From a psychological perspective, organizational life can be understood in terms of a chain of events [
1]. Despite calls to take issues of time more seriously [
2,
3,
4,
5], researchers in the field of occupational health psychology have only recently begun to consider the dynamics of relevant phenomena, such as employee strain and engagement [
6], through the lens of work events [
7]. Work engagement has been described as “a positive, fulfilling, work-related state of mind that is characterized by vigor, dedication, and absorption” [
8]. Furthermore, work engagement has attracted considerable research interest within the last 15 years [
9,
10,
11]. In particular, job characteristics have been identified as major drivers of work engagement [
6,
10,
11], and empirical evidence has consistently shown that work engagement is determined by the interplay of different types of job characteristics (e.g., resources such as autonomy and demands such as workload) [
12]. However, to fully understand the experience of work and how it relates to employee outcomes (e.g., engagement), it is advisable to go beyond generalized perceptions of how a job usually is (i.e., job characteristics measured by surveys in terms of job demands and resources). More specifically, there is a need to consider dynamic aspects (i.e., day-to-day fluctuations in job characteristics) [
12,
13] as well as factors that are more proximal to employee experiences over time [
14]. Therefore, a focus on enacted job characteristics—that is, events and activities in the job as they happen [
14]—is warranted. Work events differ from job features in that they are “discrete and bounded in space and time“ [
1]. Therefore, the study of work events (versus job characteristics) would provide the opportunity to add a temporal component to the research on job characteristics [
6] and to examine work events as more proximal antecedents of work engagement [
13,
14]. In other words, the study of work events rather than job characteristics would provide the opportunity to specify and examine how the different situations that employees experience at work combine to affect work engagement over time. For instance, over the course of a workweek, employees are likely to experience a series of positive events (e.g., praise from a supervisor after successfully finishing an important task) and negative events (e.g., an episode of interpersonal conflict with colleagues). Although the occurrence of each of these events is individually associated with short-term fluctuations in work engagement in its own right [
15], it is likely that the last week’s work events carry over to affect work engagement during the current workweek [
16]. Furthermore, different work events may interact to predict work engagement. Put another way, receiving praise from one’s supervisor in the current week’s team meeting may foster work engagement, but the events of the previous week (e.g., positive feedback from the same supervisor or interpersonal conflict with colleagues) may alter the impact of this current event [see 1]. Hence, it is worthwhile to consider work events embedded within a chain of events over time [
17]. To account for the richness of the work experience [
18], we drew on a taxonomy of work events that encompasses a broad range of relevant positive and negative work events [
7]. This taxonomy was derived from qualitative research [
7] and can be considered exhaustive with regard to the most relevant work events from the perspective of employees. The taxonomy provides an integrative framework covering a broad range of work events that have been considered in the literature to date (see [
7] for a literature review). In the present study, we leverage this taxonomy to determine which specific type of work event is most relevant to work engagement, aside from the effects of positive and negative work events in general terms.
Above, we have outlined that an event-oriented approach permits specifying the order of what happens and interactions among current events and recent events. Recently, Wickham and Knee [
16] have proposed applying experience sampling data to analyze such interactions between current events and recent events to describe psychological processes of sensitization and satiation over time. For instance, in the case of sensitization, last week’s conflict makes the current week’s conflict seem worse. That is, employees become more vulnerable or susceptible to work events with each episode. Conversely, in the case of satiation, last week’s conflict makes this week’s conflict seem less threatening. In other words, employees become less vulnerable or susceptible to work events with each episode. We adopt this approach to examine sensitization and satiation to the study of both positive and negative events predicting work engagement. Furthermore, a positive event like praise from the supervisor may yield particularly strong effects on work engagement after a negative event has occurred [
17,
19,
20]. Hence, we extend the sensitization-satiation perspective and scrutinize the interplay of positive events and negative events from one week to the next week. Interestingly, experiencing a set of events in a given order (e.g., conflict with colleagues after praise from the supervisor) may not be equivalent to the reverse order and is likely to result in different levels of work engagement. However, theory and empirical research on job characteristics and work engagement so far have largely focused on the situational features of work [
12] and have rarely considered temporal issues in depth. Put another way, research on job demands and resources usually does not distinguish between experiencing a specific resource prior to or after being confronted with high levels of a specific job demand. Accordingly, in this study, we aim to account for the order of positive and negative events and examine competing hypotheses. Given that job characteristics are linked to work events as more proximal precursors of work engagement [
13,
14], our event-oriented temporal approach has implications beyond the study of work events per se. In this sense, the different types of work events correspond to immediate situational consequences of a broad range of job characteristics [
13,
14]. Hence, our research informs researchers interested in the interplay of job demands and job resources and may contribute to reconcile inconsistent findings on this interplay as well.
On a related note, it is important to gain insights into how frequent exposure to positive and negative events may accumulate to affect work engagement over longer periods of time [
6,
21,
22]. These insights are important as they pave the way to connect transient processes to longer-term processes underlying employee well-being [
21]. In the study of work events, researchers have rarely gone beyond considering the cross-sectional associations or short-term effects of events over a couple of hours (see [
7] for a review). Hence, we know little about sustained effects due to the accumulation of negative or positive events over time. However, if work events do not have longer-term implications for individual outcomes, one may question their practical relevance [
21]. Conversely, studying accumulation effects may contribute to gain insights in how mundane events in the daily grind of work add up and lead to potentially profound changes in work engagement over time. We therefore conducted a week-level diary study over a period of four months, which fits these aims best: capturing meaningful events shortly after they happen, but, at the same time, monitoring mid-term changes in work engagement by applying an intensified longitudinal design.
1.1. What Happens in the Short Run: Work Events as Antecedents of Work Engagement
In recent years, evidence on antecedents of work engagement at the intraindividual level has started to accumulate [
12]. However, links between work events and work engagement have rarely been considered explicitly. According to Weiss and Cropanzano [
13], affective events are “things [that] happen to people in work settings” to which “people react emotionally” (p. 11). From the perspective of the conservation of resources theory [
23], positive events signal the availability of resources or opportunities for resource gain [
24]. Given that positive work events refer to experiences that either overlap in content with or are triggered by resources such as rewards or reinforcement [
12,
25,
26], we assume that positive events at work are positively related to work engagement. Accordingly, positive work events, such as praise from supervisors, predict work engagement within [
19,
27] and between individuals [
26]. By contrast, negative events can be considered factors that detract attention and may inhibit engagement in focal tasks [
28]. So far, empirical evidence on negative events and work engagement has been mixed. One study has favored significant negative links between negative events and work engagement at the day-level [
15,
19]. By contrast, other researchers found negligible lagged associations with work engagement [
29]. Their results suggest no lagged main effects of previous-day positive event intensity on work engagement the next day. Moreover, in some studies negative events paradoxically even yielded beneficial lagged effects on job satisfaction [
17] and work engagement [
19]. More specifically, given these inconsistencies, we need to account for what happens in the aftermath of the focal events. Events probably do not affect employee well-being in isolation, and it is unlikely that “participants in diary studies … become a tabula rasa once they have completed the diary report for a given interval” [
16]. Therefore, the present study incorporates a temporal component and considers work events embedded in a series of events that may happen to an employee over time [
1,
17]. For one, we take into account that the effects of recent events on work engagement may carry over from one week to the next and affect work engagement. Second, we consider how past events affect the impact of current work events. Given that there are contrasting views on what the interplay of work events may look like, we formulate competing hypotheses. Prototypical patterns of interactions are depicted in
Figure 1. Panel A refers to prototypical patterns of work engagement that may arise from the interaction of current × lagged positive events. Panel B describes prototypical patterns for interactions of current × lagged negative events. Finally, Panel C illustrates how positive events and negative events may combine over time to affect work engagement. Given that we aim to extend the perspective beyond prior-day-level research, we focus on links and interactions at the week-level, which is a time frame rarely applied to work events. This approach appears to be adequate, because the seven-day week is a salient unit for structuring time [
30]. Furthermore, week-to-week associations tap into less transient and more profound effects over time [
31].
1.2. Temporal Patterns of Positive Events
While the concurrent association between positive events and work engagement is well-established [
15,
19,
26,
27,
29], the carryover effects of positive events on work engagement have rarely been considered (see [
29] for the only exception). However, [
29] was focused on negative event intensity and several features of their design (e.g., events sampled on three consecutive days only, time frame of focal measures referred to the day level), their measures (e.g., affective reaction to events versus frequency of events as predictor), and their focal analyses (e.g., coefficients for positive events when controlling for several other aspects) prevent us from drawing strong conclusions regarding lagged effects of positive events per se. Basically, there are two perspectives: First, positive events experienced in the course of the previous workweek may linger on to affect work engagement in the current week, for instance by means of positive reflection (e.g., about successfully finishing a project) [
32] or capitalization on the same event through social sharing with others [
33]. Second, positive events from the previous workweek may change the way current positive events are perceived and experienced. To investigate these temporal processes, Wickham and Knee [
16] have suggested applying interactions of current events (concurrent) and more recent events (lagged) to experience sampling data. As illustrated in
Figure 1A, there are two prototypical patterns of the interaction. On the one hand, employees may get used to high frequencies of positive events. For instance, research on the hedonic treadmill suggests that individual standards may change, and positive events will be taken for granted when positive events have occurred frequently before [
34]. That is, in light of many positive events in the previous week, current-week high frequencies of positive events have a reduced impact on work engagement. Throughout this manuscript, we label this pattern the satiation effect (right side of
Figure 1A) (Wickham and Knee [
15]). On the other hand, positive events in the past may contribute to benefits even more from current positive events, as positive events broaden awareness for positive events that might follow [
35]. Throughout this manuscript, we label this pattern the intensification effect (right side of
Figure 1A). Positive events may even trigger behaviors of the individual that provoke positive events in the future [
36]. Given that there are competing theoretical views and that prior empirical results do not allow for firm conclusions, we state two competing hypotheses for satiation versus intensification effects:
Hypothesis 1. Concurrent positive events in week n and lagged positive events in week n-1 interact to predict work engagement in week n. Lagged positive events (a) amplify (intensification) or (b) alleviate the effect of concurrent positive events (satiation).
1.3. Temporal Patterns of Negative Events
The rationale regarding sensitization and satiation effects presented above can also be applied to negative events. The prototypical patterns of work engagement are illustrated in
Figure 1B. Negative work events in the previous workweek may affect employees even after a couple of days have passed [
29,
37]. So, negative events in the current workweek may shift attention to negative cues in the environment and make employees react more sensitively to negative events during the next workweek [
22,
38]. In line with this perspective, Farmer and Kashdan [
39] found that individuals reacted more sensitively to negative social events on a given day when negative events had preceded the day before. A prototypical pattern of work engagement is depicted on the left side of
Figure 1B. Throughout this manuscript, we label this pattern the sensitization effect.
By contrast, from the perspective of the allostatic load model [
40], it is also plausible that employees will adapt to negative events and will not mind negative events when they re-occur. This may be because employees might become more proficient in dealing with negative events [
41] or become more resilient due to having been challenged before [
42]. Throughout this manuscript, we label this pattern adaptation [
43]. In sum, negative events in the previous workweek, may make employees either more susceptible to the detrimental effects of negative events (sensitization) or may contribute to adapting to negative events (adaptation, see right side of
Figure 1B). Again, we state two competing hypotheses:
Hypothesis 2. Concurrent negative events in week n and lagged negative events in week n-1 interact to predict work engagement in week n. Lagged negative events (a) amplify (sensitization) or (b) reduce the effect of concurrent negative events (adaptation).
1.4. The Interplay of Positive and Negative Events over Time
Beyond sensitization and satiation effects, our study addresses the question of whether experiencing positive events in the aftermath of negative events results in different levels of work engagement than experiencing positive events after a period of few positive events. Above we have discussed that positive events in the previous week may broaden awareness of and strengthen the impact of current positive events. In a similar way, negative events in the past may also change the way current positive events are perceived. For instance, experience-sampling research on work events and after-work fatigue—a state of tiredness and reduced functional capacity—suggest that employees benefit most from positive events in the face of negative events and in the face of chronically high job demands [
20]. Other researchers have argued that work engagement results from a shift in affect in the aftermath of negative events – that is, down-regulation of negative affect and up-regulation of positive affect [
19]. Empirically, these authors found that negative events enhance, rather than impair work engagement, when followed by high levels of positive affect. Given that prior research is mute on the triggers of affective shift and the beneficial effects on work engagement, we consider positive work events to be predictors, because positive events have consistently been linked to positive affective outcomes [
7]. Positive events in the aftermath of negative events may be particularly beneficial for work engagement because positive events create a contrast effect at the backdrop of prior negative events [
20,
44,
45]. Accordingly, and in line with the contrast after a negative-events perspective, we expect that negative events in the past and current positive events interact to predict work engagement. A prototypical pattern of work engagement is depicted on the left side of
Figure 1C. Throughout this manuscript, we label this pattern the contrast effect. More specifically, we expect that positive events in the aftermath of negative events will have a particularly strong effect on work engagement:
Hypothesis 3. Concurrent positive events in week n and lagged negative events in week n-1 interact to predict work engagement in week n. Lagged negative events amplify the effect of positive events (contrast effect after negative events).
To gain a more complete picture of how positive and negative events interact over time, we need to consider whether positive events in the past change the impact of current negative events. We argue, that positive events in the previous workweek may also contribute to build up personal resources [
24,
25] that change the way current negative events affect work engagement. For instance, a high frequency of positive events in the previous workweek is associated with positive affect [
7] and may therefore replenish coping resources [
46]. In this sense, positive events likely strengthen self-efficacy [
47] and self-regulation capacity [
48] as personal resources (see, for instance [
49]). Hence, after experiencing positive events in the previous workweek, employees may be well-equipped to face negative events in the current week. In line with this idea, Kuba and Scheibe [
15] found that habitual acceptance as a personal resource buffers the detrimental effects of negative events on work engagement at the day-level. Given that positive events likely feed personal resources and that resources, in turn, attenuate the detrimental effects of negative events on work engagement, we assume that positive events in the previous workweek attenuate the impact of negative events in the current week. Throughout this manuscript we label this pattern the buffering effect. A prototypical pattern of work engagement is depicted on the right side of
Figure 1C.
Hypothesis 4. Concurrent negative events in week n and lagged positive events in week n-1 interact to predict work engagement in week n. Lagged positive events attenuate the effect of current negative events (buffering effect).
1.5. What Happens in the Long Run: Sustained Effects of Work Events over Time
Recently, Ilies and Aw (2015) have reviewed the theory and empirical evidence on intraindividual models of well-being and noted that we need to connect transient processes (as reflected in fluctuations in well-being from day to day) to longer-term processes (as reflected in changes in well-being over periods of weeks, months, or years). If applied research provides evidence that, for instance, positive events are associated with sustained changes in work engagement over longer periods of time, these findings would underscore the practical relevance of these concepts in organizations from a practitioner’s point of view, whereas associations at the day- or week-level may reflect fluctuations around characteristic average levels that might be largely stable over time (see also [
34]), sustained effects address the issue of whether work events indeed yield chronically beneficial effects [
21]. Given that prior intraindividual research has not considered this aspect empirically, we examine whether frequent exposure to positive and negative events is associated with mid-term changes in work engagement over time at the interindividual level.
Drawing on the conservation of resources theory [
23], it has been suggested that work engagement results from resource abundance [
50,
51]. According to Halbesleben and colleagues [
25], positive aspects in organizational settings like social support, justice, and trust act as signals that the “investment of resources will help the individual realize his or her goal of achieving more resources.” (p. 1347). Given that positive events tap into these kinds of signals, we assume that a high frequency of such signals over time is associated with gains in work engagement. The frequent experience of positive events over time should accumulate to feed higher levels of work engagement. In other words, trajectories of work engagement should be more positive (steeper increase) when positive events occur frequently compared to when positive events occur infrequently.
Hypothesis 5. Trajectories in work engagement differ between persons dependent upon the frequency of positive events over time. Higher (lower) frequencies of positive events are associated with steeper (flatter) increases in work engagement.
Given the pioneering nature of our study with regard to mid-term trajectories of work engagement dependent upon accumulation of work events, we do not state a formal hypothesis on the effects the frequency of negative events over time might have. However, we do investigate the concurrent effects of the frequency of negative events within our focal analyses on the accumulation of positive events. Our analyses, therefore, also provide insights into the relative importance of positive versus negative events for work engagement in the long run.