Next Article in Journal
Multilayer Granular Recycled Rubber for Its Application to Technical Flooring
Previous Article in Journal
Vehicle Emission Changes in China under Different Control Measures over Past Two Decades
Previous Article in Special Issue
In Search of Social Resilience? Regeneration Strategies for Polish Cities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Well-Being and Geography: Modelling Differences in Regional Well-Being Profiles in Case of Spatial Dependence—Evidence from Turkey

1
Izmir Institute of Technology (IZTECH), Department of City and Regional Planning, Izmir 35430, Turkey
2
The Faculty of Management, Open University of the Netherlands (OU), 6419 Heerlen, The Netherlands
3
Centre for European Studies, Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, 700506 Iasi, Romania
4
School of Architecture, Planning and Design, Polytechnic University (UM6P), Ben Guerir 43150, Morocco
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(24), 16370; https://doi.org/10.3390/su142416370
Submission received: 24 October 2022 / Revised: 23 November 2022 / Accepted: 28 November 2022 / Published: 7 December 2022

Abstract

:
The aim of this study is to provide a new quantitative perspective on the geography of well-being using an urban–rural typology and characteristic city size elements in order to detect where people are happier and to examine the determinants of well-being by considering spatial dependence effects. We use 81 NUTS 3 regions and the time period 2012–2019 to analyse the geography of well-being for Turkey with panel and spatial panel models. Our results show that living in an urban area, in general, makes people happy, but that density negatively affects well-being. In addition, city size matters for enhancing well-being. We also analyse the determinants of well-being by using several socio-economic well-being indicators. Next, the aspatial and spatial model results based on spatial econometric regressions show that education, health, employment, and income are all important for well-being, whereas indirect effects (spillovers) of these indicators also exist. Our results indicate that ignoring spatial effects causes a misinterpretation of the effects of critical determinants of well-being in geography.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

For decades, the analysis of societal well-being and individual happiness has attracted a wealth of interest in economics, sociology, psychology, and geography, and in particular in welfare and utility theory. However, the empirical measurement of well-being and happiness has turned out to be quite a challenge. As a solution, most applied research resorted to GDP as a proxy measurement tool for national, group, or individual well-being or happiness. However, the limitations of this international comparative yardstick have been articulated in many publications, in particular in the context of ‘green GDP’ concepts in ecological economics. This has prompted the ‘beyond GDP’ orientation in environmental sciences, reflected, inter alia, in the nowadays popular Human Development Index (HDI) and other related welfare indicators (see, e.g., [1]). In parallel with amended welfare measurement systems, it is noteworthy that recently in quantitative geographic research, new approaches have been developed to measure individual and group happiness at a spatially disaggregated scale (e.g., cities or urban neighbourhoods) (see, e.g., [2,3,4,5,6,7]). Gradually a new research trajectory on the comparative study of well-being measurement is emerging. At present, the challenge of finding an operational measurement of well-being is attracting much attention from the side of both policymakers and the quantitative social science research community.
Two disciplines have formed the cradle of this new research orientation, viz. psychology and economics (see [8,9]). In psychology, subjective well-being is defined as ‘a person’s cognitive and affective evaluation of his or her life’ ([10], p. 63), and is often used interchangeably with happiness, life satisfaction, and quality of life. In the early psychological literature, a happy person is generally described as being well-paid, young, educated, religious, and married [11], and later on also as positive, optimistic, and socially confident, while possessing adequate resources [12]. From an economic perspective, Easterlin’s [13] study is one of the first attempts to use happiness data in economic research. The author states that happiness does not follow the same rising path as income does over time, which is called the Easterlin Paradox. In general, richer people report higher subjective well-being. However, the income-happiness relationship is not linear, and an additional income rise does not necessarily increase happiness. After the pioneering work of Easterlin, it was not until the late 1990s that economists started to link the psychological term ‘happiness’ to the economic literature by undertaking empirical studies to analyse the determinants of happiness on the national level [14,15].
Even though economic considerations are seen as essential to people’s happiness, they are not the only causes that affect it. Several decades ago already, Sen [16] argued that well-being should be investigated from a multidimensional perspective, instead of from a single macroeconomic indicator (e.g., GDP) that ignores other sources of heterogeneous distribution. In the report of the commission on ‘Beyond GDP: Measurement of Economic Performance and Social Progress’, Stiglitz et al. [17] emphasise the key dimensions that need to be considered simultaneously in order to analyse well-being instead of relying merely on GDP which is incapable of capturing all key aspects. According to Stiglitz et al. [17] increasing well-being is the main goal of economic and social development, and in order to assess well-being, both objective (economic and social conditions) and subjective (people’s perception) approaches are needed as neither of them alone is able to capture the information provided by the other approach. After that report, it could be observed that many countries/organisations attempted to measure well-being at the national level with a multidimensional approach, homing subjective measures, as a complement to objective indicators (e.g., OECD-How is life, 2017; Japan-Commission on Measuring Well-Being, 2010; UK-Measuring National Well-being programme, 2010). By aggregating different indicators and weighting them, a composite well-being index is created, and this index can be used to compare well-being levels across time and space (e.g., [18]. However, most national well-being index studies clearly neglect the geographical dimension of well-being at the sub-national (e.g., regional or urban) level [19]. This study aims to provide a new perspective on the geography of well-being: (1) by taking into account an urban–rural typology and city size elements in order to detect ‘Where are people happier?’; and, at the same time, (2) by investigating ‘What is affecting well-being?’ by considering spatial effects. Building on these motivations, we categorise the characteristics of a place by considering population (metropolitan city, medium-sized city, small-size city) and region typology (urban, rural). In addition, we investigate the determinants of well-being by including not only macroeconomic dimensions (GDP, employment) but also socio-economic elements (health, education, population density) with a special emphasis on spatial dependency.
Taking into account that Turkey is both one of the countries that rank at the bottom of well-being measures among OECD countries [20], and the large gap in well-being between its regions, it is remarkable to focus on the Turkish case. Considering the lack of studies that use spatial models which analyse spillover effects of well-being, the limited number of studies that use panel data, and the necessity of more frequent data at disaggregate scale as recently pointed out (e.g., [21,22,23,24]), this study differs from the existing literature in three ways. To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to take on the regional well-being concept with both its geography and the determinants at the same time. In this study, our goal is to reveal the spatial pattern of well-being, as well as explain indicators that affect well-being. Second, we use both aspatial and spatial models in order to compare and evaluate the spatial effects by adopting panel data. By employing spatial lag models, we analyse not only the direct effects of indicators but also the spatial spillover (indirect) effects on well-being. Lastly, in order to overcome the problem of a lack of annual well-being data at the regional level, we create a special proxy for well-being values that can be applied and replicated easily for any other country. Thus, it will be possible to observe the evolution in regional well-being over time and space without excessive efforts in order to raise happiness with public policy as suggested by Veenhoven [25].
The remaining part of the study is organised as follows. In Section 2, previous studies that investigate well-being are reviewed. Section 3 introduces the data, and Section 4 describes the empirical models applied to analyse well-being in Turkey. The findings of the models are reported in Section 5. Finally, Section 6 contains the main conclusion together with several policy recommendations that ensue from the estimated results from the previous section.

2. Literature Review

The disparity between the subnational (region, province, urban) and cross-national well-being values has opened a new discussion field which is called ‘the geography of well-being’. Considering the geographical clustering pattern of well-being, it is necessary to understand where people are happy. Urbanisation level and city size are the main factors that studies use to shed light on the where question. Living in big cities can be beneficial in terms of access to jobs, employment, and amenities. In particular, the New Economic Geography (NEG) addresses this question by assuming that larger cities create greater agglomeration economies and attract a more productive population thereby fostering their economic development [26]. There are many empirical studies that find a positive relationship between city size and well-being. Želinský et al. [27] provide evidence that a higher population density affects well-being positively in Slovakia by employing a representative survey with more than 10,000 observations. For the Turkish case, Çevik and Taşar [28] investigate national-level TUIK survey data on happiness and confirm that mean happiness is higher in urban areas than in rural areas. Using a meta-analysis, Melo et al. [29] also provide evidence of the positive link between city size and development.
However, there is another side of the coin: negative externalities. Bigger cities may harm their resident’s happiness due to problems of congestion, pollution, and the high cost of living. Therefore, the literature on city size and well-being is heterogeneous; so whereas Glaeser [30] claims people who live in cities are happier, Berry and Okulicz-Kozaryn [31] and Okulicz-Kozaryn and Valente [32] find there is a negative effect of urbanisation on well-being. Loschiavo [33] investigates the Italian case and provides evidence that people are less happy in the cities than in the non-urban areas and reveals the negative effect of urbanisation, when measured by the size of the city, on happiness (subjective well-being). Similarly, Lawless and Lucas [34] test for the size and density of the region and conclude that larger and denser regions report lower life satisfaction. Using regional data from 22 European countries in 2014, Weckroth and Kemppainen [35] find that subjective well-being is higher in rural areas than in urban areas, especially in more developed countries. This result from the European case is also confirmed by Morrison and Weckroth’s [36] study. According to their findings, living in metropolitan Finland (the Helsinki-Uusimaa region) negatively affects subjective well-being compared with living in the rest of the country. Zhang et al. [37] also reach evidence that urban residents’ well-being is higher than rural residents in Western China. In another study, Okulicz-Kozaryn and Mazelis [38] aim to understand the impacts of city size and population density on happiness for 232 U.S. counties. They argue that, even after controlling urban problems such as poverty, crime, low education, and ethnicity, people are not happier in large cities and dense areas: in other words, urbanism leads to unhappiness. Surprisingly, according to Florida et al. [39], density does not have a significant effect on happiness at the metropolitan level.
There are also differences in well-being in urban–rural areas depending on the level of development of the country (e.g., [40]). Although for wealthy countries the difference is low, for poor countries the difference is large [41,42]. More recently, Lenzi and Perucca [43] investigated the relationship between urbanization and subjective well-being to find out why large cities are sources of unhappiness. They suggest a threshold point for city population which turns from positive to negative when increased population causes more disadvantages than advantages for the city. They argue that living in the largest city in a country that exceeds the threshold point can lower subjective well-being, in line with [36], but this does not necessarily mean that urbanisation is the source of dissatisfaction per se, and nor do rural areas provide higher levels of well-being to their residents.
There is no consensus on the results from studies that also analyse the determinants of well-being. In contrast to Easterlin’s [13] assertion that money cannot buy happiness, Headey et al. [8] claim that money does matter to happiness when wealth, income, and consumption are included as independent variables. The main reason for expecting a positive association between well-being and income is that wealth can provide choices and opportunities to fulfil a person’s needs and desires, which can lead to higher satisfaction [44]. Yet, Diener et al. [45] find a curvilinear relationship between income and well-being, especially for poor countries at the national level. Analysing cross-national data from 1995, Inglehart and Klingemann [46] claim that economic development measured by GNP per capita significantly affects happiness measured by subjective well-being. They also find evidence that economic development is linked with both well-being and the level of democracy of a country. Ferrer-i-Carbonell [47] finds a weak but significant effect of income on individual well-being using the German Socio-Economic Panel from 1992 to 1997 with an ordered probit model. She also claims that studies that employ different types of data (e.g., time series, cross-sectional, or panel) tend to reach a different sign and significance level. For the Turkish case, Eren and Aşıcı [48] state that income and happiness are positively associated with cross-section data however any increase in income does not affect happiness over time. Another study by Eren and Aşıcı [49] also finds that the increase in GDP between 2004–2014 fails to enhance the domains that define the quality of life in Turkey.
Understanding geographical differences in well-being at the regional level, Rentfrow [50] states that wealth, occupation, education, physical health, tolerance, and personality indicators are related to the regional clustering of subjective well-being in the U.S. Lawless and Lucas [34] examine the correlation of income, employment, density, health, and education with well-being at the county level for the U.S. in order to determine the predictors of well-being for the policy-making process, based on the assumption that a regional variance in well-being exists. The results yield that the education indicator, measured by the percentage of high school and college graduates, has the strongest correlation with well-being among the predictors. Florida et al. [39] use the share of the population with a Bachelor’s degree as a human capital indicator and provide evidence of a highly significant and robust association between human capital and happiness, especially at the metropolitan level. Renfrow et al. [51] examine the happiness distribution in the U.S. and analyse the characteristics of happy places using the Gallup Organization’s Well-being Index. They find evidence of the positive effect of human capital and creative class on well-being. For the Turkish case, Çevik and Taşar [49] find a positive effect of education on happiness both with OLS and ordered logit models. Their findings of a positive effect of education on well-being at the regional level contradict the existing literature at the individual level which states that education has no significant or only a very weak effect on personal well-being. As expected, physical health is considered to be highly related to a person’s well-being. Especially unhealthy people with chronic diseases report a low level of well-being. Lawless and Lucas [34] find evidence of a higher, negative, and significant correlation between heart disease and obesity with well-being compared with all causes of mortality. So, taking into account the diverse results from many research studies, it is now necessary to take a deeper look into the well-being concept at the regional level from a broader angle, beginning with a discussion of the available data for the case of Turkey.

3. Data

As an emerging economy with a long history of socio-economic disparities among its regions, Turkey is one of the good examples which has succeeded in resolving the well-being phenomenon. In order to observe and understand the spatial pattern of well-being in a country, we need to measure well-being over time on the sub-national level. There are two official data sets provided by the Turkish Statistical Institute (TUIK) that measure well-being. The first is the Life Satisfaction Survey which is on the national level and has been carried out annually since 2003. Using a subjective approach, this survey simply asks: ‘taking all things together, how happy are you?’ in order to measure individuals’ happiness perceptions [52]. According to the survey results, the level of happiness has, in general, been decreasing during the last decade from 61% to 48%. Moreover, the results provide evidence in line with Blanchflower’s [15] U-shaped happiness–age curve findings, which emphasise the existence of a midlife nadir in subjective well-being [52]. This survey has been carried out at the NUTS 3 regions level for the first—and last—time in 2013 [53]. Life Satisfaction by Provinces reveals that Sinop province has the highest share (77%) of people who declared themselves happy compared with the national average of 59% [53].
The second official data set, the Well-Being Index for Provinces, aims to measure and compare the well-being of individuals by using both subjective and objective criteria [54]. As a composite index, the Well-Being Index for Provinces consists of 41 indicators related to 11 equally-weighted dimensions, based on the OECD Better Life framework and Turkey’s specific conditions on data availability. Life Satisfaction by Provinces is the main data source for the subjective indicators, whereas the objective indicators are gathered from various sources [54]. According to the Well-Being Index for Provinces, Isparta province has the highest index value of 0.674, whereas the national average is 0.525 [54]. Figure 1 shows the distribution of the well-being index values for the regions and reveals a tendency for low and high levels of well-being to be concentrated in particular regions. The Well-Being Index for Provinces (2015) study reveals that well-being is not evenly distributed over space and that there is a significant disparity in well-being among the Turkish regions (Figure 1). According to Figure 1, regions in the centre and the West have the highest well-being values, whereas regions in the Southeast possess the lowest level of well-being. The distribution of well-being in Figure 1 is highly similar to regional disparities in Turkey [55]. Clusters of low and high well-being values demonstrate the need for empirical evidence to examine spatial effects. Therefore, in order to obtain a better understanding of the spatial disparity of well-being, and to clarify where people are happy or unhappy, it is important to analyse the characteristics of regions.
In Figure 2, the association between GDP and subjective well-being (happiness) can be observed from the results of the Life Satisfaction Survey in Turkey and the change in GDP per capita levels over two decades. At the national level, happiness shows a similar pattern to that of the economic progress indicator; when happiness is high, GDP per capita is high, and when GDP per capita decreases happiness also decreases, except for the period of 2012–2015. However, this pattern is vague when the sub-national level is analysed. The results of both the Life Satisfaction by Provinces (2013) and the Well-Being Index for Provinces (2015) studies demonstrate that the spatial pattern of well-being does not overlap with the distribution of GDP per capita (Figure 1 and Figure 3). Both the well-being index and GDP per capita are the lowest in the southeastern part of the country, but the dispersion of higher levels of well-being and income differs significantly. According to Figure 3, the wealthiest regions are not the happiest regions in Turkey. This indicates that the macroeconomic dimension alone is inadequate to explain the region’s well-being in Turkey, and the determinants of well-being should be investigated from a broader approach.
The two existing official data sets have a problem that they lack the measurement of annual well-being on the regional level. To be able to monitor and compare the well-being of regions over time, we generated a new well-being indicator that originates from the Well-Being Index for Provinces (2015) study. We collected easily accessible, periodically updated, and well-being-related region-level data and ran a correlation analysis with the Well-Being Index for Provinces values. The highest correlation with the Well-Being Index was observed using the following demographic variables: median age; share of the population aged 0–4; and household size (Table 1). Considering the age–happiness relationship, it is not surprising to find that median age has the highest correlation with the Well-Being Index at 0.843.
Using the three correlated variables, we ran a principal component analysis (PCA) to generate a new variable, while reducing dimensionality and preserving variability in the data. Based on the PCA technique results, one component labelled PCA (Well-Being) explains 96.61% of the total variance (Table 2), and the correlation of the existing official Well-Being Index with the newly generated variable PCA (Well-Being) is 0.840 and is statistically significant.
After generating a new well-being indicator (PCA well-being), the following steps are run in order to provide not only a descriptive but also an explanatory analysis to observe and understand the spatial pattern of well-being. We adopt an urban–rural typology for Turkey to distinguish where people live based on the share of population criteria. Because urban–rural definitions were changed in 2012 by Law. No 6360 in Turkey, it is not possible to separate urban–rural populations in the 30 NUTS 3 regions. To overcome this problem, we employ the Eurostat urban–rural typology from 2013. Another important element to describe a region is to consider city size by examining the total population. As seen in Table 3, we include metropolitan, medium, and small-size regions. We also aim to explain the determinants of well-being and, following relevant arguments in the literature, we take into account both material well-being and socio-economic well-being indicators in the models. The effects of material well-being are addressed by using income and employment variables. As indicated in Section 2, having a job is considered an important determinant for enhancing both objective and subjective well-being. Therefore, we include GDP per capita and the employment share in the model. As socio-economic indicators, we use density, education, and health variables. The four main types of chronic diseases, also known as non-communicable diseases (NCD), are cardiovascular diseases, cancers, chronic respiratory diseases, and diabetes, and the deaths caused by these are included as an explanatory variable in the model in order to control for the effect of health problems on well-being. To understand the effect of education on well-being, we use the schooling ratio for secondary education in the regions. Lastly, we use gross population density per km2 as the core characteristic of cities to analyse urban agglomerations. We collect GDP per capita, schooling ratio, and health problems data from the TUIK, and employment data from the Social Security Institution from 2012 to 2019. Table 3 also provides descriptive information on each variable from 81 regions between 2012–2019.

4. Methodology

To test our hypotheses, we adopt both aspatial panel models and spatial panel models for T = 8 years and N = 81 NUTS 3 regions (provinces). As indicated in [56], panel data models offer several advantages over cross-sectional data by capturing heterogeneity across units (regions) and time (year). The panel model can be expressed using the following equation:
y i t = β 1 x i t + α i + c t + U i t ,   t = 1 , , T ,
where x′, i, t, U, α, and c represent a vector of independent variables, region, year, residual, individual effect, and year fixed-effects, respectively. The Typology (urban, rural) and City Size (metropolitan, medium-size, small-size city) elements are represented with a dummy variable; the urban–rural typology is time-invariant; and the Population density GDP per capita, Employment, Schooling ratio, and Health problems variables are measured in natural logarithms. We hypothesise that we will obtain positive effects from GDP per capita, employment ratio, schooling ratio, urban areas, and a negative effect from health problems, density, and rural areas on well-being value.
The second step is to include spatial interactions across regions and over time with spatial panel models. The benefit of bringing spatial effects into panel data is to be able to control for interdependence between regions [57]. Prior to applying spatial regression analyses, we need to calculate the spatial weight matrix (W), which is the simplest measure of spatial influence [58]. Based on the distance decay method, we generate the inverse distance spatial weight matrix for 81 regions with real distance (km) data, which is obtained from the General Directorate of Highways. The spatial weight matrix can be formulated as:
W i j = 1 d i j 2 ,
where d i j denotes distance between region i and region j, and W i j is the spatial interaction between region i and j. We employ the normalization procedure proposed by Elhorst [59] and Kelejian and Prucha [60] where each element W i j of the pre-normalized inverse distance matrix is divided by its largest characteristic root in order to have a valid economic interpretation of distance decay. Finally, we produce an 81 × 81 size non-negative spatial weight matrix with zeros in the diagonals. The spatial panel data model can be estimated with random (RE) or fixed effects (FE) model specifications, and the spatial Hausman test [61] helps researchers to decide which specification to select. A set of tests from Baltagi et al. [62] and Lagrange multiplier tests for spatial dependence are used for choosing the most appropriate specification along with the theoretical justification. As [63,64,65] state, well-being (happiness) is contagious, and a spatial effect is generally accounted for by considering a spatial lag model (SAR) specification. Thus, the spatial lag model (SAR) specification of the fixed effects model for Equation (1) can be expressed as:
y i t = ρ i j w i j y i j + β 1 x i t + α i + c t + U i t ,   t = 1 , , T
where ρ is the spatial autoregressive coefficient, and i j w i j y i j is the spatial lagged dependent variable; ε i t is the iid error term, and μ i represents a spatial specific effect. In the spatial model, we use density, GDP per capita, employment, education, and health indicators. To estimate the model, we employ maximum likelihood (ML) among the main approaches to estimate spatial models, instead of the GMM estimation which is hardly used in the literature [66], whereas OLS is inefficient and biased.

5. Empirical Results

The geography of well-being has been analysed in our study with panel data models; the results are shown in Table 4. Using Equation (1), we first run the pooled OLS model assuming no significant differences across regions. The Breusch–Pagan Lagrange multiplier (LM) test indicated a rejection of the null hypothesis (no panel effect), and thus we continued with panel data. According to the results of the random effects (Model 2) in Table 4, living in urban areas has a significant and positive effect on well-being, whereas living in rural areas has the opposite effect. For the city size elements, medium- and small-size cities positively affect well-being, whereas metropolitan areas have no statistically significant effect. Moreover, population density has a negative effect on well-being. Based on the Hausman test and the LM test results, we continue with the fixed effects model with time effects (Model 3). According to the results from the fixed-effects model, city sizes, female employment, and schooling ratio have a significant and positive effect on well-being. On the other hand, GDP per capita and health problems have no significant effect on well-being.
However, the findings from Table 4 are based on the assumption that regions are independent of each other, which means that the results contain no spatial interactions of the determinants of well-being. Hence, in the next step, we proceed with spatial models and exclude dummy and time-invariant variables. To justify adopting spatial models to test our hypothesis that well-being is contagious, it is necessary to investigate the existence of spatial autocorrelation in the data. Moran’s I test results display highly significant spatial autocorrelation with 0.843 for the year 2012 and 0.826 for the year 2019. We also run Local Moran’s I (LISA) proposed by [67] to detect the location of spatial clusters. The cluster maps for the dependent variable for 2012 and 2019 are presented in Figure 4. According to the LISA results, it is clear that the high level of well-being is clustered in the western and northern parts of the country, whereas a low level of well-being is highly clustered in the eastern regions.
In order to benefit from controlling both spatial and time-specific effects [66], we employ spatial panel models and explore spatial spillover effects. Recently, spatial panel models have been attracting a lot of attraction from researchers, but it is necessary to perform several preliminary specification procedures in order to select the best model [68]. To choose a model, first, we ran the spatial Hausman test, and the results indicated that the fixed-effects model is appropriate (Table 5). Afterwards, we used the LM tests and the robust LM tests for spatial error and spatial lag dependence in order to decide which model specification to employ. Both LM tests for lag and error dependence are significant, which provide evidence of spatial dependence in the data. To decide what type of spatial dependence will work, we looked at the robust LM test results, as indicated in [69]. According to the robust LM test, the SAR has a lower p-value, and thus the spatial lag fixed effects model performs better, which is also consistent with the previous spatial models in the literature. In Model 4, we ran fixed effects with all contagious variables while excluding dummies. Table 5 presents the results from the spatial lag fixed-effects model. The spatial autocorrelation coefficient (λ), which is the coefficient parameter of the spatial lag term of the dependent variable, is added to the model. The spatial autocorrelation coefficient (λ) is highly significant and positive, which indicates spatial spillover effects. The spatial model results show that employment, schooling ratio, health problems, and density all have statistically significant effects, whereas GDP per capita has an insignificant impact on well-being. These results mean that the explanatory variable in a particular region affects the well-being of that particular region, as well as the well-being of neighbouring regions. Unlike aspatial models and spatial error models, for the SAR, it is necessary to calculate the direct effects (on the particular region) and the indirect effects (on the neighbouring regions) of the explanatory variables on the outcome. Following LeSage et al. [70], we measure the direct and the indirect effects of the explanatory variables which are shown in the last three columns of Table 5. According to the results, it is possible to state that the spillover effects of employment, health, density, and education variables are highly significant and lower than the direct effects.

6. Discussion

The above-mentioned findings of our analysis of the two research questions provide fruitful elements for discussion. First, the results are highly significant and show that urban areas enhance well-being compared with rural areas, but well-being is also closely related to population density. A high density of population significantly decreases the well-being of a region. It is clear that even though urban areas appear to make people happy, living in a highly dense metropolitan city does not contribute to its residents’ well-being. Yet, urban areas which are not highly dense can still provide sufficient services and opportunities for their residents to be happy, which is in line with [43]. Moreover, the determinants of well-being analysis results state that any increase in education level and employment can enhance well-being as well as living in a small, medium, or metropolitan city, whereas GDP per capita and health problems have no significant effects. Second, we analysed those indicators that affect regional well-being with spatial panel models. This finding proves our hypothesis that regions are not independent, and spatial autocorrelation exists in the data. The model appeared to yield significant results on density, employment, education, and health indicators which means that improvements in these indicators cause an increase in regional well-being. It is also interesting to find that GDP per capita has no impact on regional well-being in contrast to the mainstream literature. However, as [48] pointed out, GDP per capita fails to explain well-being in the Turkish case, which leads to including non-material dimensions of well-being. In addition, spatial models also show that the indirect (spillover) effects of these predictors on well-being exist. According to the SAR, a region’s well-being is related to neighbouring regions’ well-being as well. This indicates that a change in the determinants of well-being in a region can cause a change in the well-being of the neighbouring regions too. This means that any improvement in employment, health, density, and education in one particular region will affect its neighbouring regions’ well-being as well. On the basis of the spatial model findings, we can say that the impacts of determinants on well-being are misinterpreted by aspatial models. These results provide evidence that ignoring spatial effects will cause the misapprehension of the effects of the determinants of well-being in the Turkish regions.

7. Conclusions

Several subnational studies on well-being demonstrate that well-being is not evenly distributed over space, and spatial clusters of high and low well-being are evident. This study investigates the geography of regional well-being with a spatial emphasis on Turkish NUTS 3 regions, characterized by different environmental and urbanisation conditions. In order to understand the structure and pattern of well-being, we analysed the relationship between urbanism and well-being by considering city size, density, and urban–rural typology. We use 81 NUTS 3 regions and the time period 2012–2019 to analyse the geography of well-being for Turkey with panel and spatial panel models. According to the results, living in an urban area, in general, makes people happy, but population density negatively affects well-being. Analysis of the determinants of well-being reveals that education, health, employment, and income are all affecting well-being not only in the region itself but also in neighbouring regions as well. Our results indicate that ignoring spatial effects causes a misinterpretation of the effects of critical determinants of well-being in geography. As one of the primary goals of socio-economic development, enhancing well-being is on the agenda not only of researchers but also of policymakers. Therefore, it is crucial to measure, compare, and evaluate regional well-being periodically in order to gain a better understanding of the spatial pattern of well-being and its determinants and implement efficient, suitable policies for regions. Future studies might investigate the evaluation of the well-being of regions from a broader time span by considering their own characteristics of the regions, such as spatial and environmental, particularly.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Z.E., K.K. and P.N.; Methodology, Z.E., K.K. and P.N.; Data curation, Z.E.; Writing—original draft, Z.E., K.K. and P.N.; Supervision, K.K. and P.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Karima Kourtit and Peter Nijkamp acknowledge the grant from the Axel och Margaret Ax:son Johnsons Stiftelse, Sweden. Karima Kourtit and Peter Nijkamp also acknowledge the grant from the Romanian Ministry of Research and Innovation, CNCS-UEFISCDI, project number PN-III-P4-ID-PCCF-2016-0166, within the PNCDI III” project ReGrowEU—Advancing ground-breaking research in regional growth and development theories, through a resilience approach: towards a convergent, balanced and sustainable European Union (Iasi, Romania).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kalimeris, P.; Bithas, K.; Richardson, C.; Nijkamp, P. Hidden Linkages between Resources and Economy: A “Beyond-GDP” Approach Using Alternative Welfare Indicators. Ecol. Econ. 2020, 169, 106508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Levy, C. Gender and the environment: The challenge of cross-cutting issues in development policy and planning. Environ. Urban. 1992, 4, 134–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ballas, D. The Economic Geography of Happiness. In Handbook of Labor, Human Resources and Population Economics; Zimmermann, K.F., Ed.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2021; pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kourtit, K.; Nijkamp, P.; Östh, J. My home is my castle—Assessment of city love in Sweden. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 2020, 58, 102213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dang, Y.; Chen, L.; Zhang, W.; Zheng, D.; Zhan, D. How does growing city size affect residents’ happiness in urban China? A case study of the Bohai rim area. Habitat Int. 2020, 97, 102120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Wang, D.; Schwanen, T.; Mao, Z. Does exposure to richer and poorer neighborhoods influence wellbeing? Cities 2019, 95, 102408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Morrison, P.S. Whose Happiness in Which Cities? A Quantile Approach. Sustainability 2021, 13, 11290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Headey, B.; Muffels, R.; Wooden, M. Money Does not Buy Happiness: Or Does It? A Reassessment Based on the Combined Effects of Wealth, Income and Consumption. Soc. Indic. Res. 2008, 87, 65–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Frey, B.S. Economics of Happiness; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  10. Diener, E.; Oishi, S.; Lucas, R.E. Subjective Well-Being: The Science of Happiness and Life Satisfaction. In Handbook of Positive Psychology; Snyder, C., Lopez, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 463–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wilson, W.R. Correlates of avowed happiness. Psychol. Bull. 1967, 67, 294–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Diener, E.; Suh, E.M.; Lucas, R.E.; Smith, H.L. Subjective Well-Being: Three Decades of Progress. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 125, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Easterlin, R.A. Does Economic Growth Improve the Human Lot? Some Empirical Evidence. In Nations and Households in Economic Growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramovitz; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1974; pp. 89–125. [Google Scholar]
  14. Frey, B.S.; Stutzer, A. What can Economists Learn from Happiness Research? J. Econ. Lit. 2002, 40, 402–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Blanchflower, D.G. Is happiness U-shaped everywhere? Age and subjective well-being in 145 countries. J. Popul. Econ. 2021, 34, 575–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sen, A.K. Commodities and Capabilities; North-Holland: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  17. Stiglitz, J.E.; Sen, A.; Fitoussi, J.P. The Measurement of Economic Performance and Social Progress Revisited: Reflections and Overview; Sciences Po Publications 2009-33: Paris, France, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  18. Arcidiacono, A.; Torrisi, G. Decentralisation and Resilience: A Multidimensional Approach. Sustainability 2022, 14, 9919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Calcagnini, G.; Perugini, F. A Well-Being Indicator for the Italian Provinces. Soc. Indic. Res. 2019, 142, 149–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. OECD. How’s Life? 2015: Measuring Well-Being; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  21. Fadda, G.; Jirón, P. Quality of Life and Gender: A Methodology for Urban Research. Environ. Urban. 1999, 11, 261–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Dardha, E.; Rogge, N. How’s Life in Your Region? Measuring Regional Material Living Conditions, Quality of Life and Subjective Well-Being in OECD Countries Using a Robust, Conditional Benefit-of-the-Doubt Model. Soc. Indic. Res. 2020, 151, 1015–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Samuel, R.; Hadjar, A. How Welfare-State Regimes Shape Subjective Well-Being Across Europe. Soc. Indic. Res. 2015, 129, 565–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Facchinetti, S.; Siletti, E. Well-being Indicators: A Review and Comparison in the Context of Italy. Soc. Indic. Res. 2021, 159, 523–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Veenhoven, R. Greater Happiness for a Greater Number Is that Possible and Desirable? J. Happiness Stud. 2010, 11, 605–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Duranton, G. Viewpoint: From cities to productivity and growth in developing countries. Wiley Online Libr. 2008, 41, 689–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Želinský, T.; Hudec, O.; Mojsejová, A.; Hricová, S. The effects of population density on subjective well-being: A case-study of Slovakia. Socio-Economic Plan. Sci. 2021, 78, 101061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Çevik, S.; Taşar, M.O. Urban-rural Differences in Subjective Well-Being: Turkey. Ann. Wars. Univ. Life Sci.-SGGW Hortic. Landsc. Archit. 2016, 37, 23–39. [Google Scholar]
  29. Melo, P.C.; Graham, D.J.; Noland, R.B. A meta-analysis of estimates of urban agglomeration economies. Reg. Sci. Urban Econ. 2009, 39, 332–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Glaeser, E.L. Triumph of the City: How Our Greatest Invention Makes Us Richer, Smarter, Greener, Healthier, and Happier; Macmillan: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  31. Berry, B.J.L.; Okulicz-Kozaryn, A. An Urban-Rural Happiness Gradient. Urban Geogr. 2011, 32, 871–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Okulicz-Kozaryn, A.; Valente, R.R. No urban malaise for Millennials. Reg. Stud. 2019, 53, 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Loschiavo, D. Big-city life (dis)satisfaction? The effect of urban living on subjective well-being. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2021, 192, 740–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Lawless, N.M.; Lucas, R. Predictors of Regional Well-Being: A County Level Analysis. Soc. Indic. Res. 2010, 101, 341–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Weckroth, M.; Kemppainen, T. (Un)Happiness, where are you? Evaluating the relationship between urbanity, life satisfaction and economic development in a regional context. Reg. Stud. Reg. Sci. 2021, 8, 207–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Morrison, P.S.; Weckroth, M. Human values, subjective well-being and the metropolitan region. Reg. Stud. 2017, 52, 325–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhang, J.; Xie, J.; Zhang, X.; Yang, J. Income, Social Capital, and Subjective Well-Being of Residents in Western China. Sustainability 2022, 14, 9141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Okulicz-Kozaryn, A.; Mazelis, J.M. Urbanism and Happiness: A Test of Wirth’s Theory of Urban Life. Urban Stud. 2018, 55, 349–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Florida, R.; Mellander, C.; Rentfrow, P.J. The Happiness of Cities. Reg. Stud. 2013, 47, 613–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sørensen, J.F. The rural happiness paradox in developed countries. Soc. Sci. Res. 2021, 98, 102581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Walker, J.; Frediani, A.A.; Trani, J.-F. Gender, difference and urban change: Implications for the promotion of well-being? Environ. Urban. 2012, 25, 111–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Sorensen, J.F.L. Rural–Urban Differences in Life Satisfaction: Evidence from the European Union. Reg. Stud. 2013, 48, 1451–1466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lenzi, C.; Perucca, G. Urbanization and Subjective Well-Being. In Regeneration of the Built Environment from a Circular Economy Perspective; Della Torre, S., Cattaneo, S., Lenzi, C., Zanelli, A., Eds.; Research for Development; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 21–28. [Google Scholar]
  44. Diener, E.; Lucas, R.E.; Schimmack, U.; Helliwell, J.F. Well-Being for Public Policy; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  45. Diener, E.; Diener, M.; Diener, C. Factors Predicting the Subjective Wellbeing of Nations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 851–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Inglehart, R.; Klingemann, H.D. Genes, Culture, Democracy and Happiness. In Culture and Subjective Well-Being; Diener, E., Suh, E., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 165–183. [Google Scholar]
  47. Ferrer-I-Carbonell, A. Income and well-being: An empirical analysis of the comparison income effect. J. Public Econ. 2005, 89, 997–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Eren, K.A.; Aşıcı, A.A. Subjective Well-Being in an Era of Relentless Growth: The Case of Turkey Between 2004 and 2014. J. Happiness Stud. 2017, 19, 1347–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Eren, K.A.; Aşıcı, A.A. The Determinants of Happiness in Turkey: Evidence from City-Level Data. J. Happiness Stud. 2016, 18, 647–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Rentfrow, P.J. Geographical Variation in Subjective Well-Being. In Handbook of Well-Being; Diener, E., Oishi, S., Tay, L., Eds.; DEF Publishers: Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  51. Rentfrow, P.J.; Mellander, C.; Florida, R. Happy States of America: A state-level analysis of psychological, economic, and social well-being. J. Res. Pers. 2009, 43, 1073–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. TUIK. Life Satisfaction Survey. 2020. Available online: https://data.tuik.gov.tr/Bulten/Index?p=Life-Satisfaction-Survey-2020-37209&dil=2 (accessed on 18 February 2021).
  53. TUIK. Life Satisfaction by Provinces. 2013. Available online: https://data.tuik.gov.tr/Bulten/Index?p=Il-Duzeyinde-Yasam-Memnuniyeti-2013-18507 (accessed on 18 February 2021).
  54. TUIK. Well-Being Index for Provinces. 2015. Available online: https://data.tuik.gov.tr/Bulten/Index?p=Illerde-Yasam-Endeksi-2015-24561 (accessed on 18 February 2021).
  55. Karahasan, B.C.; Pinar, M.; Deniz, P. Political climate and regional well-being in Turkey. Territ. Politi.-Gov. 2021, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hsiao, C. Panel Data Analysis–Advantages and Challenges. Test 2007, 16, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Bouayad, A.S.; Le Gallo, J.; Védrine, L. Spatial Econometrics on Panel Data. In Handbook of Spatial Analysis; de Bellefon, M.-P., Loonis, V., Eds.; INSEE Methods: Montrouge, France, 2018; Volume 131, pp. 185–212. [Google Scholar]
  58. Bavaud, F. Models for Spatial Weights: A Systematic Look. Geogr. Anal. 1998, 30, 153–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Elhorst, J.P. Dynamic Models in Space and Time. Geogr. Anal. 2001, 33, 119–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Kelejian, H.H.; Prucha, I.R. Specification and estimation of spatial autoregressive models with autoregressive and heteroskedastic disturbances. J. Econ. 2010, 157, 53–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Mutl, J.; Pfaffermayr, M. The Hausman test in a Cliff and Ord panel model. Econ. J. 2011, 14, 48–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Baltagi, B.H.; Song, S.H.; Koh, W. Testing panel data regression models with spatial error correlation. J. Econ. 2003, 117, 123–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ergüt, Ö.; Çilingirtürk, A.M. Türkiye’nin Yaşam Memnuniyetine Mekansal Etkinin Analizi. J. Life Econ. 2022, 9, 65–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Lin, C.-H.A.; Lahiri, S.; Hsu, C.-P. Happiness and Regional Segmentation: Does Space Matter? J. Happiness Stud. 2013, 15, 57–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lin, C.-H.A.; Lahiri, S.; Hsu, C.-P. Happiness and Globalization: A Spatial Econometric Approach. J. Happiness Stud. 2016, 18, 1841–1857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Elhorst, J.P. Spatial Panel Data Models. In Handbook of Applied Spatial Analysis; Fischer, M.M., Getis, A., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany; New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 377–407. [Google Scholar]
  67. Anselin, L. The Local Indicators of Spatial Association—LISA. Geogr. Anal. 1995, 27, 93–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kopczewska, K.; Kudła, J.; Walczyk, K. Strategy of Spatial Panel Estimation: Spatial Spillovers Between Taxation and Economic Growth. Appl. Spat. Anal. Policy 2015, 10, 77–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Anselin, L. Spatial Econometrics: Methods and Models; Kluwer Academic: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  70. LeSage, J.; Banerjee, S.; Fischer, M.M.; Congdon, P. Spatial Statistics: Methods, Models & Computation. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 2009, 53, 2781–2785. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Distribution of well-being and GDP per capita (Source: TUIK, 2016).
Figure 1. Distribution of well-being and GDP per capita (Source: TUIK, 2016).
Sustainability 14 16370 g001
Figure 2. Happiness and GDP per capita at the national level (source: TUIK—Life Satisfaction Survey, 2021).
Figure 2. Happiness and GDP per capita at the national level (source: TUIK—Life Satisfaction Survey, 2021).
Sustainability 14 16370 g002
Figure 3. Happiness and GDP per capita at the regional level in 2013 (source: TUIK—Life Satisfaction Survey, 2014).
Figure 3. Happiness and GDP per capita at the regional level in 2013 (source: TUIK—Life Satisfaction Survey, 2014).
Sustainability 14 16370 g003
Figure 4. LISA cluster map of the dependent variable (PCA well-being) for 2012 and 2019.
Figure 4. LISA cluster map of the dependent variable (PCA well-being) for 2012 and 2019.
Sustainability 14 16370 g004aSustainability 14 16370 g004b
Table 1. Pearson correlation matrix of indicators.
Table 1. Pearson correlation matrix of indicators.
 Well-Being IndexMedian AgeHousehold SizePopulation 0–4
Well-Being Index1   
Median age0.843 ***1  
Household size−0.836 ***−0.962 ***1 
Population aged 0–4−0.802 ***−0.966 ***0.933 ***1
Note: *** statistical significance at the 1% level.
Table 2. PCA results for well-being.
Table 2. PCA results for well-being.
Component MatrixComponent% of VarianceCumulative %Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin MeasureBartlett’s Test of Sphericity
Median age0.991PCA (Well-being)96.61196.6110.740 ***0.000
Household size0.979
Population aged 0–40.978
Note: *** statistical significance at the 1% level.
Table 3. Descriptive analysis of variables.
Table 3. Descriptive analysis of variables.
VariableDescriptionSourceTime PeriodNMinMaxMean
PCA (Well-Being)New well-being value for regionsAuthors’ calculation2012–2019648−2.851.340.00
Typology (Urban)80% of the population lives in urban clustersEurostat2013810.001.000.0671
Typology (Rural)At least 50% of the population lives in rural grid cellsEurostat2013810.001.000.6049
City Size (Metropolitan)Total population > 2,500,000TUIK2012–20196480.001.000.1481
City Size (Medium)250,000 < Total population < 500,000TUIK2012–20196480.001.000.358
City Size (Small) Total population < 250,000TUIK2012–20196480.001.000.321
Density (Population density) Gross population density per km2TUIK2012–2019648112987125.699
GDP per capitaGross domestic product per capita ($)TUIK2012–20196482946207268301.27
EmploymentShare of registered female employmentSocial Security Institution2012–201964811.3735.8524.31
Schooling ratioSchooling ratio for secondary education TUIK2012–201964835.4610080.89
Health problemsShare of deaths caused by cancer, heart disease, obesity, respiratory problemsTUIK2012–201964844.7886.3775.12
Table 4. The estimation results of the panel models.
Table 4. The estimation results of the panel models.
 Model 1
(Pool)
Model 2
(Random Effects)
Model 3
(Fixed Effects)
GDP per capita0.450 **0.319 ***0.019
 (0.194)(0.057)(0.076)
employment1.664 ***0.520 ***0.307 ***
 (0.280)(0.062)(0.072)
schooling 2.432 ***0.469 ***0.404 ***
 (0.367)(0.060)(0.078)
health problems2.886 ***−0.070−0.171
 (0.714)(0.077)(0.131)
density −0.310 ***−0.596 ***−0.920 ***
 (0.077)(0.068)(0.168)
typology (urban)0.2320.836 ***-
 (0.217)(0.224) 
typology (rural)0.025−0.595 ***-
 (0.098)(0.120) 
city size (metropolitan)−0.2050.0430.071 ***
 (0.195)(0.061)(0.014)
city size (medium)0.00030.068 ***0.069 ***
 (0.074)(0.018)(0.025)
city size (small) −0.0050.055 *0.050 ***
 (0.119)(0.032)(0.013)
constant−30.765 ***−3.522 ***-
 −2.493(0.761) 
observations648648648
R20.8850.8090.464
adjusted R20.8820.8040.371
log-likelihood−2179421160
AIC470−1848−2128
Note: *, ** and *** statistical significance at the 10%, 5%, and 1% levels, respectively. N = 648 for all models. Region and time-fixed effects are included. Dependent variable = PCA (Well-Being).
Table 5. The estimation results of the spatial panel models.
Table 5. The estimation results of the spatial panel models.
 Model 4 (Fixed Effects)Model 5
SAR-FE
Impacts
(Direct)
Impacts
(Indirect)
Impacts
(Total)
GDP per capita0.0240.0540.0540.0100.064
(0.081)(0.038)(0.036)(0.007)(0.043)
employment 0.333 ***0.225 ***0.226 ***0.041 ***0.267 ***
(0.073)(0.039)(0.041)(0.007)(0.049)
schooling 0.369 ***0.300 ***0.296 ***0.054 ***0.351 ***
(0.078)(0.038)(0.039)(0.007)(0.046)
health problems −0.163−0.146 ***−0.147 ***−0.027 **−0.174 ***
(0.134)(0.048)(0.047)(0.009)(0.056)
density −0.958 ***−0.834 ***−0.836 ***−0.154 ***−0.989 ***
(0.167)(0.064)(0.065)(0.013)(0.077)
Λ -0.157 ***   
 (0.005)   
Note: ** and *** statistical significance at the 5%, and 1% levels, respectively. N = 648, R2 = 0.434, log-likelihood = 1162, AIC = −2311, LM-lag p-value = 0.000, LM-error p-value = 0.000, RLM-lag p-value = 0.000, RML-error p-value = 0.031, Hausman test for spatial models p-value = 0.000, Baltagi, Song, and Koh LM*-lambda conditional LM test = 0.000, dependent variable = PCA (well-being), spatial weight matrix = inverse distance (queen contiguity-based spatial weight is used for robustness check. The results are available upon request).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Elburz, Z.; Kourtit, K.; Nijkamp, P. Well-Being and Geography: Modelling Differences in Regional Well-Being Profiles in Case of Spatial Dependence—Evidence from Turkey. Sustainability 2022, 14, 16370. https://doi.org/10.3390/su142416370

AMA Style

Elburz Z, Kourtit K, Nijkamp P. Well-Being and Geography: Modelling Differences in Regional Well-Being Profiles in Case of Spatial Dependence—Evidence from Turkey. Sustainability. 2022; 14(24):16370. https://doi.org/10.3390/su142416370

Chicago/Turabian Style

Elburz, Zeynep, Karima Kourtit, and Peter Nijkamp. 2022. "Well-Being and Geography: Modelling Differences in Regional Well-Being Profiles in Case of Spatial Dependence—Evidence from Turkey" Sustainability 14, no. 24: 16370. https://doi.org/10.3390/su142416370

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop