%)
326 (70.4%)

18 (15%)
102 (85%)
0.001
Father’s educational level
 No studies
 Primary
 High school
 Graduate University
 Master/Doctorate
 Do not know

30 (6.5%)
114 (24.6%)
197 (42.6%)
33 (7.1%)
70 (15.1%)
20 (4.1%)

5 (4.2%)
59 (49.2%)
43 (35.8%)
4 (3.3%)
8 (6.7%)
1 (0.8%)
0.000
Mother’s educational level
 No studies
 Primary
 High school
 Graduate University
 Master/Doctorate
 Do not know

12 (2.6%)
86 (18.5%)
205 (44.2%)
25 (5.4%)
124 (26.7%)
12 (2.6%)

4 (3.3%)
20 (16.7%)
59 (49.2%)
7 (5.8%)
29 (24.2%)
1 (0.8%)
0.778
Employeed father
 Yes
 No

402 (86.7%)
62 (13.3%)

114 (95%)
6 (5%)
0.043
Father’s job
 Autonomous
 Family business
 Unskilled manual labor
 Skilled manual labor
 Domestic/caregiver
 Professional
 Student

35 (8.7%)
2 (0.5%)
183 (45.5%)
37 (9.2%)
0
100 (24.9%)
45 (11.2%)

14 (12.3)
2 (1.7%)
70 (61.7%)
13 (11.4%)
0
10 (8.7%)
5 (4.2%)
0.000
Employeed motther
 Yes
 No

78 (16.8%)
386 (83.2%)

10 (8.3%)
110 (91.7%)
0.021
Mother’s job
 Autonomous
 Family business
 Unskilled manual labor
 Skilled manual labor
 Domestic/caregiver
 Professional
 Student

7 (9%)
0
5 (6.4%)
3 (3.8%)
3 (3.8%)
27 (34.6%)
33 (42.3%)

0
0
0
0
0
4 (40%)
6 (60%)
0.808
M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
Table 3. Neuropsychological performance of Palestinian children.
Table 3. Neuropsychological performance of Palestinian children.
DomainTestRefugee
n = 464
M (SD)
Non-Refugee
n = 120
M (SD)
Male
n = 295
M (SD)
Female
n = 289
M (SD)
6
n = 196
M (SD)
7
n = 195
M (SD)
8
n = 193
M (SD)
Main Effects
and
Interactions
p
LanguagePhonemic Fluency2.76 (2.16)2.95 (2.13)2.37 (1.94)3.23 (2.27)2.02 (1.66)2.68 (1.96)3.70 (2.42)Sex (Female > male)
Age (1 < 2 < 3)
0.000
0.000
ComFig8.52 (1.29)8.33 (1.39)8.39 (1.38)8.57 (1.24)8.03 (1.39)8.58 (1.33)8.84 (1.07)Age (1 < 2; 1 < 3)0
ComIma8.67 (1.25)9.01 (1.07)8.65 (1.26)8.83 (1.17)8.36 (1.34)8.84 (1.69)9.02 (1.05)Ref < noR
Age (1 < 2; 1 < 3)
0.003
0.000
Sustained
Attention
CPT/CA278.97 (25.60)228.16 (101.53)269.70 (54.29)267.34 (56.18)265.47 (55.12)271.88 (51.66)268.25 (58.71)noR < Ref0
CPT/OM10.58 (9.12)5.84 (5.66)9.26 (8.38)9.96 (9.09)11.24 (9.51)8.94 (8.43)8.61 (7.99)noR < Ref
Age (3 < 1)
0.000
0.030
CPT/RT0.63 (0.14)0.35 (0.43)0.59 (0.24)0.56 (0.28)0.57 (0.87)0.57 (0.26)0.58 (0.24)Area (Ref > noR)
Sex (Male > Female)
Area × Sex
0.000
0.006
0.013
VisuomotorVis-motor 1109.74 (55.02)105.63 (51.20)112.87 (60.14)104.84 (47.23)134.61 (66.01)102.77 (45.88)88.98 (35.63)Age (1 > 2; 1 > 3)0
Memory—RecognitionVerbal Memory15.83 (2.45)16.18 (2.11)15.61 (2.63)16.20 (2.07)14.97 (2.68)16.13 (2.28)16.61 (1.81)Age (1 < 2; 1 < 3)0
Visual Memory41.73 (6.39)42.70 (4.89)41.05 (6.61)42.83 (5.43)40.17 (6.64)42.17 (6.22)43.47 (5.14)Age (1 < 3)
Area × Sex
0.001
0.042
Executive
Functions
Working Memory4.04 (1.93)3.75 (1.90)3.90 (1.94)4.07 (1.91)3.23 (1.48)3.90 (1.74)4.82 (2.16)Age (1 < 2 < 3)0
Planning11.09 (1.57)11.45 (2.93)11.01(1.85)11.32 (2.00)11.23 (2.14)10.97 (1.60)11.28 (2.01)NSNS
Abstract Reasoning12.12 (5.33)11.70 (5.03)11.72 (5.26)12.35 (5.27)9.92 (4.33)11.96 (5.45)14.25 (5.07)Age (1 < 2 < 3)0
Semantic Fluency8.42 (3.43)8.42 (3.14)8.35 (3.43)8.49 (3.31)7.13 (3.02)8.35 (3.15)9.79 (3.40)Age (1 < 2 < 3)0
Vis-motor 2123.61 (55.22)117.94 (50.39)127.66 (59.73)117.12 (47.57)145.05 (54.23)122.21 (57.39)99.72 (39.83)Sex (Male > Female)
Age (1 > 2 > 3)
0.016
0.000
ComFig = Verbal Comprehension—Figures; ComIma = Verbal comprehension images; CPT = Continuous performance Test; CA = Correct Answers; OM = omission; RT = Reaction time; Vis-motor 1 = visuomotor coordination; Vis-motor 2 = Alternate Visuomotor; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Sex = main effect of sex variable; Age = main effect of age variable; Area = main effect of area variable; Area × Sex: interaction of area and sex variables; Ref = refugee group; noR = non-refugee group; NS = non-significant.
Table 4. Verbal and visual memory tests performance.
Table 4. Verbal and visual memory tests performance.
DomainRefugee
n = 463
M (S. D)
Non-Refugee
n = 119
M (SD)
Male
n = 295
M (SD)
Female
n = 287
M (SD)
6
n = 195
M (SD)
7
n = 194
M (SD)
8
n = 193
M (SD)
Main Effects
and
Interactions
p
Verbal Memory 14.15 (1.68)4.57 (1.75)4.14 (1.73)4.33 (1.66)3.74 (1.62)4.28 (1.76)4.68 (1.59)Area (noR > Ref)
Age (1 < 2 < 3)
0.041
0.000
Verbal Memory 25.68 (1.69)5.85 (1.76)5.74 (1.64)5.68 (1.80)5.21 (1.77) 5.68 (1.70)6.25 (1.49)
Verbal Memory 36.48 (1.60)6.39 (1.69)6.47 (1.60)6.45 (1.64)5.82 (1.67)6.59 (1.56)6.99 (1.40)
Verbal Memory 45.49 (1.74)5.81 (1.84)5.60 (1.81)5.51 (1.71)4.93 (1.62)5.62 (1.69)6.11 (1.77)
Visual Memory A6.48 (2.14)6.95 (2.12)6.50 (2.14)6.66 (2.14)5.80 (2.08)6.76 (2.03)7.18 (2.08)Area (noR > Ref)
Age (1 < 2, 1 < 3)
0.000
0.000
Visual Memory B4.65 (1.92)5.35 (2.22)4.45 (2.03)4.79 (2.00)3.91 (1.99)4.94 (1.99)5.01 (1.89)
1 = Immediate Recall first attempt; 2 = Immediate Recall second attempt; 3 = Immediate Recall third attempt; 4 = Delayed Recall; A = Immediate Recall; B = Delayed Recall; Ref = refugees; noR = non-refugees; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
Table 5. Effect size and interpretation.
Table 5. Effect size and interpretation.
DomainTestVariableEta Squared
( η   p 2 )
Interpretation
LanguagePhonemic FluencyArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.004
0.044
0.069
0.001
Small
Small
Medium
Small
Verbal Comprehension—FiguresArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.002
0.001
0.051
0.005
Small
Small
Small
Small
Verbal Comprehension ImagesArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.016
0.001
0.032
0.000
Small
Small
Small
Small
Sustained AttentionContinuous performance Test (Correct answers)Area
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.144
0.003
0.002
0.003
Large
Small
Small
Small
Continuous performance Test (Omission)Area
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.052
0.000
0.012
0.002
Small
Small
Small
Small
Continuous performance Test (Reaction Time)Area
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.195
0.013
0.000
0.001
Large
Small
Small
Small
VisuomotorVisuomotor CoordinationArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.003
0.004
0.088
0.000
Small
Small
Medium
Small
Executive FunctionsWorking MemoryArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.002
0.003
0.087
0.001
Small
Small
Medium
Small
PlanningArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.006
0.003
0.010
0.001
Small
Small
Small
Small
Abstract ReasoningArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.000
0.001
0.076
0.001
Small
Small
Medium
Small
Semantic FluencyArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.000
0.000
0.049
0.001
Small
Small
Small
Small
Verbal memory Immediate Recall Attempts and Delayed RecallArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.005
0.001
0.002
0.001
Small
Small
Small
Small
RecognitionArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.002
0.073
0.000
0.003
Small
Medium
Small
Small
Visual Memory Immediate Recall and Delayed RecallArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.010
0.001
0.003
0.000
Small
Small
Small
Small
RecognitionArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.005
0.005
0.026
0.004
Small
Small
Small
Small
Alternate VisuomotorArea
Age
Sex
Interaction
0.004
0.010
0.084
0.002
Small
Small
Medium
Small
Area = refugees vs. non-refugees.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fasfous, A.F.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Al-Yamani, H.R.; Fernández-Alcántara, M. Differences in Neuropsychological Performance between Refugee and Non-Refugee Children in Palestine. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5750. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115750

AMA Style

Fasfous AF, Pérez-Marfil MN, Cruz-Quintana F, Pérez-García M, Al-Yamani HR, Fernández-Alcántara M. Differences in Neuropsychological Performance between Refugee and Non-Refugee Children in Palestine. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(11):5750. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115750

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fasfous, Ahmed F., María Nieves Pérez-Marfil, Francisco Cruz-Quintana, Miguel Pérez-García, Hala R. Al-Yamani, and Manuel Fernández-Alcántara. 2021. "Differences in Neuropsychological Performance between Refugee and Non-Refugee Children in Palestine" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 11: 5750. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115750

APA Style

Fasfous, A. F., Pérez-Marfil, M. N., Cruz-Quintana, F., Pérez-García, M., Al-Yamani, H. R., & Fernández-Alcántara, M. (2021). Differences in Neuropsychological Performance between Refugee and Non-Refugee Children in Palestine. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(11), 5750. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115750

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop