1. Introduction
Information boundaries assume a crucial role in effectively conveying product information and exert a significant influence on consumer judgment. Additionally, they serve as essential conduits through which companies can disseminate health and nutrition-related information pertaining to food products to their consumers. Within our daily lives, encounters with information boundaries associated with food are a common occurrence (“boundaries”). Notably, certain brands initially adopted an unbounded approach to their information design but later strategically introduced boundaries. For example, Nestle’s organic milk presents information encompassed by boundaries. Intriguingly, there are instances where brands initially presented their information without boundaries but subsequently incorporated them, such as Abbott’s organic pure milk in Denmark and Jason’s rye bread in Germany. This study endeavors to address the question of whether boundaries in organic food information also exert an influence on consumers’ purchase intention, aiming to provide comprehensive insights into this inquiry.
The existing body of research related to organic food information can be classified into two main categories: verbal indicators (such as product descriptions and ingredient details) and non-verbal indicators (which include colors, shapes, placements, visuals, and textual elements) [
1]. Regarding verbal cues, governmental regulations are in place to govern food labeling, with a specific focus on enhancing human health and safety while simultaneously addressing environmental risks, particularly within the context of organic food [
2]. Organic food exhibits distinct characteristics, including the purity of origin, the utilization of unique processing and production methods, as well as an abundance of specific ingredients or raw materials [
3]. Functioning as an external cue, organic food information partially unveils the intrinsic quality attributes of the food and its associated production processes [
4]. This aspect plays a critical role in fostering consumer trust and serves as an essential foundation for the evaluation of organic food. Non-verbal cues encompass various visual perceptual attributes such as color, shape, position, text, and pictures. Diverse visual presentations of these cues elicit a range of visual experiences and psychological perceptions among consumers.
Relative to verbal cues, the existing body of research has primarily focused on non-verbal cues, specifically the background color, shape, and display position of food information. Regarding information color, Baklova and Van Tripp (2010) conducted a study that revealed the significant impact of a red background in food information on increasing consumers’ purchase intentions, particularly concerning specific nutritional attributes [
5]. This effect can be attributed to the heightened attention-capturing ability of the red background. Shen et al. (2017) proposed that red boundaries are more effective than blue boundaries in enhancing the utility of food information [
6]. As a result, consumers exhibit longer reaction times during the decision-making process, leading to greater consideration of attributes and sustained attention when confronted with tasks involving multiple choices. With respect to information shape, even subtle and minimal variations in the shape characteristics of food information as an external feature can exert a surprisingly profound influence on consumers. For example, Zhong ke et al. (2019) conducted a study demonstrating that long food information shapes, as opposed to squares, influence consumers’ perception of products being usable for an extended period [
7]. This finding suggests that the aspect ratio of food information shapes can elongate consumers’ perception of product time attributes, such as battery life. In terms of information location design, Wen and Lurie (2019) found that boundaries play a role in how individuals process information when presented with multiple products and their associated information [
8]. Specifically, horizontal boundaries used to segregate product options result in an “option-option” processing order, while vertical boundaries used to segregate product attributes lead to an “attribute-attribute” processing order. The latter case enhances attribute comparisons, highlighting distinctions between options and intensifying individuals’ perceptions of diversity. This effect is particularly pronounced under high cognitive load conditions. Jing et al. (2019) identified information boundaries that influence consumers’ purchase decisions [
4]. Furthermore, several studies have indicated that food information boundaries can impact consumers’ food purchase decisions [
9]. Previous studies have thoroughly investigated multiple facets of food information, such as background color, shape, and display location, while relatively little focus has been placed on the limits of food information, particularly regarding organic food. This study aims to examine the impact of the limits of information regarding organic food on consumers’ buying intentions, addressing a gap in the existing literature related to the constraints of organic food information.
Organic food is a type of purely natural ecological food that is produced using environmentally sound methods throughout its entire lifecycle, including production, processing, storage, and transportation [
10]. It is free from modern synthetic inputs like pesticides, genetically modified organisms, and chemical food additives. Certified by relevant authorities, organic food is widely acknowledged as a high-quality and safe health food option [
11]. Organic food is characterized by its purity of origin, unique processing and production methods, and the abundance of specific special ingredients or raw materials [
12]. These distinct attributes are communicated to consumers through the presentation of food information, which acts as an external cue influencing consumers’ purchasing behavior [
13]. By applying the concepts of symbolic cognition theory, we propose that the presence of informational boundaries in organic food significantly influences consumers’ intentions to purchase. In particular, incorporating boundaries in organic food (as opposed to those without boundaries) bolsters consumers’ purchasing intentions (Experiment 1), a phenomenon that is mediated by perceived health benefits (Experiment 2). Additionally, our analysis reveals that cues associated with the architecture of credence labels influence how organic food information boundaries affect purchasing intentions. Specifically, when considering ingredient-level cues tied to the credence-label framework, consumers exposed to boundaries in organic food (compared to those without boundaries) tend to perceive higher levels of healthiness, which in turn, results in more robust purchase intentions. Conversely, for product-level cues within the credence-label structure, the absence of boundaries in organic food (vs. without boundaries) enhances consumer perceptions of healthiness and, thus, boosts purchase intentions (Experiment 3).
This study contributes in three ways. Firstly, it expands the existing research on the boundaries of organic food information. Although earlier research has mainly concentrated on aspects like background color, shape, and position concerning organic food information, there has been less emphasis on the limits of organic food information itself. This paper explores the deficiency in existing research by analyzing how organic food information boundaries influence consumers’ intentions to buy. Additionally, this investigation deepens our comprehension of the intermediary role played by the perceptions of health benefits. The results indicate that the way healthiness is perceived greatly influences how organic food information boundaries affect buying intentions. Ultimately, this research broadens the exploration of signals related to the credence-label framework specifically in relation to organic food. The findings indicate that the relationship between organic food and the components of the credence-label framework influences the ways in which consumers interpret food information and assess its health benefits. Furthermore, this study emphasizes how the informational limits surrounding organic food affect consumers’ intentions to buy, thus enhancing the empirical understanding of food information in the field of consumer research. The outcomes presented in this paper provide valuable insights for effective management strategies in marketing.
3. Experiment 1: Influence of Organic Food Information Boundaries on Purchase Intention
3.1. Purpose of the Experiment
The main aim of Experiment 1 was to assess the research hypotheses H1 and H2, particularly focusing on the impact of the presence or absence of organic food information boundaries on purchase intention (H1) and the mediating role of perceived healthiness (H2). To enhance the external validity of the findings, we chose the authentic brand “SATINE” organic milk as the stimulus and analyzed the food information related to this same product, including its production location, storage requirements, shelf life, and other relevant details.
3.2. Experimental Design and Subjects for Study 1
The first experiment utilized a one-way between-group design to investigate how the presence of organic food information (yes vs. no) influenced purchase intentions. The organic food information condition was identified as the independent variable in this study, whereas the dependent variable concentrated on the intention to purchase. Additionally, perceived healthiness functioned as the mediating variable.
To minimize the potential biases caused by individual characteristics, we implemented a controlled experimental design. Participants were recruited to ensure a balanced distribution across age, gender, and consumer habits. A total of 166 participants were recruited through a reputable online survey platform in China. The average age of the participants was 26.49 years (SD = 5.316), with 86 females representing 51.80% of the total sample. Prior to the main experiment, a screening questionnaire was used to identify and exclude individuals with extreme or unrepresentative consumer habits. Additionally, participants were randomly assigned to different experimental groups, thereby enhancing the internal validity and reliability of the results. The entire study was conducted online, and each participant received between CNY 5 and 10 in cash upon completing all questions.
3.3. Experimental Procedure for Study 1
This experiment used the real brand “SATINE” (famous brand in China) organic milk as the stimulus. The experiment was randomly assigned into 2 experimental groups: the bounded group and the unbounded group.
The initial segment involved a reading exercise. This exercise aimed to alter consumers’ perceptions regarding “SATINE” organic milk as a type of organic food. The researcher started by giving the participants a concise overview of “SATINE” organic milk. This product is enriched with 19 varieties of natural nutrients and boasts over 3.5% of premium milk protein for every 100 g, surpassing the national standard by 20%. The entirety of the production zone is regulated, featuring an exclusive seventh organic farm dedicated solely to supplying high-quality milk for the creation of Golden Classic products. The entire production process is meticulously overseen to maintain the quality of Golden Classic products from the very beginning, supported by an exemplary quality inspection system. This inspection system ensures the safety and integrity of organic milk from the farm all the way to the final product. Following the reading, participants were requested to respond to questions pertaining to the manipulation test.
The second section involved a task for evaluating food. Initially, participants were informed that the YIlI manufacturer intended to introduce an organic milk line and sought to gauge consumer opinions prior to its official release. Subsequently, they were shown an image representing the product details of the organic milk, which was described as a screenshot taken from the packaging. Participants in the no-border condition viewed the product information image without any frames (
Figure A1a without boundaries), whereas those in the border condition viewed the image enclosed within boundaries (
Figure A1b boundaries). Both groups were exposed to identical content regarding the product information. This information comprised the product’s name (organic milk), its shelf life (6 months), the recommended storage conditions (between 2 °C and 6 °C, in an airtight container), and the production date. Questions were then answered after the subjects had read the stimulus material.
3.4. Measurement Scale
The variables that were manipulated primarily involved the perception of “SATINE” organic milk as an organic product [
82]. The inquiry made was as follows: “Do you view ‘SATINE’ organic milk as organic food?” Participants provided their answers using a 7-point scale, with 1 indicating strong disagreement and 7 signifying strong agreement. The mediating variables were assessed using a perceived healthiness scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.933), which included 5 items across 7 subscales, rated from 1 (disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The statements included the following: “I believe this organic milk is healthy”; “I find this organic milk to be environmentally friendly”; “I view this organic milk as nutritious”; “I consider this organic milk to be low-fat”; “I think this organic milk benefits my body”. The assessment of the dependent variable was conducted concerning the organic food purchase intention scale (Cronbach α = 0.908), which was created by Schifferstein et al. [
83] and Grunter et al. [
84]. The measurement included three statements (encompassing seven subscales, where 1 represents disagreement and 7 indicates strong agreement): “I might think about purchasing this organic plain milk; it is very probable that I would buy this organic plain milk; I would choose to purchase this organic plain milk”.
The scales selected for this study were chosen based on their demonstrated reliability and validity in prior research. The perceived healthiness scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.933) was selected because it has been widely validated and comprehensively assesses various dimensions of health perception, including nutritional value, environmental friendliness, and overall health benefits. The purchase intention scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.908) was chosen for its reliability and effectiveness in measuring consumers’ willingness to purchase organic products.
Control variables encompassed brand familiarity, product preference, level of hunger, and weight loss goals. The selection of these variables was informed by prior research, which identified them as potential confounders that could influence consumers’ purchase intentions and perceptions of healthiness. By controlling for these variables, we were able to more accurately assess the impact of information boundaries on purchase intentions, thereby mitigating the interference of these potential confounders and enhancing the robustness and credibility of our findings. Additionally, demographic information, including participants’ age and gender, was documented.
3.5. Experimental Results for Study 1
3.5.1. Manipulation Test for Study 1
The findings from the one-way ANOVA indicated that both groups rated “SATINE” organic pure milk as organic food similarly (M with boundaries = 6.21, SD = 1.195; M without boundaries = 6.13, SD = 1.189; F(1, 198) = 1.321, p > 0.05), showing no significant difference. However, a notable difference was observed concerning the information boundary size perceived by the two groups of participants (M with boundaries = 4.86, SD = 1.401; M without boundaries = 4.31, SD = 1.623; F(1, 198) = 2.286, p > 0.05), product familiarity (M without boundaries = 3.79, SD = 2.107; M with boundaries = 4.07, SD = 1.845; F(1, 198) = 1.522, p > 0.05), and product preference (M without boundaries = 4.73, SD = 1.487; M with boundaries = 4.91, SD = 1.277; F(1, 198) = 1.298, p > 0.05). No significant difference was found regarding whether participants from the two groups had a clear weight loss objective (M without boundaries = 4.11, SD = 1.956; M with boundaries = 4.33, SD = 1.943; F(1, 198) = 0.988, p > 0.05). These results suggest that the experimental manipulation of information boundaries in organic food is effective.
3.5.2. Purchase Intention for Study 1
A variance analysis (ANOVA) was performed using the information boundary as the independent variable and the intention to purchase as the dependent variable. As shown in
Figure 2, the results indicate that the information boundary related to organic food had a significant impact on purchase intentions. Based on the results from the one-way ANOVA, participants in the boundary group (M with boundaries = 5.021, SD = 1.231) exhibited a greater intention to purchase organic plain milk compared to those in the group without boundaries (M without boundaries = 4.282, SD = 0.951) [F (1, 207) = 5.187,
p = 0.024]. This supports H1.
To control for potential confounding effects of gender and age, a further analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted, including gender and age as covariates. The independent variable remained the information boundary, and the dependent variable was the intention to purchase organic plain milk. The results showed that, after adjusting for gender and age, the information boundary still had a significant impact on purchase intention (p < 0.05). This result suggests that the effect of the information boundary on purchase intention remained robust despite the potential influence of these demographic variables. The covariate analysis also revealed that neither gender [F (1, 205) = 0.421, p = 0.517] nor age [F (1, 205) = 1.142, p = 0.286] had a significant direct effect on purchase intention. These findings further support H1, indicating that the presence of information boundaries significantly influences consumers’ purchase intentions, independent of gender and age differences.
3.5.3. Perceived Healthiness for Study 1
The results of the ANOVA indicated that participants in the bounded group (M = 4.839, SD = 1.022) considered organic pure milk to be healthier compared to those in the unbounded group (M = 4.193, SD = 1.879) [F(1, 148) = 4.717, p = 0.024]. This suggests that the perceived health benefits of organic food positively influence intentions to purchase such products.
3.5.4. Mediating Role of Perceived Healthiness of Food in Study 1
The method of Bootstrapping described in the process (Model 4; Hayes, 2013) [
85] was utilized to investigate the mediating role of the perceived healthiness of organic food in the connection between the boundary of food information and the intention to purchase. In the current analysis, the boundary of food information was considered the independent variable, whereas the intention to purchase was identified as the dependent variable, with the perceived healthiness of food acting as a mediating factor. This research involved a repeated measures sample consisting of 5000 participants and determined a confidence interval of 95%. Results demonstrated that the food information boundary positively impacted the perception of food healthiness (β = 0.382, t = 2.32,
p = 0.0348). In addition, the perception of food healthiness had a significant influence on the intention to purchase (β = 1.021, t = 11.257,
p < 0.001). At the same time, the direct impact of the food information boundary on purchase intention was found to be non-significant (β = −0.217, t = −1.107,
p = 0.425). Additionally, the perceived healthiness of food demonstrated a mediating effect of 0.3209, with a confidence interval that did not include 0 (LLCI = −0.7221, ULCI = −0.0623), suggesting that this mediating effect is indeed significant. The verification of H2 was also conducted.
3.6. Discussion of Study 1 Results
The first experiment confirmed that the presence of boundaries in organic food information significantly enhances purchase intention through the mediating role of perceived healthiness. This finding aligns with previous research conducted by Shen et al. (2017), who demonstrated that visual boundaries can significantly influence consumer perceptions of healthiness [
6]. Our study extends this by focusing specifically on organic food products. Our study accounted for potential influences of gender and age by including them as control variables in the analysis. The results indicate that the effects of information boundaries on purchase intentions and perceived healthiness are robust and not significantly influenced by gender or age differences. This strengthens the validity of our findings and suggests that the observed effects are primarily driven by the presence or absence of information boundaries. Controlling for the selected variables helps to minimize potential biases and strengthens the robustness of the findings, allowing for more accurate conclusions regarding the impact of information boundaries on consumers’ purchase intentions. Specifically, consumers tend to exhibit enhanced perceptions of healthiness and, in turn, increased intentions to purchase when they encounter information about organic food that incorporates certain boundaries, compared to when such information lacks those boundaries. No significant differences emerged between the two groups regarding brand familiarity, product familiarity, product preference, set weight loss goals, and hunger levels, leaving aside potential alternative interpretations of these aspects. It is crucial to note that, although this study employed the genuine brand “SATINE” organic milk for validation purposes, the effect of participants’ prior perceptions of the actual brand on the results was not entirely managed. Consequently, Experiment 2 opted for the virtual brand “Natural Life” organic whole wheat bread to further evaluate H1 and H2.
4. Experiment 2: Influence of Organic Food Information Boundaries on Purchase Intention (Virtual Brand)
4.1. Experimental Purpose
The aim of this study was to confirm H1 and H2. In contrast to Experiment 1, we substituted the stimuli with a virtual brand to remove the impact of the subjects’ views on the actual brand and enhance the experiment’s robustness. In this study, we utilized the organic whole wheat bread from the fictitious brand “Natural Life” as our stimulus. To closely simulate a real-world scenario, we adopted the presentation style of products and product information from e-commerce platforms like Jindong and Taobao—Chinese shopping websites. Specifically, the food image appeared on the left side of the screen, with the related information placed on the right side, enabling consumers to see the details more clearly. Simultaneously, we provided increasingly detailed food information to the participants, such as the source, expiration date, production standard number, components, and conditions for food storage.
4.2. Experimental Design and Subjects for Study 2
The methodology employed in Experiment 2 closely resembled that of Experiment 1. This study utilized a one-way between-group design based on food information boundary, with conditions of yes and no. A total of 250 participants were sourced via a professional online survey platform, averaging 29.32 years in age (SD = 7.274), which included 159 females, comprising 59.6% of the overall sample. Upon finishing the questionnaire, every participant was awarded a cash incentive between CNY 5 and 10.
4.3. Experimental Procedure for Study 2
In Experiment 2, to eliminate the impact of the participants’ perceptions of the actual brand, the virtual brand “Natural Life” organic whole wheat bread served as one stimulus, while the real brand “SATINE” organic milk was utilized as another stimulus. The experiment involved random assignment into two groups: the bounded group and the unbounded group.
The initial section involved a reading assignment. This reading assignment aimed to influence consumers’ views of “Natural Life” whole wheat bread as a product of organic food production and to shape the perceived limits of the information presented. The subjects were initially presented with a brief overview of Natural Life whole wheat bread by the researcher: “Founded in 2020 in Guilin, Guangxi, the Natural Life Food Company focuses on organic food items, including breads and cereals, and its products are now available throughout China”. The company is preparing to introduce a new product, “Natural Life”, which is organic whole wheat bread. During production, we employed a cutting-edge management process that leads internationally. The wheat undergoes comprehensive milling, adhering to the principle of utilizing every part without discarding or wasting any. Thus, the fiber present in the wheat husk was maintained, and the essential value of wheat malt was safeguarded as well. Whole wheat bread, produced from whole wheat flour, is rich in carbohydrates, proteins, unsaturated fats, dietary fiber, vitamins, minerals, and various other nutrients, particularly B vitamins. This food item excludes artificial components, preservatives, and radiation treatment. Following the review of the material, participants were requested to respond to questions regarding the manipulation test.
The second part was a food evaluation task. First, subjects were told that a manufacturer was about to launch an organic whole wheat bread and that the manufacturer wanted to find out consumers’ attitudes towards this product before officially launching it. The subjects were then presented with a picture of the whole wheat bread and the product information, with the border group presented with the product information with a border (
Figure A2b boundaries) and the borderless group presented with the product information without a border (
Figure A2a without boundaries). The subjects in both groups saw the same product information. The product information included the product name (organic whole wheat bread), shelf life (6 months), storage conditions, and production date. Questions were then answered after the subjects had read the stimulus material.
4.4. Variable Measurement for Study 2
The variables that were manipulated primarily included assessments of perceptions regarding “Natural Living” organic whole wheat bread as organic food. The question posed was “Do you believe that ‘Natural Living’ organic whole wheat bread qualifies as organic food?” Every scale used in this study was a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The mediating variables were assessed based on the perceived healthiness scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.933), which comprised five measures (7 subscales, where 1 represents disagreement and 7 indicates strong agreement): “I think this whole wheat bread is healthy”, “I think this whole wheat bread is environmentally friendly”, “I think this whole wheat bread is nutritious”, “I think this whole wheat bread is low fat”, and “I think this whole wheat bread is good for my body” (Cronbach’s α = 0. 822) [
58]. The assessment of the dependent variable, purchase intention (Cronbach’s α = 0.908), was carried out using the organic food scale designed by Schifferstein [
83] and Grunter et al. [
84]. This scale comprised three primary items, categorized into seven subscales, with responses ranging from 1 (disagree) to 7 (strongly agree): “I would consider buying this whole wheat bread”; “I am very likely to purchase this whole wheat bread”; and “I plan to acquire this whole wheat bread”. Furthermore, participants were assessed for control variables, which included brand familiarity, preferences for products, their level of hunger at the time of the survey, and whether they had a specific weight loss goal. Additionally, various demographic information, such as the participants’ age and gender, was also collected.
4.5. Experimental Results for Study 2
4.5.1. Manipulation Test for Study 2
The results showed that the two groups of participants did not perceive a significant difference in the organic food “Natural Life” whole wheat bread (M with boundaries = 6.01, SD = 1.195; M without boundary = 6.13, SD = 1.182; F(1, 264) = 0.981, p > 0.05). Additionally, there was no notable disparity in the perception related to the size of the information boundary (M with boundaries = 4.68, SD = 1.501; M without boundary = 4.23, SD = 1.723; F(1, 264) = 0.981, p > 0.05). The perceived magnitude of information boundaries (M with boundaries = 4.68, SD = 1.501; M without boundaries = 4.23, SD = 1.723; F(1, 264) = 2.386, p < 0.05) showed a significant difference. To further eliminate potential alternative explanations related to other variables, this study also assessed participants’ familiarity with the brand, familiarity with the product, and emotional response to the brand. The findings indicated that there were notable differences between the two subject groups regarding brand familiarity (M unbounded = 3.92, SD = 1.321; M bounded = 4.11, SD = 1.190; F(1, 264) = 1.515, p > 0.05), product familiarity (M unbounded = 3.79, SD = 2.107; M bounded = 4.07, SD = 1.845; F(1, 264) = 1.562, p > 0.05), and product preference (M unbounded = 3.73, SD = 1.457; M bounded = 3.91, SD = 1.317; F(1, 264) = 1.368, p > 0.05), which were not significantly different. This effectively confirmed the success of the experimental stimulus.
4.5.2. Purchase Intention for Study 2
The results of the ANOVA indicated that participants in the boundary group (M = 5.173, SD = 1.515) showed a higher likelihood of purchasing whole wheat bread compared to those in the no-boundary group (M = 4.711, SD = 1.166) [F(1, 148) = 5.936,
p = 0.016]. As shown in
Figure 3, this suggests that organic food communications featuring information boundaries can enhance consumers’ intention to purchase. The hypothesis H1 was supported.
4.5.3. Perceived Healthiness for Study 2
The ANOVA findings revealed that participants in the bounded group (M = 5.139, SD = 1.022) rated the health benefits of whole wheat bread higher than those in the unbounded group (M = 5.093, SD = 1.879) [F(1, 148) = 4.717, p = 0.024]. This suggests that the perception of the healthiness of organic foods positively influences the decision to buy organic products. Thus, it can be concluded that how healthy organic food is perceived significantly impacts consumers’ intentions to purchase such items.
4.5.4. Mediating Role of Perceived Healthiness of Food in Study 2
The Bootstrapping technique described in the process (Model 4; Hayes, 2013) [
85] was utilized to examine how the perceived healthiness of organic food mediates the connection between the food information boundary and purchase intention. In this study, the food information boundary was treated as the independent variable, with purchase intention identified as the dependent variable, and the perceived healthiness of food functioning as the mediating variable. This research employed a repeated measures sample consisting of 5000 participants, with a confidence interval established at 95%. Results demonstrated that the boundary of food information positively influenced perceptions of food healthiness (β = 0.355, t = 2.28,
p = 0.0242). Additionally, this perception of healthiness regarding food positively affected the intention to purchase (β = 1.046, t = 13.255,
p < 0.001). However, the direct influence of the food information boundary on purchase intention was found to be insignificant (β = −0.167, t = −1.167, t = −1.097,
p = 0.275). The mediating effect of the perceived healthiness of food was calculated to be 0.3709, with a confidence interval that did not include 0 (LLCI = −0.6242, ULCI = −0.0593), suggesting the significance of this mediating effect. The hypothesis H2 was confirmed.
4.6. Discussion of Study 2 Results
To remove the inherent impact of subjects’ views on the actual brand, Experiment 2 employed the virtual brand “Natural Life” organic whole wheat bread as a stimulus. This approach aimed to further confirm the main effect (H1) by circumventing the potential biases stemming from participants’ familiarity with and preference for the existing brand. This approach is consistent with the methodology used by Baklova and Van Tripp (2010), who also utilized virtual brands to assess consumer perceptions without prior brand bias [
5]. In particular, organic food that provided specific information (as opposed to no specific information) would enhance consumers’ perception of healthiness and markedly boost their intention to purchase. Furthermore, Experiment 2 confirmed this mediating effect (H2), which indicates that the perception of healthiness mediated the relationship between information boundaries and purchase intention. After excluding the influence of various potential alternative explanations, we found no significant differences regarding brand familiarity, familiarity with the product, liking of the product, explicit weight loss objectives, and levels of hunger. In Experiment 1, we utilized basic product information as our experimental material; however, it is essential to integrate product images with basic product details to assess the significance and complexity of intentions to purchase organic food. Consequently, Experiment 2 incorporated product images as experimental stimuli, building upon Experiment 1. These images and corresponding details were provided to the participants in order to examine how the complexity of information boundaries influences their food purchasing intentions.
5. Experiment 3: The Moderating Role of Cues of Credence-Label Structure
5.1. Experiment Purpose
The purpose of Experiment 3 was twofold: first, to employ the product information available on the packaging of the virtual brand “Green Classic” organic milk as a stimulus, enhancing the realism of the experimental context and increasing this study’s validity; second, to investigate the moderating influence of cues associated with the design of credence labels, particularly to assess H3.
5.2. Experimental Design and Subjects for Study 3
Experiment 3 used the virtual brand “Green Classic” organic milk as the stimulant, which differed from Experiments 1 and 2 in that the way the pictures were presented to the subjects was changed, and this experiment presented the pictures in three dimensions, with the front and back of the product as a whole, which was more three-dimensional and comprehensive than Experiments 1 and 2. This study implemented a 2 (food information boundary: yes vs. no) × 2 credibility labeling structure cue (product vs. ingredient) intergroup design that involved 350 participants who were recruited from a university located in Southern China. The average age of the participants was 20.32 years (SD = 7.274), with 189 females, representing 59.6% of the overall participant pool. Upon completion of all survey questions, each participant was compensated with a cash reward between CNY 5 and 10.
5.3. Experimental Procedure for Study 3
Participants were randomly allocated into four different experimental groups. We notified the participants that the experiment consisted of two distinct components.
The initial component involved a reading task. The purpose of the reading task was to manipulate the consumer’s perception of “Green Code” organic milk as an organic food product and the perceived information boundaries. Initially, the researcher offered the participants a concise overview of “Green Code” organic milk. This type of milk is generated through a top-tier global production management process that ensures it is free from pollution and additives. It comprises 20 varieties of natural nutrients and boasts over 5% high-quality milk protein per 100 g, exceeding the national standard by 30%. The product is backed by an impeccable quality inspection framework, guaranteeing the integrity of the organic milk from the farm to the final product, making it both safe and trustworthy. Following their review of the material, the participants were requested to respond to questions pertaining to the manipulation test.
The second part was a food evaluation task. First, the subject was told that a manufacturer was about to launch an organic milk product and that the manufacturer wanted to find out consumers’ attitudes towards the product before officially launching it. Subsequently, participants were presented with an image showcasing the product details of Green Dairy Organic Pure Milk and informed that it represented a screenshot of the packaging information for this organic pure milk. Regarding product-level cues within the credence-label framework, individuals in the no-boundary category viewed the product information image without any boundaries (
Figure A4a without boundaries in Product-level Cues of Credence-label Structure), while those in the bounded category observed the food information image with defined boundaries (
Figure A3b boundaries in Product-level Cues of Credence-label Structure). In terms of ingredient-level indicators within the credence-label framework, participants in the no-boundary condition viewed the nutrition information image without any boundaries (
Figure A3a without boundaries in Ingredient-level Cues of Credence-label Structure), while those in the bounded condition observed the nutrition information image with boundaries (
Figure A4b boundaries in Ingredient-level Cues of Credence-label Structure). All four participant groups received identical product information, which encompassed the product name (organic milk), shelf life (6 months), storage requirements (airtight at 2 °C to 6 °C), and production date. Following their review of the stimulus material, participants were then asked to respond to questions.
5.4. Variable Measurement for Study 3
The organic food scale (Cronbach α = 0.908), developed by Grunter et al. [
84] and Schifferstein [
83], was employed to assess the dependent variable of purchase intention. This measurement comprised three items across seven subscales, where a score of 1 indicates disagreement and a score of 7 denotes strong agreement: “I would contemplate purchasing this machine for pure milk”; “it is likely that I would buy this machine for pure milk”; and “I would acquire this machine for pure milk”. The measured variables were assessed according to the perceived healthiness scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.933), utilizing 5 measures across 7 subscales, where 1 indicates disagreement and 7 signifies strong agreement: “I think this organic pure milk is healthy”; “I think this organic pure milk is environmentally friendly”; “I think this organic pure milk is nutritious”; “I think this organic pure milk is low-fat”; and “I think this organic pure milk is good for my body”. Ultimately, factors like the participants’ familiarity with brands, their preference for the product, their hunger levels at the time of the survey, and the presence of a specific weight loss objective were assessed. Additionally, demographic information including the age and gender of the participants was documented.
5.5. Experimental Results for Study 3
5.5.1. Manipulation Test for Study 3
Initially, tests involving manipulation were conducted to explore the cognitive capabilities and the indicators of the credence-label framework associated with organic foods. The results revealed that there was no significant distinction between the two groups of participants concerning their views on Green Classic organic milk as organic food (M with boundaries = 6.24, SD = 1.205; M without boundaries = 6.08, SD = 1.198; F(1, 278) = 0.931, p > 0.05); A notable difference was observed in how the four subject groups perceived the size of information boundaries. Specifically, within the component cues of the credence-label structure, participants in the boundary group (M = 5.721, SD = 1.188) perceived the healthiness of organic pure milk to be significantly higher compared to those in the no-boundary group (M = 3.293, SD = 1.570) [F(1, 278) = 211.321, p < 0.001]. In the analysis of product-level cues related to the credence-label structure, participants from the no-boundary group (M = 4.829, SD = 1.188) perceived organic pure milk as healthier compared to those in the boundary group (M = 3.792, SD = 1.570) [F(1, 278) = 211.321, p < 0.001]. This outcome confirms H3, suggesting that the experimental manipulation of the credence-label structure cues was effective.
5.5.2. Moderating Effect of Cues of Credence-Label Structure
The results from the ANOVA indicated that neither the main effects of the organic food information boundary (
p = 0.934) nor the cues related to credence-label structure (
p = 0.919) significantly influenced perceptions of healthiness regarding organic food. However, the interaction effects were significant [F(1, 276) = 13.693,
p < 0.001]. Subsequently, we employed the Bootstrapping technique in the process (Model 7; Hayes, 2013) [
85], designating the food information boundary as the independent variable, perceptions of food healthiness as the dependent variable, and cues from the credence-label structure as the moderating variable. This analysis utilized a repeated measures sample size of 5000 and established a 95% confidence interval. As shown in
Figure 4, the results showed that the interaction between the food information boundary and cues of the credence-label structure was significant (F = 13.693,
p = 0.0003). Specifically, for product-level cues of the credence-label structure group, the addition of boundaries reduced subjects’ perceptions of the healthiness of organic pure milk (M boundary group = 4.851, SD boundary group = 1.213 vs. M without boundary group = 5.571, SD no-boundary group = 0.992; β = −0.420, t = −2.035,
p = 0.0197), whereas, for ingredient-level cues of the credence-label structure group, the addition of boundaries increased subjects’ perceptions of the healthiness of organic pure milk (M boundary group = 5.560, SD boundary group = 0.939 vs. M without boundary group = 5.043, SD without boundary group = 1.074; β = 0.517, t = 2.888,
p = 0.0042). Thus, H3 was supported.
5.5.3. Moderated Mediating Role
Subsequently, we continue to employ the Bootstrapping technique to examine the mediated model that is influenced by moderation, utilizing Process Model 7 from Hayes [
85]. In this study, the independent variable refers to the information boundary, the dependent variable pertains to the intention to purchase, the mediating variable is related to the perceived healthfulness of food, and the moderating variable includes cues from the credence-label structure. We established a sample size of 5000 for repeated measures, maintaining a confidence level of 95%. The results indicated that the mediated model being moderated was significant with an effect value of 0.9751 (LLCI = 0.4476, ULCI = 1.5099). Specifically, for product-level cues of the credence-label structure group, the mediated effect value for perceived food healthiness was −0.437 (SE = 0.1917, 95% CI [−0.8128, −0.0637]); for ingredient-level cues of the credence-label structure group, the mediated effect value for perceived food healthiness was 0.5; and, for ingredient-level cues of the credence-label structure group, the mediated effect value of perceived healthiness of food was 0.5381 (SE = 0.1806, 95% CI [0.1953, 0.9038]). H3 (a) and H3 (b) are supported.
5.5.4. Exclusion Explanation
The findings indicated that participants across the four categories exhibited an interest in brand familiarity (M unbounded = 3.92, SD = 1.321; M bounded = 4.11, SD = 1.190; F(1, 264) = 1.515, p > 0.05), product familiarity (M with boundaries = 3.79, SD = 2.107; M boundary = 4.07, SD = 1.845; F(1, 264) = 1.562, p > 0.05), and product preference (M unbounded = 3.73, SD = 1.457; M bounded = 3.91, SD = 1.317; F(1, 264) = 1.368, p > 0.05), with no significant differences observed. Furthermore, the presence of a distinct weight loss objective among the participants in the four groups did not show a significant difference (M without boundary = 4.11, SD = 2.056; M boundary = 4.33, SD = 1.963; F(1, 264) = 1.088, p > 0.05). Consequently, the potential confounding influence of alternative explanatory factors, such as consumers’ brand preferences, was ruled out.
5.6. Discussion of Study 3 Results
The third experiment reinforced the initial effect by using the virtual brand Green Classic organic milk as a stimulus, which helped reduce the possible impact of participants’ prior familiarity and preference for the existing brand. In particular, the presence of organic food information boundaries (as opposed to those without) significantly boosted consumers’ intent to purchase (H1). Additionally, the mediating role of healthiness perception (H2) was substantiated; that is, healthiness perception acted as a mediator between the information boundary and purchase intention. Simultaneously, Experiment 3 checked the boundary condition of this proposed effect (H3), focusing on how cues related to the credence-label structure moderated the influence of food information boundaries on perceptions of food healthiness. Specifically, when considering product-level cues of the credence-label structure, the inclusion of food information boundaries leads to a decrease in individuals’ perceived healthiness of food products, which subsequently lowers their purchase intentions (H3b). In contrast, for ingredient-level cues related to the credence-label structure, consumers tend to view food products as healthier, and the addition of food information boundaries enhances their perceived healthiness, thereby increasing their purchase intentions (H3a). In contrast, the distinction between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 lies in the fact that Experiment 3 offers participants a three-dimensional representation, whereas Experiments 1 and 2 provide them with a two-dimensional image format. This advancement in the method of picture presentation greatly enhances the external validity and strength of the experiment.
6. General Discussion
6.1. Conclusions
Research on labels for organic food information has increased; however, the primary emphasis has been placed on aspects such as text, images, design, and color of these labels, with insufficient focus on their boundaries. This study aimed to investigate the influence of these informational boundaries on consumers’ intentions regarding the purchase of organic food, thereby addressing a gap identified in the existing literature. Through experimental data collected from three distinct experiments, the findings illustrate that the boundaries of information labels for organic food significantly affect consumer purchase intentions. Experiment 1 used the real brand “SATINE” organic milk as a stimulant to present the product information to the subjects in a flat format and found that organic food products with boundary information labels could increase consumers’ purchase intention compared to those without boundary information, which verified H1 and H2. Experiment 2 used the virtual brand “Natural Life” organic whole wheat bread as the stimulant and presented the product picture and information to the subjects in a flat way, which improved the validity of the experiment and further verified H1 and H2. The subjects were presented with the overall three-dimensional effect, and the boundary effect (H3) was confirmed, meaning that various types of cues related to the credence-label structure influenced how the organic food information boundary affected consumers’ intention to purchase.
6.2. Theoretical Contributions
This study expands the investigation into information boundaries specifically within the realm of organic food. Previous research on this topic has predominantly centered around conventional food products, indicating that information boundaries relating to food tend to correspond with a diminished perception of health and a reduced likelihood of purchase compared to food information that lacks such boundaries [
86,
87]. In contrast to the findings, this research revealed that, for organic food, information that is limited in scope was more effective in boosting consumers’ purchase intentions compared to information that is unlimited. Moreover, several researchers have examined how the organization of food-related data [
88] and the hue of this information impact traditional purchasing behavior for products [
89]. While both aspects pertain to the external features of food information, they do not specifically address the product information regarding organic food. Li RQ et al. (2021) directly explored the impact of information boundaries on purchase intentions, yet they also focused on food [
9]. This research expands the concept of information boundaries by applying it to organic food, revealing that incorporating a boundary for organic food information relates to the significance of health attributes—a discovery that contrasts with previous studies on food. As a result, this study presents a new viewpoint for exploring the boundaries of information regarding organic food.
Our study aligns with international research that has explored the impact of packaging and labeling on consumer perceptions and purchase intentions, particularly in the context of organic food consumption. For example, studies in Europe and North America have shown that clear and structured information boundaries can significantly influence consumer trust and purchasing behavior [
90]. Our findings further support these conclusions, demonstrating that information boundaries play a crucial role in enhancing perceived healthiness and purchase intentions for organic products. Additionally, research has highlighted the importance of health-related determinants in influencing consumer attitudes towards organic food [
2]. Studies have also shown that credible labeling can significantly impact consumer willingness to pay for certified organic products [
3]. These findings are consistent with global consumer attitudes towards nutrition information on food labels [
74].
Compared to studies that have focused on traditional food products, our research highlights the unique impact of information boundaries on organic food, which is often perceived as healthier and more environmentally friendly [
57]. For instance, a study conducted by Meng Shen et al. (2018) found that information boundaries had a significant effect on health perceptions, similar to our findings [
6]. However, our study extends this by examining the moderating role of credence-label structure cues, which has not been extensively explored in previous research.
Second, this study broadens the scope of the consideration of factors influencing the intentions to purchase organic food. Research has primarily focused on the impact of prior factors like brand identity [
91], environmental consciousness [
46], advertising attractiveness, and product availability on the intention to buy organic food [
92]. Additionally, there has been a greater emphasis on examining the connection between consumers and their heightened purchasing intention from the viewpoint of the consumers themselves [
93]. This research contributes valuable insights to the exploration of intentions behind purchasing organic food. Additionally, it expands upon the investigation of healthiness perception as a mediating factor, enhances the theoretical framework regarding healthiness perception within the context of organic food information boundaries, confirms the impact of these information boundaries on purchase intentions, and uncovers the mediating role that healthiness perception plays in this relationship. Research examining perceptions of healthiness has been undertaken to investigate how these perceptions affect the intentions to repurchase organic food, largely focusing on advertising claims [
94]. Simultaneously, additional research has validated the mediating role of health perceptions in the connection between environmental consciousness and purchasing intentions [
95]. Nevertheless, none have explored the constraints related to the scope of information on organic foods. The findings of this research significantly contribute to understanding the boundaries of organic food information and introduce a fresh perspective for future studies.
Third, this research enhances the insights regarding the credence-label structure within the context of organic food information. Previous studies have explored the cues related to the credence-label structure, primarily emphasizing traditional products [
57]. This research utilizes elements of credence-label structure within the organic food industry, demonstrating that the product cues associated with credence-label structure differ from the ingredient cues within the organic food information framework. Additionally, it confirms the varying impacts of these cues on the organic food information boundary via an empirical investigation. This offers empirical evidence for the significance of implementing credence-label structure cues within marketing science and contributes to enhancing the academic community’s comprehension of these cues in organic food.
6.3. Marketing Insights
Initially, this study’s findings indicate that introducing limitations to organic food information can enhance consumers’ intent to purchase. This suggests that companies may influence consumers’ perceptions of the health benefits of organic food by strategically framing the information surrounding it. On the one hand, businesses can attract consumers’ attention to their products by adding boundaries when displaying information about organic food. On the other hand, when designing promotional images for organic food, advertisers should design the boundaries of the information in the images to be as eye-catching and prominent as possible, making the most of every information position, which may trigger customers’ attention to organic food.
Secondly, this study indicates that how healthy individuals perceive food acts as a mediator in the impact of organic food information constraints on consumers’ intent to purchase. Retailers have the opportunity to utilize health perceptions to differentiate their offerings and strategically place them to capture new customer prospects within the organic market segment. Consequently, in the promotion of organic items, such as healthy organic milk and raw organic fruits and vegetables, adding boundaries to the key information segments of these offerings may enhance consumers’ awareness of the conservation benefits associated with organic foods while also emphasizing health and safety to boost sales.
Third, marketers should create strategies for the layout of organic foods tailored to various population segments. The influence of information boundaries on the intention to purchase organic products relies on the characteristics of the cues related to credence-label structures. When it comes to organic items that emphasize health and safety features, businesses ought to showcase the product’s distinct qualities and incorporate information boundaries, particularly when presenting organic options. This approach aims to address the desires of health-conscious consumers. Conversely, for organic foods lacking health-related information, omitting boundaries may enhance consumer perceptions of these products as environmentally sustainable and cost-effective, thereby encouraging purchases. Consequently, retailers must carefully design their information boundaries in accordance with the specific attributes of the credence-label structures.
Our findings contribute to the existing literature by providing empirical evidence that information boundaries can influence consumer behavior in the context of organic food. This is consistent with broader research on consumer psychology and marketing, which emphasizes the importance of visual cues and information presentation. For example, studies have shown that clear and structured information boundaries can significantly enhance consumer trust and purchasing behavior [
5]. Additionally, research has highlighted the role of visual cues in shaping consumer perceptions of health and environmental sustainability [
96]. By incorporating these insights, our study offers practical guidance for marketers aiming to develop effective packaging strategies for organic products.
6.4. Legal Framework for Product Appearance and Labeling
The labeling and appearance of organic food products are strictly regulated by legal frameworks such as China’s National Standard for Organic Products (GB/T 19630) and the European Union’s Regulation (EC) No 834/2007 [
97,
98], which require clear certification information to protect consumer rights and ensure the authenticity of product claims. Our study strictly adheres to these regulations, ensuring that experimental materials align with real-world organic food labeling practices to enhance the relevance of our findings.
6.5. Limitations and Future Research
First, in presenting consumers’ perceptions of healthiness, emphasis is placed on the perception of organic food as an attribute of healthiness, which includes not only healthy nutritional properties but also attributes such as naturalness and lack of contamination, and consumers may have different cognitive responses depending on the attribute they perceive.
Secondly, this research examines how perceived healthiness might act as a mediating factor in the impact of organic food information boundaries on consumers’ purchase intentions. Additionally, in future studies, it may be beneficial to incorporate variables like perceived utility and green trust when analyzing the effect of organic food information boundaries on purchase intentions.
Third, this study focused only on moderating variables related to the product itself; other boundary conditions, such as the level of organic knowledge and the restrictive nature of variables such as product type (search-based versus experiential, functional versus hedonic), could be explored in the future.
Fourth, this study primarily focused on younger consumers in the sample, while future research might investigate how perceived healthiness influences purchase intentions among middle-aged and older individuals.