Next Article in Journal
Climate Change and Social Perception: A Case Study in Southern Italy
Next Article in Special Issue
Impact of a Mindfulness and Self-Care Program on the Psychological Flexibility and Well-Being of Parents with Children Diagnosed with ADHD
Previous Article in Journal
Dry Port Terminal Location Selection by Applying the Hybrid Grey MCDM Model
Previous Article in Special Issue
Investigating Factors that Influence Math Homework Expectancy: A Multilevel Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Do Learning Approaches Set the Stage for Emotional Well-Being in College Students?

by
Jesús de la Fuente
1,2,*,
Francisco Javier Peralta-Sánchez
2,
José Manuel Martínez-Vicente
2,
Flavia H. Santos
3,
Salvatore Fadda
4 and
Martha Leticia Gaeta-González
5
1
School of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, 31009 Pamplona, Spain
2
School of Psychology, University of Almeria, 04120 Almeria, Spain
3
School of Psychology, University College of Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
4
Health Prevention Service, University of Sassari, 07100 Sassari, Italy
5
School of Education, Autonomous Popular University of the State of Puebla (UPAEP), 72410 Puebla, Mexico
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2020, 12(17), 6984; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176984
Submission received: 28 June 2020 / Revised: 13 August 2020 / Accepted: 21 August 2020 / Published: 27 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Academic Motivation, Performance and Student Well-Being)

Abstract

:
The research aim of this paper was two-fold: to generate evidence that personality factors are linear predictors of the variable approaches to learning (a relevant cognitive-motivational variable of Educational Psychology); and to show that each type of learning approach differentially predicts positive or negative achievement emotions, in three learning situations: class time, study time, and testing. A total of 658 university students voluntarily completed validated questionnaires referring to these three variables. Using an ex post facto design, we conducted correlational analyses, regression analyses, and multiple structural predictions. The results showed that Conscientiousness is associated with and predicts a Deep Approach to learning, while also predicting positive achievement emotions. By contrast, Neuroticism is associated with and significantly predicts a Surface Approach to learning, as well as negative achievement emotions. There are important psychoeducational implications in the university context, both for prevention and for self-improvement, and for programs that offer psychoeducational guidance.

1. Introduction

The experience of students in tertiary education is important because of its consequences in their physical and mental health and in their achievement, adaptation, and well-being [1,2,3,4]. For this reason precisely, it has drawn the interest of researchers in Educational Psychology [5]. Given the impact of stress on the academic and social functioning of university students, it is very important to identify factors that predict stress and well-being [6].

1.1. Students’ Well-Being at University

Analysis of well-being at university has been undertaken in response to the Positive Psychology paradigm [7], and contrasts with the exclusive study of negative stress experiences at university [8]. Well-being at university has become an added value of the university learning experience [9]. Universities compete to produce a comprehensive experience of well-being in their students, beyond the offer of quality training during this academic period [10,11,12].
The well-being of university students has been conceptualized as a multidisciplinary phenomenon; analysis and intervention may occur at general, systemic, and cross-disciplinary levels [13]. Previous research has tried to identify factors that foster such well-being, including the student’s personality [14,15], pleasurable experiences during this period [16], the quality of the teaching process [17], and the quality of the learning context and of professional advancement [18,19].

1.2. Learning Approaches and Students’ Well-Being at University

Traditionally, Educational Psychology research has attempted to identify the personal and contextual variables that predict academic achievement at university. Among the many personal variables that have been analyzed, the construct of learning approaches [20,21,22,23,24,25] is one of the most highly recognized by educational psychologists in the international scientific community. Basically, it is a subjective construct, referring to the way in which students perceive their learning and study activities, as they complete the courses that constitute this academic period. The literature emphasizes two approaches to learning [26,27]: (a) a deep approach, where motivation is intrinsic; learners enjoy exploring the academic subject, which takes on personal meaning to them; (b) a surface approach, where motivation is extrinsic; the learners’ strategy is oriented towards accumulating, storing, and reproducing knowledge, while minimizing the effort required to achieve this [28,29].
There is a large body of research that relates this construct to different motivational, affective, and cognitive variables, where learning approach is shown to predict other factors during the university period [30,31,32,33,34]. Furthermore, this construct has been translated and validated in numerous cultures and countries, showing factor invariance [35].
However, the type of emotional experience associated with each type of learning approach has not yet been clearly established. This question is relevant, because the students’ most prevalent emotions predispose them to a satisfactory or stressful university experience [36]. Certain prior studies have tentatively established this relationship [37]. Others have highlighted the relationship between learning difficulties and well-being [38]. The relationship between learning approaches and self-regulation has also been established [31,39]. Some have even established the relationship between learning approaches and religious beliefs, as mediating variables in the well-being of university students [40]. Teaching preferences have also been established according to the learning approach [41]. In a complementary fashion, achievement emotions during university learning have also become a study variable, being considered a correlate of university students’ well-being [42,43]; however, the precise relationship of achievement emotions to learning approaches is yet to be established.

1.3. Big Five and Learning Approaches

The relationship between personality factors and learning approaches has already been the object of previous analysis, and significant relationships were found [44,45]. Although Conscientiousness proved to be most predictive of achievement, some studies indicate that it combines with other learning-related variables to create this impact, particularly so in the case of learning approaches. The factors that most contribute to predicting individual differences are Conscientiousness and Openness to Experience. They are both good positive predictors of deep learning approaches [46,47,48]. By contrast, Neuroticism is a positive predictor of surface approaches [49,50]. Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, and Deep Approach are each positively associated with academic achievement [51], revealing that both cognitive and non-cognitive variables must be included in any prediction of academic achievement.

1.4. Big Five and Achievement Emotions

Personality factors of the Big Five (BF) model have been consistently and selectively associated with many types of emotional experiences in multiple contexts. Most of the personality factors have been shown to consistently predict positive emotions (especially Conscientiousness), while Neuroticism predicts negative emotions [52,53].
Achievement-related emotions emerge as a construct specific to the realm of learning and academic achievement, based on expectancy value theory [54]. The universal nature of achievement emotions has been reported recently, being consistent across cultures, although with certain modulating adjustments [55]. Given the specificity of the construct, there are few studies to date that relate the BF factors to types of achievement emotions [56,57].
Recent research has reported a consistent relationship between the Big Five model and achievement emotions. The conscientiousness factor predicts positive achievement emotions, while neuroticism predicts negative emotions [58,59].

1.5. Learning Approaches and Achievement Emotions

Different learning approaches reveal different ways of learning and understanding curriculum content and may also be related to different learning and achievement contexts. The learning environment is evidently important in how students engage with learning tasks [60], if we consider that they must make a personal assessment about the teaching context, grading methods, type of course, and the tasks involved in learning [23,61,62] in order to choose their strategies. Recent evidence has established that learning approaches depend on both the student’s degree of self-regulation and how well the teaching process promotes regulation [63,64,65].
Certain recent research has demonstrated the specific relationship between learning strategies and achievement emotions when learning [66,67,68]. However, the degree to which this construct is related to the experience of emotional well-being at university is yet to be understood [69,70]. Other recent studies have shown that positive emotions predispose problem-focused coping strategies and engagement, while negative emotions predispose coping strategies geared to managing emotions, ultimately leading to an emotional state of burnout [71]. On this account, it is important to establish the relations between achievement emotions and learning approaches.

1.6. Objectives and Hypotheses

The aim of this research was to verify whether personality factors predict learning approaches and whether these two aspects jointly predict positive and negative achievement emotions. For this purpose, we tested the following hypotheses: (1) The positive factors of the Big Five model will significantly predict the Deep Approach, especially so in the case of Conscientiousness. The negative factor in the model, Neuroticism, will predict the Surface Approach. (2) The Deep Approach and its components will predict positive emotions, while the Surface Approach will predict negative emotions. (3) Conscientiousness and Deep Approach will appear as joint predictors of positive emotions, and Neuroticism and the Surface Approach will jointly predict negative emotions.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The study sample contained a total of 642 undergraduate students who were enrolled at one of two universities in Spain. The students pursued degrees in Psychology, Primary Education, and Educational Psychology; 85.5% were female and 14.5% were male. Ages ranged from 19 to 25, with a mean of 21.33 years. The students were evenly split between the two universities, 324 attended one and 318 attended the other. An incidental, nonrandomized study design was used. Each university’s Guidance Department invited participation from the teachers, and the teachers invited their students to participate, on an anonymous, voluntary basis. Each class subject was considered one specific teaching-learning process, questionnaires were completed online for each subject.

2.2. Instruments

We used the Big Five Questionnaire BFQ-N [71], based on the original version [72], in its adaptation for young university students [73]. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) reproduced a five-factor structure corresponding to the Big Five Model. Adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices were found. The second-order confirmatory model showed good fit (Chi-square = 38.273; Degrees of freedom (20–15) = 5; p < 0.001, Incremental Fix Index, NFI = 0.939; Relative Fix Index, RFI = 0.917; Incremental Fix Index, IFI = 0.947; Tucker-Lewis Index, TLI = 0.937, CFI = 0.946; Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, RMSEA = 0.065; HOELTER = 2453 (p < 0.05) and, 617 (p < 0.01)). The total scale also showed good internal consistency (Alpha = 0.956; Part 1 = 0.932, Part 2 = 0.832; Spearman-Brown = 0.962; Guttman = 0.932).
Learning Approaches. This variable was measured using the revised two-factor study process questionnaire, R-SPQ-2F [74,75], in its Spanish validated version [76]. There are four subscales (Deep Motivation, Deep Strategy, Surface Motivation, Surface Strategy) that measure the two dimensions of Deep and Surface approaches to learning, respectively. Items are answered on a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 (‘rarely true of me’) to 5 (‘always true of me’). A second factor structure with two factors was produced by using confirmatory factor analysis (Chi-Square = 2645.77; df = 169, CFI = 0.95, GFI = 0.91, AGFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.07). Reliability coefficients were also acceptable (Deep, α = 0.81; Surface, α = 0.77), similar to what the original authors found.
Achievement Emotions Questionnaire, AEQ [77]. This instrument includes scales for nine different emotions (enjoyment, hope, pride, relief, anger, anxiety, hopelessness, shame, and boredom), classified along two axes. One axis is their valence, as positive or negative emotions; the other axis refers to activation, whether the emotions are activating or deactivating. Emotions are then categorized into the four quadrants: (1) positive activating: enjoyment, hope, pride; (2) positive deactivating: relief; (3) negative activating: anger, anxiety, shame; (4) negative deactivating: hopelessness, boredom.
They can also be classified according to the source of the emotion: the activity in progress (enjoyment, boredom, anger), a prospective outcome (hope, anxiety, hopelessness), or a retrospective outcome (pride, relief, shame). A factor structure that corresponds to the AEQ Model was confirmed in this sample through Confirmatory Factor Analysis [78]:
(1) Achievement Emotions in Class (80 items). Results showed adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices. The confirmatory model showed good fit (Chi-square = 643,028; Degrees of freedom = 79; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.954; RFI = 0.967; IFI = 0.953; TLI = 0.958, CFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.081; SMRM = 8.13; HOELTER = 156 (p < 0.05) and 158 (p < 0.01). Internal consistency for the total scale was good (Alpha = 0.904; Part 1 = 0.803, Part 2 = 0.853; Spearman-Brown = 0.903 and 853; Guttman = 0.862). Sample items include: Item 1: I get excited about going to class; Item 36: I get bored; Item 75: I feel so hopeless that all my energy is depleted.
(2) Achievement Emotions during Study time (80 items). Adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices were found. The confirmatory model showed good fit (Chi-square = 629,890; Degrees of freedom = 79; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.964; RFI = 0.957; IFI = 0.973; TLI = 0.978, CFI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.080; SRMR = 7.91; HOELTER = 165 (p < 0.05) and 178 (p < 0.01). The total scale also showed good internal consistency (Alpha = 0.939; Part 1 = 0.880, Part 2 = 0.864; Spearman-Brown = 0.913 and 884; Guttman = 0.903). Sample items include: Item 90: I get angry when I have to study; Item 113: My sense of confidence motivates me; Item 144: I am proud of myself.
(3) Achievement Emotions in Testing (80 items). Adequate psychometric properties and acceptable fit indices were found The confirmatory model showed good fit (Chi-square = 376,658; Degrees of freedom = 79; p < 0.001; NFI = 0.978; RFI = 0.969; IFI = 0.983; TLI = 0.978, CFI = 0.963; RMSEA = 0.080; SRMR = 4.76; HOELTER = 169 (p < 0.05) and 188 (p < 0.01)). Internal consistency for the total scale was good (Alpha = 0.913; Part 1 = 0.870, Part 2 = 0.864; Spearman-Brown = 0.824 and 0.869; Guttman = 0.868). Sample items include: Item 170: Before the exam I feel nervous and uneasy; Item 181: I enjoy taking the exam; Item 224: I am very satisfied with myself.

2.3. Procedure

Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Scales were completed on a voluntary basis, using an online platform [79]. Over a two-year period, students reported on five specific teaching-learning processes, each one referring to a different university subject they were taking during this time. The September-October assessment, in 2018 and 2019, covered Presage variables. Process variables were assessed in the following February–March, and Product variables in May–June. The procedure was approved by the respective Ethics Committees of the two universities, in the larger context of an R&D Project (2018–2021).

2.4. Data Analysis

Hypothesis 1.
The positive factors of the Big Five model will significantly predict the Deep Approach, especially so in the case of Conscientiousness. The negative factor in the model, Neuroticism, will predict the Surface Approach.
Hypothesis 2.
The Deep Approach and its components will predict positive emotions, while the Surface Approach will predict negative emotions.
Hypothesis 3.
Conscientiousness and Deep Approach will appear as joint predictors of positive emotions, and Neuroticism and the Surface Approach will jointly predict negative emotions.
Previous analyses. In order to ensure that the university variable did not affect the analyses, we confirmed that there were no significant differences between the variables analyzed, using different one-way and multi-way ANOVAs.
Correlation analyses. For Hypothesis 1, we also calculated reliability (Pearson bivariate correlation) using SPSS (version 25, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).
Multiple regression. For Hypothesis 2, we conducted a multiple regression analysis, also using SPSS (V. 25).
Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Reliability. For Hypothesis 3, a Structural Equation Model (SEM) was used to test in this sample. Data were aggregated by the determination of factors obtained in the previous exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses—not in a summational fashion, in order to avoid false positives. We assessed model fit by first examining the ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom, SRMR, then the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Normed Fit Index (NFI), Incremental Fit Index (IFI), and Relative Fit Index (RFI). Ideally, these should all be greater than 0.90. Sample size adequacy was checked using the Hoelter Index [80]. The analyses were conducted using AMOS (version 22, IBM Corporation, Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Big Five and Learning Approaches

3.1.1. Bivariate Association

Bivariate association results showed that the factors of Conscientiousness (C), Openness (O), Extraversion (E), and Agreeableness (A) had a significant, positive association with the Deep Approach (DA), and negative association with the Surface Approach (SA). A significant, negative relationship also appeared between Neuroticism (N) and the Deep Approach; its relationship with the Surface Approach was positive. Association strength was greatest between the personality factors and the factor Deep Motivation (DM). Direct values are presented in Table 1.

3.1.2. Multiple Regression

Regression results were consistent with previous results, showing that the personality factors C and O positively predicted the Deep Approach and negatively predicted the Surface Approach. Factor N positively predicted the Surface Approach and negatively predicted the Deep Approach. Statistical effect size appeared in the prediction of the Deep Approach. See Table 2 for more details.

3.2. Learning Approaches and Achievement Emotions

3.2.1. Bivariate Association

Bivariate association results showed that the Deep Approach (DA) and its components had a consistent, significant, positive association with positive emotions, and were negatively associated with negative emotions. In the case of the Surface Approach (SA), the inverse effect appeared, correlating negatively with positive emotions, and correlating positively with negative emotions. The strongest associations were seen with the positive emotion of enjoyment, and the negative, deactivating emotions of boredom and hopelessness. As seen in Table 3, this behavioral pattern is stable across the three academic situations examined.

3.2.2. Multiple Regression

Results of the multiple regression analyses between Achievement Emotions (IVs) and Learning Approaches (DV) showed that: (1) the positive emotions enjoyment and hope positively predict the Deep Approach (DA), especially the DM component. However, the emotions of boredom, anger, and hopelessness negatively predict DA. The negative emotion of shame also predicts this approach in study and testing situations. (2) The emotions that positively predict Surface Approach (SA) are boredom and hopelessness, particularly so in the aspect of Surface Strategies (SS). There are some differences between the situations, however. In the study and testing situations, enjoyment negatively predicts the Surface Approach, while boredom and anger are positive predictors. The hopelessness emotion also positively predicts the Surface Approach, in the class situation as well as in testing. See Table 4.

3.3. Big Five and Achievement Emotions

3.3.1. Bivariate Association

Bivariate association results showed that Big Five (BF) and its components (E,C,A, and O) had a consistent, significant, positive association with positive emotions, as well as a negative association with negative emotions. In the case of Neuroticism (N), the inverse effect appeared, correlating negatively with positive emotions, and correlating positively with negative emotions. The strongest associations were seen with the negative emotions of anxiety, anger, and hopelessness. As seen in Table 5, this behavioral pattern is stable across the three academic situations examined. The strongest positive association between N and negative emotions was produced in study and testing situations.

3.3.2. Multiple Regression

Results of the multiple regression analyses between Achievement Emotions (IVs) and the Big Five (DVs) showed that: (1) the positive emotions enjoyment and hope positively predicted E, C, and A, and negatively predicted N, in class and testing, but not in the study situation. Predictions that differ according to the situation are worth noting. In the class situation, the factors mostly strongly predicted by emotions are C and E; in the study and testing situations, C and N are most strongly predicted. In no situation was C predicted by enjoyment. Negative emotions (anger or anxiety) were usually predictors of the N factor; however, in the study and testing situations, for example, they are predictive of both C and E. See Table 6.

3.4. Structural Predictions: Personality, Learning Approaches and Achievement Emotions

Multiple prediction analysis, using SEM, showed three consistent prediction models; their statistics are presented in Table 7. Model 0 (three situations) tested the relationship with the complete construct (5 BF factors), and the statistical values obtained were less adequate. Models 1 to 3 selected only C and N as predictors; these models showed adequate significance.

3.5. Model 1. Class

3.5.1. Direct Effects

There were several significant, direct predictive effects. Conscientiousness (C) was a significant positive predictor of the Deep Approach (while negatively predicting the Surface Approach); the Deep approach in turn predicted Positive Emotions (PE). However, Neuroticism (N) was a significant positive predictor of the Surface Approach (SA) and of Negative Emotions (NE). There was also a positive direct effect of DA on Positive Emotions, and of SA on Negative Emotions. See Table 8 for more details.

3.5.2. Indirect Effects

There were several indirect positive effects of the Conscientiousness factor on DA factors and on Positive Emotions, as well as negative effects on SA factors and on Negative Emotions. In the case of Neuroticism, the positive effect was on SA factors and on Negative Emotions. Also worth noting is the negative indirect effect of the DA factor on the SA factor, as well as on Negative Emotions, and the positive effect of the SA factor on Negative Emotions. See Table 9 for more details. Figure 1 graphically illustrates these effects.

3.6. Model 2. Study Situation

3.6.1. Direct effects

The Conscientiousness factor (C) showed a negative direct effect on Neuroticism (N). It also had a positive effect on DA and a negative effect on SA, as well as on Positive Emotions. The N factor, however, was a positive, indirect predictor of SA and of Negative Emotions. DA appeared as a negative predictor of SA and positive predictor of Positive Emotions. SA appeared as a positive predictor of Negative Emotions. See Table 10 for further details.

3.6.2. Indirect Effects

In a complementary fashion, the C factor had numerous positive indirect effects on the DA factor and its components, as well as on positive emotions. The N factor showed these effects in the opposite direction. Similarly, the Deep Approach showed an indirect predictive effect on emotions, positively predicting Positive Emotions, and negatively predicting Negative Emotions. See Table 11 and Figure 2 for further details.

3.7. Model 3. Test Situation

3.7.1. Direct Effects

The C factor negatively predicted N and SA, while positively predicting DA and Positive Emotions. The N factor positively predicted SA and negatively predicted Negative Emotions. The Deep Approach positively predicted Positive Emotions, while the Surface Approach predicted Negative Emotions. See Table 12 and Figure 3 for further details.

3.7.2. Indirect Effects

The C factor had a negative predictive effect on N, SA, and Negative Emotions. It also had a positive effect toward DM, its components, and Positive Emotions. The N factor produced effects in the opposite direction. As for predictive effects on DA, C was a positive predictor, and N was a negative predictor. See Table 13 and Figure 3 for further details.

4. Discussion

Generally speaking, the results support our hypotheses. Regarding the first hypothesis, on the potential association and linear prediction between personality variables and learning approaches, the relationships found here confirmed prior evidence. While Conscientiousness positively predicted the Deep Approach and negatively predicted the Surface Approach and its components, Neuroticism positively predicted the SA approach and its components. These results are similar to those reported in previous research, where these two factors appear as protective vs. risk factors with respect to achievement [14,49,51,52,59,81,82]. These results remain unchanged in the three situations analyzed (class, study, testing); This would suggest a constant effect of personality factors on motivation, with either positive or negative directionality [83].
Regarding the second hypothesis, on the possible association and predictive relationship of learning approaches and achievement emotions, there was a consistent relationship between DA and Positive Emotions, and between SA and Negative Emotions. These results are novel because they offer precise evidence of how learning approaches also possess an unmistakable emotional component [84]. The classical view of learning approaches, as eminently cognitive-motivational variables, should therefore incorporate these affective-type results [85] (Sharp, Sharp, & Young, 2020). It seems reasonable to assume that positive emotions (enjoyment, etc.) positively reinforce one’s motivational state during class, study and testing; while the feedback of negative emotions (anger, anxiety, etc.) interferes with learning [86,87,88]. The latter would contribute to greater avoidance and flight responses because of the negative emotional component of this learning profile. It is furthermore interesting to note that, while the positive emotions of DA are more associated with the DM component, in the case of SA, emotions are linearly associated with SS more than with SM, indicating that the emotional state affects the cognitive processes of surface strategy, and not only the surface motivational state [66]. The negative deactivating emotion of boredom has greater weight in class and study situations, while the positive deactivating emotion of relief is more relevant in testing situations. However, the relationship is maintained in the three situations, revealing stability in the students’ emotional responses, according to their learning approaches. This would confer on learning approaches a personalistic component, or a stable motivational-affective style [50,88,89].
Regarding the third hypothesis, the results present three structural predictive models, which are quite similar in the three situations analyzed. This demonstrates that the relationship between the BF characteristics (C and N, as essential factors), learning approaches (as a mediating variable), and achievement emotions is stable in the three situations (class, study, testing), despite the differences between them. The differences in emotional response between one situation and another, according to their level of additional stress, has been analyzed previously [90].
That emotions would be jointly predicted by personality variables and learning approaches, the structural models have shown two consistent triangles—seen graphically in our figures. One triangle represents protective variables of learning (C- > DA- > PE) and one triangle represents risk variables (N- > SA- > NE). Moreover, the two triangles are produced consistently across the three learning situations. Although this result is modulated by the statistically greater predictive strength of the personality variables (C and N), it clearly shows the role of learning approaches in predicting positive vs. negative achievement emotions [91]. This relationship shows that personality factors would also have a direct predictive effect on learning approaches, and an indirect effect on achievement emotions; in this relationship, learning approaches would also predict different types of emotions. If we join this relationship to prior evidence that shows that positive emotions predispose problem-focused coping strategies and engagement, while negative emotions predispose emotion-focused strategies and burnout [71], we can conclude that surface learning approaches would tend toward burnout, while deep approaches tend toward engagement. This complex relationship introduces new emotional factors that have not been sufficiently addressed in the research to date.

5. Conclusions

In summary, this research has shown that learning approaches also have a positive or negative emotional dimension that is worthy of consideration. Thus, while the metacognitive component of learning approaches helps regulate cognitive strategies [70,92,93] (there also seems to be an emotion-regulating component. The Deep Approach not only involves cognitive regulation but also emotion regulation, thanks to the Conscientiousness factor. However, in the case of the Surface Approach, the opposite occurs, that is, it is associated with a lack of cognitive and emotional regulation, due to the effect of Neuroticism.

5.1. Limitations and Future Research

The present research study also has limitations. The first limitation of this study refers to its limited sample. Future research should expand on student characteristics and different university origins. Another relevant limitation to consider is that the characteristics of the teaching process under way have not been taken into account when measuring the learning approaches. Recent research has shown how the teaching process induces modifying effects on motivation [94] (Kaplan & Patrick, 2016).
Another important limitation refers to the male/female imbalance in our sample. Prior research has shown the importance of the female gender in learning approaches [95]. Emerging adult women also show greater interindividual variability than men in N and C, in their trajectories between the ages of 16 and 20 years [96]. Consequently, these aspects should be analyzed in future research.
Finally, one important limitation in the present research has to do with the absence of context variables in the analysis. It must not be forgotten that learning approaches are also mediated by factors from the teaching context, and may also mediate the relationships presented here [97,98,99,100]. Future research must integrate analysis of the role of context variables in learning approaches.

5.2. Practical Implications: Psychoeducational Intervention

One important practical implication for Educational Psychologists is the need to become aware of students’ individual differences, to detect personal characteristics that may be predictive of inappropriate learning approaches. This is especially important in reference to emotional experiences [101] (Li, 2020). University Guidance and Counseling services have a very important role in this preventive evaluation. Assessment of university students’ personality characteristics and achievement emotions, in the three situations analyzed, can be a first-order preventive strategy. This information would allow us to detect adaptive or maladaptive emotional states in students, so that we can adjust our intervention to each situation. For example, intervention for the emotion of boredom in class is different from intervention to improve enjoyment while learning or test anxiety during exams.
Another implication for students is the need to assess the student variable of approach to learning, and so be able to promote any help they may need, and to identify students who will most likely need psychoeducational counseling during the course of their university studies. It would be interesting to implement a comprehensive program for managing emotions and coping with stress at university, given the effect that negative emotions have on students and how they may trigger academic burnout [42]. Students with surface approaches to learning are more likely to experience negative emotionality and, consequently, to end in an emotional state of burnout; a deep approach can also lead a high level of perfectionism [71]. Intervention for improving and adjusting coping strategies during study, personalized for each student, might be of considerable help.
Finally, it is essential that teaching faculties become familiar with this evidence and are aware of the relationships between these variables in their students. Effective or regulatory teaching helps to minimize harmful factors, and is largely dependent on the university teacher [17]. Teachers should know their students’ approach to learning in order to adjust the teaching process and help students improve their learning process, whether in class, study time, or an exam situation [101,102].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.d.l.F.; data curation, F.J.P.-S., F.H.S. and M.L.G.-G.; investigation, F.J.P.-S.; methodology, J.d.l.F.; project administration, J.M.M.-V.; resources, S.F.; software, M.L.G.-G.; writing—review & editing, J.d.l.F. and F.H.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the R&D Project PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra (Ministry of Science and Education, Spain), and the R&D Project UAL18-SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER (University of Almería, Spain), and the European Social Fund.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Denovan, A.; Macaskill, A. Stress and subjective well-being among first year UK undergraduate students. J. Happiness Stud. 2017, 18, 505–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Shokri, O.; Kadivar, P.; Naghsh, Z.; Ghanai, Z.; Daneshvarpour, Z.; Molaei, M. Personality traits, academic stress, and academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 2007, 3, 25–48. [Google Scholar]
  3. Vaez, M.; Laflamme, L. Experienced stress, psychological symptoms, self-rated health and academic achievement: A longitudinal study of Swedish university students. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2008, 36, 183–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Winerman, L. By the numbers: Stress on campus. APA Monit. Psychol. 2017, 48, 88. [Google Scholar]
  5. de la Fuente, J.; Zapata, L.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Sander, P.; Cardelle-Elawar, M. The role of personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching to predict motivational-affective variables, achievement, and satisfaction: A structural model. Front. Psychol 2015, 6, 399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Seligman, M.E.P.; Csikszentmihalyi, M. Positive Psychology—An Introduction. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Stallman, H.M. Psychological distress in university students: A comparison with general population data. Aust. Psychol. 2010, 45, 249–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Karaman, M.A.; Lerma, E.; Vela, J.C.; Watson, J.C. Predictors of academic stress among college students. J. Coll. Couns. 2019, 22, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Douglas, J.A.; Douglas, A.; McClelland, R.J.; Davies, J. Understanding Student Satisfaction and Dissatisfaction: An Interpretive Study in the UK Higher Education Context. Stud. High. Educ. 2015, 40, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Browne, B.A.; Kaldenberg, D.O.; Browne, W.G.; Brown, D.J. Student as Customer: Factors Affecting Satisfaction and Assessments of Institutional Quality. J. Mark. High. Educ. 1998, 8, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Durand-Bush, N.; McNeill, K.; Harding, M.; Dobransky, J. Investigating Stress, Psychological Well-Being, Mental Health Functioning, and Self-Regulation Capacity Among University Undergraduate Students: Is This Population Optimally Functioning? Can. J. Couns. Psychother. 2015, 49, 253–274. [Google Scholar]
  12. Elassy, N. The Concepts of Quality, Quality Assurance and Quality Enhancement. Qual. Assur. Educ. 2015, 23, 250–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Van der Bijl-Brouwer, M.; Key, T.; Kligyte, G.; Malcolm, B.; Thurgood, C.; Reddy, P. Improving Wellbeing in Universities. In Proceedings of the Relating Systems Thinking and Design—RSD8 Symposium, Chicago, IL, USA, 17–19 October 2019. [Google Scholar]
  14. Chesser, S.; Murrah, W.; Forbes, S.A. Impact of Personality on Choice of Instructional Delivery and Students’ Performance. Am. J. Distance Educ. 2020, 34, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kennedy, J.W. The Relationship between the Big Five Personality Traits and Likelihood of Experiencing Flow. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 160, 109931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Denovan, A.; Dagnalla, N.; Dhingrab, K.; Grogana, S. Evaluating the Perceived Stress Scale among UK university students: Implications for stress measurement and management. Stud. High. Educ. 2019, 44, 120–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Avry, S.; Chanel, G.; Bétrancourt, M.; Molinari, G. Achievement appraisals, emotions and socio-cognitive processes: How they interplay in collaborative problem-solving? Comput. Hum. 2020, 107, 106267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Dyrbye, L.N.; Lipscomb, W.; Thibault, G. Redesigning the learning environment to promote learner well-being and professional development. Acad. Med. 2020, 95, 674–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. White, M.A.; Kern, M.L. Positive education: Learning and teaching for wellbeing and academic mastery. Int. J. Wellbeing 2018, 8, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Biggs, J. Assessing Student Approaches to Learning. Aust. Psychol. 1988, 23, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Biggs, J. The Reflective Institution: Assuring and Enhancing the Quality of Teaching and Learning. High. Educ. 2001, 41, 221–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Marton, F. What Does it Take to Learn? Some Implications of an Alternative View of Learning. In Strategies for Research and Development in Higher Education; Entwistle, N.J., Ed.; Swets and Zeitlinger: Amsterdam, The Netherland, 1976; pp. 32–43. [Google Scholar]
  23. Biggs, J.; Tang, C. Teaching for Quality Learning at University; Open University Press: Berkshire, UK, 2011; p. 106267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Biggs, J.; Tang, C. Train-the-Trainers: Implementing Outcomes-Based Teaching and Learning in Malaysian Higher Education. Malays. J. Learn. Instr. 2011, 8, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Entwistle, N.J.; Ramsden, P. Understanding Student Learning; Croom Helm: London, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  26. Entwistle, N.J. Teaching for Understanding at University: Deep Approaches and Distinctive Ways of Thinking; Palgrave Macmillan: Basingstoke, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  27. Entwistle, N.J. Student Learning and Academic Understanding: A Research Perspective and Implications for Teaching; Elsevier: New York, NY, USA; Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  28. Asikainen, H.; Parpala, A.; Lindblom-Yl€anne, S.; Vanthournout, G.; Coertjens, L. The development of approaches to learning and perceptions of the teaching-learning environment during bachelor level studies and their relation to study success. High. Educ. Stud. 2014, 4, 24–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Asikainen, H.; Parpala, A.; Virtanen, V.; Lindblom-Ylanne, S. The relationship between student learning process, study success and the nature of assessment: A qualitative study. Stud. Educ. Eval. 2013, 39, 211–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Asikainen, H.; Gijbels, D. Do students develop towards more Deep approaches to learning during studies? A systematic review on the development of students’ deep and surface approaches to learning in higher education. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 29, 205–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. de la Fuente, J.; Pichardo, M.C.; Justicia, F.; García-Berbén, A. Learning approaches, Self-Regulation and achievement in three European universities. Psicothema 2008, 20, 705–711. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  32. Monroy, F.; González-Geraldo, J.L. Measuring learning: Discrepancies between conceptions of and approaches to learning. Educ. Stud. 2018, 44, 81–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Parpala, A.; Lindblom-Ylänne, S.; Komulainen, E.; Litmanen, T.; Hirsto, L. Students’ approaches to learning and their experiences of the teaching-learning environment in different disciplines. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 80, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sam, L. Relationship between Learning Approaches and Academic Achievement of Accounting Education Students. Int. J. Sci. Res. Publ. 2020, 10, 919–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Barros, R.; Monteiro, A.; Nejmedinne, F.; Moreira, J.A. The Relationship between Students’ Approach to Learning and Lifelong Learning. Psychology 2012, 4, 792–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Rubin, M.; Scevak, J.; Southgate, E.; Macqueen, S.; Williams, P.; Douglas, H. Older women, deeper learning, and greater satisfaction at university: Age and gender predict university students’ learning approach and degree satisfaction. J. Divers. High. Educ. 2018, 11, 82–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Tho, N.D.; Trang, N.T.; Gregory, S. Positivity and quality of college life of business students: The mediating role of learning approaches. Stud. Educ. Eval. 2020, 66, 100908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Korhonen, J.; Linnanmäki, K.; Aunio, P. Learning difficulties, academic well-being and educational dropout: A person-centred approach. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 31, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Heikkilä, A. University Students’ Approaches to Learning, Self-Regulation, and Cognitive and Attributional Strategies. Connections with Well-Being and Academic Success. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  40. Muñoz-García, A. Is religión Independent of Students´ Approaches to Learning. Studia Psychol. 2013, 55, 215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Baeten, M.; Dochy, F.; Struyven, K.; Parmentier, E.; Vanderbruggen, A. Student-centred learning environments: An investigation into student teachers’ instructional preferences and approaches to learning. Learn. Environ. Res. 2015, 19, 443–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Miller, Z.; Godfroid, A. Emotions in incidental language learning: An individual approach. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2020, 42, 115–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ruohoniemi, M.; Parpala, A.; Lindblom-Ylänne, S.; Katajavuori, N. Relationships Between Students’ Approaches to Learning, Perceptions of the Teaching, Learning Environment, and Study Success: A Case Study of Third-Year Veterinary Students. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2010, 37, 282–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Moreira, P.A.; Inman, R.A.; Rosa, I.; Cloninger, K.; Duarte, A.; Cloninger, C.R. The psychobiological model of personality and its association with student approaches to learning: Integrating temperament and character. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 2020, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Von Stumm, S.; Furnham, A.F. Learning approaches: Associations with typical intellectual engagement, intelligence and the big five. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 720–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Busato, V.V.; Prins, F.J.; Elshout, J.J.; Hamaker, C. Intellectual ability, learning style, personality, achievement motivation and academic success of psychology students in higher education. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2000, 29, 1057–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Farsides, T.; Woodfield, R. Individual differences and undergraduate academic success: The roles of personality, intelligence, and application. Personal Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 1225–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lievens, F.; Ones, D.S.; Dilchert, S. Personality scale validities increase throughout medical school. J. Appl. Psychol. 2009, 94, 1514–1535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Chamorro-Premuzic, T.; Furnham, A. Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. J. Res. Personal. 2003, 37, 319–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Zhang, L.F. Does the Big Five predict learning approaches? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 1431–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Chamorro-Premuzic, T.; Furnham, A. Personality, intelligence and approaches to learning as predictors of academic performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2008, 44, 1596–1603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Abe, J.A.A. Big Five, linguistic styles, and successful online learning. Internet High. Educ. 2020, 45, 100724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Sorić, I.; Penezić, Z.; Burić, I. Big Five personality traits, cognitive appraisals and emotion regulation strategies as predictors of achievement emotions. Psihol. Teme 2013, 22, 325–349. [Google Scholar]
  54. Pekrun, R.; Loderer, K. Emotions and learning from multiple representations and perspectives. In Handbook of Learning from Multiple Representations and Perspectives; Meter, P.V., List, A., Lombardi, D., Kendeou, P., Eds.; Routlegd: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  55. Loderer, K.; Gentsch, K.; Duffy, M.C.; Zhuc, M.; Xie, X.; Chavarría, J.A.; Vogl, E.; Soriano, C.; Scherer, K.R.; Pekrun, R. Are concepts of achievement-related emotions universal across cultures? A semantic profiling approach. Cogn. Emot. 2020, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Karagiannopoulou, E.; Milienos, F.S.; Athanasopoulos, V. Associations Between Defense Styles, Approaches to Learning, and Achievement Among University Students. Front. Educ. 2018, 3, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Karagiannopoulou, E.; Milienos, F.S.; Kamtsios, S.; Rentzios, C. Do defence styles and approaches to learning ‘fit together’ in students’ profiles? Differences between years of study. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 40, 570–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. de la Fuente, J.; Paoloni, P.; Kauffman, D.; Yilmaz Soylu, M.; Sander, P.; Zapata, L. Big Five, Self-Regulation, and Coping Strategies as Predictors of Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Sander, P.; de la Fuente, J. Modelling students’ academic confidence, personality and academic emotions. Curr. Psychol. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Monteiro, S.C.; da Silva Almeida, L.; Vasconcelos, R.M.D.C.F. Abordagens à aprendizagem, autorregulação e motivação: Convergência no desempenho acadêmico excelente. Learning approaches, self-regulation and motivation: Converging in excellent academic performance. Revista Brasileira Orientação Profissional 2012, 13, 153–162. [Google Scholar]
  61. Endedijk, M.D.; Vermunt, J.D. Relations between student teachers’ learning patterns and their concrete learning activities. Stud. Educ. Eval. 2013, 39, 56–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Vermunt, J.D. Relations between student learning patterns and personal and contextual factors and academic performance. High. Educ. 2005, 49, 205–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. de la Fuente, J.; Sander, P.; Kauffman, D.; Yilmaz-Soylu, M. Differential Effects of Self- vs External- Regulation on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement and Satisfaction in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Entwistle, N.; McCune, V.; Hounsell, J. Investigating ways of enhancing university teaching-learning environments: Measuring students’ approaches to studying and perceptions of teaching. In Powerful Learning Environments: Unravelling Basic Components and Dimensions, 1st ed.; de Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., Entwistle, N., van Merriënboer, J., Eds.; Pergamon: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2003; p. 89. [Google Scholar]
  65. Kulakow, S. How autonomy support mediates the relationship between self-efficacy and approaches to learning. J. Educ. Res. 2020, 113, 13–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Obergriesser, S.; Stoeger, H. Students’ emotions of enjoyment and boredom and their use of cognitive learning strategies–How do they affect one another? Learn. Instr. 2020, 66, 101285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Schweder, S. The role of control strategies, self-efficacy, and learning behavior in self-directed learning. Int. J. Sch. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 7, 29–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Mohr, S.; Grahn, H.; Krohne, C.; Brätz, J.; Guse, A.H. Academic emotions during an interprofessional learning episode in a clinical context: Assessing within-and between-variation. J. Interprof. Care 2020, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Boekaerts, M. Understanding students’ affective processes in the classroom. In Emotion in Education; Schutz, P.A., Pekrun, R., Eds.; Elsevier: San Diego, CA, USA, 2007; pp. 37–56. [Google Scholar]
  70. Schweder, S. Mastery goals, positive emotions and learning behavior in self-directed vs. teacher-directed learning. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2020, 35, 205–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. de la Fuente, J.; Lahortiga-Ramos, F.; Laspra-Solís, C.; Maestro-Martín, C.; Alustiza, I.; Aubá, E.; Martín-Lanas, R. A Structural Equation Model of Achievement Emotions, Coping Strategies and Engagement-Burnout in Undergraduate Students: A Possible Underlying Mechanism in Facets of Perfectionism. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 2106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Carrasco, M.A.; Holgado, P.; del Barrio, M.V. Dimensionalidad del cuestionario de los cinco grandes (BFQ-N) en población Española Dimensionality of the Big Five questionnaire (BFQ-N) in Spanish population. Psicothema 2005, 17, 286–291. [Google Scholar]
  73. Barbaranelli, C.; Caprara, G.V.; Rabasca, A.; Pastorelli, C. A questionnaire for measuring the Big Five in late childhood. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003, 34, 645–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. de la Fuente, J. Five Scale, BFQ-U. Adaptation for Young University Students; University of Almería: Almería, Spain, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  75. Biggs, J.; Kember, D.; Leung, D.Y.P. The revised two-factor study process questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 71, 133–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Justicia, F.; Pichardo, M.C.; Cano, F.; Berbén, A.B.; de la Fuente, J. The revised two-factor study process questionnaire (RSPQ-2F): Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses at item level. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2008, 23, 355–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Perry, R.P. Academic Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ)—User’s Manual; Department of Psychology, University of Munich: Munich, Germany, 2005; Available online: https://es.scribd.com/doc/217451779/2005-AEQ-Manual (accessed on 24 August 2020).
  78. de la Fuente, J.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Peralta-Sánchez, F.J.; Garzón-Umerenkova, A.; Vera, M.M.; Paoloni, P. Applying the SRL vs. ERL Theory to the Knowledge of Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate University Students. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. de la Fuente, J.; López, M.; Zapata, L.; Sollinas, G.; Fadda, S. Improving mental health through an online self-assessment and self-help e-Utility in university students. In Progress in Education; Nata, R.V., Ed.; Nova Publisher: New York, NY, USA, 2015; Volume 33, pp. 63–74. [Google Scholar]
  80. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Cleaning up your act: Screening data prior to analysis. Using Multivar. Stat. 2001, 5, 61–116. [Google Scholar]
  81. Buelow, M.T.; Cayton, C. Relationships between the Big Five personality characteristics and performance on behavioral decision making tasks. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 160, 109931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Chamorro-Premuzic, T.; Furnham, A. Mainly Openness: The relationship between the Big Five personality traits and learning approaches. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 524–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Ranelluci, J.; Hall, N.C.; Goetz, T. Achievement goals, emotions, learning, and performance: A process model. Motiv. Sci. 2015, 1, 98–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Trigwell, K.; Ellis, R.A.; Han, F. Relations between students’ approaches to learning, experienced emotions and outcomes of learning. Stud. High. Educ. 2012, 37, 811–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Sharp, J.G.; Sharp, J.C.; Young, E. Academic boredom, engagement and the achievement of undergraduate students at university: A review and synthesis of relevant literature. Res. Pap. Educ. 2020, 35, 144–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Boekaerts, M.; Pekrun, R. Emotions and emotion regulation in academic settings. In Handbook of Educational Psychology, 3rd ed.; Corno, L., Anderman, E.M., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 76–91. [Google Scholar]
  87. de la Fuente, J.; Amate, J.; González-Torres, M.C.; Artuch, R.; García-Torrecillas, J.M.; Fadda, S. Effects of Levels of Self-Regulation and Regulatory Teaching on Strategies for Coping with Academic Stress in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Fiedler, K.; Beier, S. Affect and cognitive processes in educational contexts. In International Handbook of Emotion in Education; Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 36–55. [Google Scholar]
  89. Padang, P.A.P. How Is the Student’s Personality in Implementing Science and Technology for Entrepreneurship Learning with a Production-Based Learning Approach in Higher Education? J. Eng. Appl. Sci. 2020, 15, 213–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. de la Fuente, J.; Paoloni, P.V.; Vera-Martínez, M.M.; Garzón-Umerenkova, A. Effect of Levels of Self-Regulation and Situational Stress on Achievement Emotions in Undergraduate Students: Class, Study and Testing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Acee, T.W.; Kim, H.; Kim, H.J.; Kim, J.-C.; Ning, H.R.; Chu, R.; Kim, M.; Cho, Y.; Wicker, F.W. Academic boredom in under- and over-challenging situations. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 35, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Richardson, J.T.E. Approaches to Learning or Levels of Processing: What did Marton and Säljö (1976a) Really Say? The Legacy of the Work of the Göteborg Group in the 1970s. Interchange 2015, 46, 239–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Richardson, M.; Abraham, C.; Bond, R. Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 353–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Kaplan, A.; Patrick, H. Learning environments and motivation. In Handbook of Motivation at School; Wentzel, K.R., Miele, D.B., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 251–275. [Google Scholar]
  95. Vecchione, M.; Alessandri, G.; Barbaranelli, C.; Caprara, G. Gender differences in the Big Five personality development: A longitudinal investigation from late adolescence to emerging adulthood. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 740–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Bost, L.W.; Riccomini, P.J. Effective instruction. Remedial Spec. Educ. 2006, 27, 301–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Du Plessis, A.E. Teacher and Student Well-being: The Policy Link. In Out-of-Field Teaching and Education Policy; Springer: Singapore, 2020; pp. 173–205. [Google Scholar]
  98. Vansteenkiste, M.; Sierens, E.; Goossens, L.; Soenens, B.; Dochy, F.; Mouratidis, A.; Beyers, W. Identifying configurations of perceived teacher autonomy support and structure: Associations with self-regulated learning, motivation and problem behavior. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 431–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Yerdelen, S.; Sungur, S. Multilevel Investigation of Students’ Self-Regulation Processes in Learning Science: Classroom Learning Environment and Teacher Effectiveness. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2019, 17, 89–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Reindl, M.; Tulis, M.; Dresel, M. Profiles of emotional and motivational self-regulation following errors: Associations with learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2020, 77, 101806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Li, C. A positive psychology perspective on chinese EFL students’ trait emotional intelligence, foreign language enjoyment and EFL learning achievement. J. Multiling. Multicult. Dev. 2020, 41, 246–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Cao, Y.; Postareff, L.; Lindblom-Ylanne, L.; Toom, A. Teacher educators’ approaches to teaching and connections with their perceptions of the closeness of their research and teaching. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2019, 85, 125–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Class Situation.
Figure 1. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Class Situation.
Sustainability 12 06984 g001
Figure 2. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Study Situation.
Figure 2. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Study Situation.
Sustainability 12 06984 g002
Figure 3. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Testing Situation.
Figure 3. Predictive relationships of two Factors of the Big Five model (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) on the Learning Approach and Achievement Emotions, in a Testing Situation.
Sustainability 12 06984 g003
Table 1. Bivariate correlations between the Big Five Factors and Learning Approaches (n = 658).
Table 1. Bivariate correlations between the Big Five Factors and Learning Approaches (n = 658).
Learning ApproachesECNAO
DEEP MOTIV0.232 **0.413 **−0.098 *0.226 **0.404 **
DEEP STRAT0.194 **0.413 **−0.0490.155 **0.312 **
DEEP APPR0.227 **0.458 **−0.083 *0.208 **0.394 **
SURFACE MOTI−0.129 **−0.365 **0.129 **−0.140 *−0.222 **
SURFACE STRAG−0.097 *−0.343 **0.197 **−0.0143 *−0.249 **
SURFACE APPR−0.139 **−0.387**0.188 **−0.148 **−0.254 **
Note. E = Extraversion; C = Conscientiousness; N = Neuroticism; A = Agreeableness; O = Openness; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Multiple Regression (Standardized Beta Index) for the Big Five Personality Factors and Learning Approaches (n = 658).
Table 2. Multiple Regression (Standardized Beta Index) for the Big Five Personality Factors and Learning Approaches (n = 658).
Learning ApproachesECNAOEffectR Square
DEEP MOT−0.0170.294 **−0.0050.0050.276 **F(5511) = 31.521 **0.234
DEEP STRAT0.0010.381 **0.040−0.0450.155 **F(5511) = 25.664 **0.201
DEEP APPRO−0.1120.374 **−0.019−0.0220.240 **F(5511) = 35.346 **0.246
SURF MOT0.026−0.333 **0.076 *0.050−0.069F(5511) = 15.401 **0.131
SURF STRAT0.072−0.279 **0.139 **0.082−0.183 **F(5511) = 17.967 **0.149
SURF APPRO0.039−0.320 **0.129 **0.068−0.137 **F(5511) = 19.279 **0.160
Note. E = Extraversion; C = Conscientiousness; N = Neuroticism; A = Agreeableness; O = Openness; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between Learning Approaches and Achievement Emotions, in three situations (n = 658).
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between Learning Approaches and Achievement Emotions, in three situations (n = 658).
CLASSDMDSDASMSSSA
Positive0.521 **0.413 **0.516 **−0.327 ***−0.328 **−0.344 **
Enjoyment0.515 **0.387 **0.542 **−0.329 **−0.336 **−0.356 **
Hope0.479 **0.395 **0.396 **−0.326 **−0.307 **−0.346 **
Pride0.413 **0.350 **0.374 **−0.293 **−0.232 **−0.279 **
Negative−0.224 **−0.150 **−0.212 **0.327 **0.344 **0.371 **
Boredom−0.334 **−0.260 **−0.361 **0.394 **0.429 **0.448 **
Anger−0.249 **−0.175 **−0.242 **0.366 **0.373 **0.405 **
Anxiety−0.161 **−0.075 *−0.146 **0.213 **0.272 **0.270 **
Shame−0.126 **−0.046−0.097 *0.221 **0.249 **0.262 **
Hopelessness−0.263 **−0.187 *−0.230 **0.299 **0.330 **0.352 **
STUDYDMDSDASMSSSA
Positive0.472 **0.395 **0.478 **−0.351 **−0.343 **−0.373 **
Enjoyment0.538 **0.444 **0.542 **−0.329 **−0.366 **−0.356 **
Hope0.394 **0.328 **0.396 **−0.326 **−0.307 **−0.346 **
Pride0.369 **0.313 **0.374 **−0.293 **−0.232 **−0.279 **
Negative−0.296 **−0.183 **−0.266 **0.363 **0.391 **0.413 **
Boredom−0.388 **−0.263 **−0.361 **0.394 **0.429 **0.488 **
Anger−0.257 **−0.175 **−0.242 **0.366 **0.373 **0.405 **
Anxiety−0.168 *−0.094−0.146 *0.213 **0.272 **0.270 **
Shame−0.133 *−0.046−0.097 *0.221 **0.241 **0.262 **
Hopelessness−0.263 **−0.154 *−0.230 **0.229 **0.330 **0.372 **
TESTDMDSDASMSSSA
Positive0.438 **0.339 **0.427 **−0.280 **−0.264 **−0.298 **
Enjoyment0.441 **0.322 **0.427 **−0.267 **−0.278 **−0.298 **
Hope0.396 **0.336 **0.405 **−0.279 **−0.257 **−0.298 **
Pride0.371 **0.284 **0.359 **−0.238 **−0.188 **−0.238 **
Relief0.080 *0.070 *0.84 *−0.059−0.012−0.039
Negative−0.208 **−0.85−0.161 **0.237 **0.307 **0.307 **
Anger−0.196 **−0.087 *−0.156 **0.297 **0.318 **0.334 **
Anxiety−0.131 **−0.048−0.099 **0.100 **0.195 **0.175 **
Shame−0.067−0.006−0.0360.175 **0.170 **0.197 **
Hopelessness−0.228 **−0.115 **−0.189 *0.293 **0.325 **0.342 **
Note. DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategy; DA = Deep Approach; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategy; SA = Surface Approach. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Regression (Standardized Beta Index) of Achievement Emotions to Learning Approaches (n = 658).
Table 4. Regression (Standardized Beta Index) of Achievement Emotions to Learning Approaches (n = 658).
CLASSDMDSDASMSSSA
Enjoyment0.333 ***0.1090.251 **−0.129−0.069−0.086
Hope0.166 *0.242 **0.221 **−0.035−0.125−0.114
Pride0.0040.0710.036−0.0110.0000.015
Boredom−0.171 **−0.174 *−0.183 **−0.1070.245 ***0.208 **
Anger−0.226 **−0.194 *−0.227 **−0.065−0.173 *0.137
Anxiety−0.0550.0970.027−0.075−0.006−0.048
Shame0.084−0.0030.048−0.0210.0110.008
Hopelessness−0.151 *−0.149 *−0.169 *0.360 ***0.267 ***0.335 **
F(8431)24.26014.99623.940 ***12.77914.79815.559 **
R square0.3100.2100.3100.1910.2150.231
STUDYDMDSDASMSSSA
Enjoyment0.588 ***0.470 ***0.585 ***−0.076−0.204 ***−0.160 *
Hope−0.0130.0350.007−0.060−0.068−0.091
Pride−0.139 *−0.081−0.119−0.157 *0.055−0.023
Boredom−0.245 ***−0.107−0.185 **0.212 **0.273 ***0.243 **
Anger0.1070.0290.0690.234 **0.157 *0.221 **
Anxiety−0.047−0.061−0.064−0.084−0.032−0.060
Shame0.0990.198 *0.178 **0.014−0.076−0.048
Hopelessness−0.087−0.085−0.106−0.0930.036−0.007
F(8420)29.714 ***17.149 **29.583 ***17.357 **19.783 **21.003 ***
R square0.3640.2480.3650.2510.2690.298
TESTDMDSDASMSSSA
Enjoyment0.334 ***0.211 **0.334 ***−0.142−0.242 **−0.196 *
Hope0.194 **0.294 ***0.194 **−0.068−0.016−0.054
Pride−0.20−0.009−0.020−0.0510.016−0.018
Relief−0.037−0.075−0.0370.0380.0060.022
Anger−0.153 *−0.115−0.153 *0.243 **0.257 **0.268 **
Anxiety−0.0190.1210.019−0.204 **−0.054−0.132 *
Shame0.160 *0.134 *0.160 *−0.054−0.167 *−0.117
Hopelessness−0.031−0.024−0.0310.164 *0.194 *0.201 *
F(8420)18.03411.05418.304 **9.46011.78012.323 **
R square0.2590.1790.2590.1520.1820.192
Note. DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategy; DA = Deep Approach; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategy; SA = Surface Approach; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Bivariate correlations between Big Five and Achievement Emotions, in three situations (n = 658).
Table 5. Bivariate correlations between Big Five and Achievement Emotions, in three situations (n = 658).
CLASSECNAO
Positive0.380 **0.586 **−0.146 **0.325 **0.523 **
Enjoyment0.285 **0.482 **−0.152 **0.275 **0.517 **
Hope0.382 **0.628 **−0.204 **0.341 **0.417 **
Pride0.369 **0.505 **−0.0660.277 **0.413 **
Negative−0.246 **−0.426 **0.369 **−0.237 **−0.373 **
Boredom−0.081 *−0.437 **0.335 **−0.193 **−0.202 **
Anger−0.177 **−0.384 **0.338 **−0.304 **−0.206 **
Anxiety−0.233 **−0.272 **0.418 **−0.160 **−0.332 **
Shame−0.312 **−0.199 **0.338 **−0.142 **−0.292 **
Hopelessness−0.172 **−0.400 **0.374 **−0.222 **−0.354 **
STUDYECNAO
Positive0.366 **0.566 **−0.168 **0.313 **0.505 **
Enjoyment0.292 **0.526 **−0.112 **0.291 **0.459 **
Hope0.357 **0.528 **−0.225 **0.306 **0.476 **
Pride0.303 **0.505 **−0.079 *0.277 **0.397 **
Negative−0.208 **−0.400 **0.459 **−0.204 **−0.356 **
Boredom−0.153 **−0.515 **0.339 **−0.251 **−0.296 **
Anger−0.068 **−0.340 **0.443 **−0.213 **−0.233 **
Anxiety−0.162 **−0.255 **0.438 **−0.110 **−0.274 **
Shame−0.251 **−0.254 **0.412 **−0.170 **−0.190 **
Hopelessness−0.231 **−0.248 **0.441 **−0.215 **−0.274 **
TESTECNAO
Positive0.352 **0.534 **−0.176 **0.245 **0.480 **
Enjoyment0.292 **0.485 **−0.110 **0.200 **0.435 **
Hope0.327 **0.535 **−0.269 **0.268 **0.469 **
Pride0.341 **0.498 **−0.137 **0.265 **0.411 **
Relief0.099 *0.215 **0.086 *0.168 **0.120 *
Negative−0.104 **−0.234 **0.486 **−0.124 **−0.275 **
Anger−0.061−0.258 **0.415 **−0.205 **−0.257 **
Anxiety−0.070−0.108 *0.438 **−0.025−0.220 **
Shame−0.142 **−0.217 **0.394 **−0.133 **−0.197 **
Hopelessness−0.104 *−0.234 **0.405 **−0.212 **−0.348 **
Note. E = Extraversion; C = Conscientiousness; N = Neuroticism; A = Agreeableness; O = Openness; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001.
Table 6. Regression (Standardized Beta Index) of Achievement Emotions to Big Five in each situation (n = 658).
Table 6. Regression (Standardized Beta Index) of Achievement Emotions to Big Five in each situation (n = 658).
CLASSECNAO
Enjoyment−0.085−0.158 *0.0220.0790.233 **
Hope0.256 **0.535 **−0.161 *0.230 **0.323 **
Pride0.248 **0.105−0.181 **0.042−0.019
Boredom0.201 *−0.211 **0.158 *0.286 **0.171 *
Anger−0.179 *−0.0790.041−0.482 **0.129 *
Anxiety−0.0440.0290.271 **−0.046−0.097
Shame−0.271 **0.1050.034−0.079−0.029
Hopelessness0.225 **−.105−0.0600.061−0.245 **
F(8416)17.357 **40.505 **10.489 **10.447 **26.849 **
R square0.2500.4360.1660.1680.339
STUDYECNAO
Enjoyment0.165 *0.0630.0050.1260.435 ***
Hope0.239 **0.202 **−0.1000.228 **0.102
Pride0.0540.185 **0.134−0.060−0.087
Boredom−0.051−0.494 **−0.001−0.053−0.011
Anger0.304 **0.197 *0.171 *−0.1310.205 **
Anxiety−0.157 **−0.0680.078−0.195 *−0.121
Shame−0.261 **0.0250.199 *−0.097−0.050
Hopelessness0.1540.1090.0770.069−0.229 **
F(8402)12.930 ***32.081 **14.915 ***6.208 **22.808 **
R square0.2050.2480.2300.1120.317
TESTECNAO
Enjoyment−0.069−0.0270.124−0.1180.118
Hope0.200 *0.384 ***−0.144 *0.199 *0.238 **
Pride0.271 **0.179 *−0.0090.1300.090
Relief−0.0950.022−0.0160.0780.013
Anger0.191 **−0.168 **0.239 **−0.243 **−0.075
Anxiety0.1070.219 **0.269 **0.141 *−0.010
Shame−0.1020.0170.1090.0000.137 *
Hopelessness−0.142−0.122−0.0730.036−0.198 *
F(8420)9.146 **26.486 **17.207 **6.045 **19.359 **
R square0.1570.3430.2530.1060.279
Note. E = Extraversion; C = Conscientiousness; N = Neuroticism; A = Agreeableness; O = Openness; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 7. Models of structural linear results of the variables.
Table 7. Models of structural linear results of the variables.
ModelChi SquareDFChi/dfNFIRFITLICFIRMSEAHoelter
05−01
0.CL(BF)863,666 ***1107.850.8650.8120.8800.8320.066246–267
0.STU(BF)1,088,044 ***1119.800.8490.7920.8490.8620.075197–214
0.TES(BF)931,255 ***1118.310.8540.7990.8690.8680.069230–250
1.Class (C&N)502,808 ***697.280.9170.9070.9200.9000.080175–194
2. Study (C&N)504,705 ***697.310.9480.9280.9240.9390.078193–204
3. Test (C&N)508,201 ***697.360.9290.9140.9070.9030.069246–268
Note. Model for each situational context (1 to 3). 0 Models = Situations and BF (Big Five factors); 1,2,3 Models: Situations with C (Conscientiousness) and N (Neuroticism) factors; *** p < 0.001.
Table 8. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Class situation.
Table 8. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Class situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR−0.194
DA0.489 0.484
SA−0.234 −0.321
DM 0.875
DS 0.733
SM 0.783
SS 0.846
POSEM0.436 0.391
NEGEM 0.328 0.296−0.298
ENJOYMENT 0.920
HOPE 0.843
PRIDE 0.836
BOREDOM 0.748
ANGER 0.861
ANXIETY 0.811
SHAME 0.654
HOPELE 0.918
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.
Table 9. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Class situation.
Table 9. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Class situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR
DA
SA−0.183
DM0.358
DS0.428
SM−0.3530.099−0.275
SS−0.3260.107−0.255
POSEM0.191
NEGEM−0.3930.037−0.224
ENJOYM0.529 0.330
HOPE0.577 0.360
PRIDE0.529 0.327
BOREDOM−0.2940.273−0.1680.221−0.245
ANGER−0.3380.314−0.1930.255−0.282
ANXIETY−0.3190.296−0.1820.240−0.266
SHAME−0.2570.239−0.1470.193−0.214
HOPELE−0.3600.335−0.2060.272−0.310
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.
Table 10. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Study situation.
Table 10. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Study situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR−0.195
DA0.486
SA−0.2250.130−0.332
DM 0.869
DS 0.738
SM 0.776
SS 0.853
POSEM0.417 0.374
NEGEM 0.401 0.233−0.348
ENJOYMT 0.865
HOPE 0.860
PRIDE 0.864
BOREDOM 0.777
ANGER 0.845
ANXIETY 0.822
SHAME 0.820
HOPELE 0.913
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.
Table 11. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Study situation.
Table 11. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Study situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR−0.187
DA0.182
SA−0.383 −0.208
DM0.423
DS−0.359
SM−0.3200.110−0.283
SS−0.3520.101−0.253
POSEM0.182
NEGEM−0.1870.030
ENJOYMT0.515 0.324
HOPE0.518 0.322
PRIDE0.515 0.323
BOREDOM−0.2980.335−0.1610.181−0.271
ANGER−0.3230.364−0.1750.197−0.294
ANXIETY−0.3150.355−0.1710.192−0.286
SHAME−0.3140.354−0.1700.1910−0.286
HOPELE−0.3490.394−0.1890.213−0.318
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.
Table 12. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Testing situation.
Table 12. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model): Testing situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR−0.201
DA0.485
SA−0.2430.125−0.323
DM 0.875
DS 0.734
SM 0.779
SS 0.851
POSEM0.426 0.306
NEGEM 0.369 0.198−0.298
ENJOYMENT 0.980
HOPE 0.856
PRIDE 0.886
RELIEF 0.319
ANGER 0.792
ANXIETY 0.716
SHAME 0.811
HOPELE 0.951
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.
Table 13. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Testing situation.
Table 13. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model): Testing situation.
VariablesCONSNEURDASAPOSEMNEGEM
NEUR−0.182
DA
SA−0.182
DM0.424 0.252
DS0.357 0.275
SM−0.3250.97
SS0.3550.106
POSEM0.148
NEGEM−0.3280.025−0.155
ENJOYMT0.521 0.278
HOPE0.491 0.262
PRIDE0.509 0.271
RELIEF0.183 0.98
ANGER−0.2600.312−0.1230.157−0.236
ANXIETY 0.282−0.1110.142−0.213
SHAME 0.319−0.1260.161−0.241
HOPELE 0.375−0.1480.188−0.283
Note. CONS = Conscientiousness; NEUR = Neuroticism; POSEM = Positive Emotions; NEGEM = Negative Emotions; DA = Deep Approaches; SA = Surface Approaches; DM = Deep Motivation; DS = Deep Strategies; SM = Surface Motivation; SS = Surface Strategies. HOPELE = Hopelessness.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

de la Fuente, J.; Peralta-Sánchez, F.J.; Martínez-Vicente, J.M.; Santos, F.H.; Fadda, S.; Gaeta-González, M.L. Do Learning Approaches Set the Stage for Emotional Well-Being in College Students? Sustainability 2020, 12, 6984. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176984

AMA Style

de la Fuente J, Peralta-Sánchez FJ, Martínez-Vicente JM, Santos FH, Fadda S, Gaeta-González ML. Do Learning Approaches Set the Stage for Emotional Well-Being in College Students? Sustainability. 2020; 12(17):6984. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176984

Chicago/Turabian Style

de la Fuente, Jesús, Francisco Javier Peralta-Sánchez, José Manuel Martínez-Vicente, Flavia H. Santos, Salvatore Fadda, and Martha Leticia Gaeta-González. 2020. "Do Learning Approaches Set the Stage for Emotional Well-Being in College Students?" Sustainability 12, no. 17: 6984. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176984

APA Style

de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Santos, F. H., Fadda, S., & Gaeta-González, M. L. (2020). Do Learning Approaches Set the Stage for Emotional Well-Being in College Students? Sustainability, 12(17), 6984. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176984

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop