Next Article in Journal
Social Security Enrollment as an Indicator of State Fragility and Legitimacy: A Field Experiment in Maghreb Countries
Next Article in Special Issue
Dating Violence: A Bibliometric Review of the Literature in Web of Science and Scopus
Previous Article in Journal
From the Inquisition Pyre to Insertion into the Church: The Familial and Social Trajectory of Hernando Ortiz, a Jewish Convert in the Spanish Empire in the 16th Century
Previous Article in Special Issue
Strategies for Territorial Peace: The Overcoming of the Structural Violence in Women Living in Palmira, Colombia
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Psychological and Gender Differences in a Simulated Cheating Coercion Situation at School

by
Marina Begoña Martínez-González
1,*,
Claudia Patricia Arenas-Rivera
1,
Aura Alicia Cardozo-Rusinque
1,
Aldair Ricardo Morales-Cuadro
1,
Mónica Acuña-Rodríguez
1,
Yamile Turizo-Palencia
1 and
Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez
2,3
1
Department of Social Science, Universidad de la Costa, Barranquilla 080001, Colombia
2
Faculty of Sport Sciences, Universidad Europea de Madrid, 28670 Villaviciosa de Odón, Spain
3
Grupo de Investigación en Cultura, Educación y Sociedad, Universidad de la Costa, Barranquilla 080001, Colombia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10(7), 265; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10070265
Submission received: 18 May 2021 / Revised: 28 June 2021 / Accepted: 7 July 2021 / Published: 9 July 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Violence, Victimization and Prevention)

Abstract

:
This study aimed to analyze gender, anxiety, and psychological inflexibility differences of high school students’ behaviors in a simulated situation of peer coercion into academic cheating. Method: A total of 1147 volunteer adolescents participated, (Men: N = 479; Mage = 16.3; Women: N = 668; Mage = 16.2). The participants saw 15 s animated online video presenting peer coercion into an academic cheating situation, including a questionnaire about their reactions to face the situation. They also answered the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory for children and adolescents and the Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (AFQ-Y). Gender was associated with the behaviors facing the situation. Higher state anxiety and inflexibility were present in those participants that avoided aggressive behaviors facing the situation; on the other hand, trait anxiety was present in those who reacted aggressively. Finally, higher anxiety and inflexibility were associated with the used moral disengagement mechanisms, but also with peers’ perception as sanctioning or being against the participants’ decision. The most aggressive students were more flexible and less stressed than those who tried to solve assertively. Expectations about peers seem to be relevant to the decision-making facing moral dilemmas and peer victimization.

1. Introduction

Legitimacy is considered the moral basis of social interaction (Kelman 2001). This construct has been used to describe beliefs that justified different punishable actions as they were normal or socially accepted (Martínez-González et al. 2021). Studies have focused on children’s evaluation of the situation ( Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Fernández Villanueva 2009; Martínez-González et al. 2019), the moral disengagement mechanisms they use (Bandura 1999, 2002), and the expectations about peers and adults as legitimizers of violence (Martínez-González et al. 2021).
The moral disengagement mechanisms refer to beliefs that people use to maintain a positive self-concept and reduce their guilt when acting against moral standards (Bandura 1999, 2002). There are (a) moral justifications, which link a violent act to a heroic or social desire; (b) euphemistic labeling, which changes the destructive connotation of the act; (c) advantageous comparison, which reduces the immoral act comparing it with another more despicable; (d) displacement of responsibility, transferring blame to an authority figure; (e) diffusion of responsibility when the perception of guilt is reduced as a consequence of acting in a group; (f) distorting the consequences, which reduces the damaging effects of a behavior; (g) victim blaming, considering the victim as the motivator of the situation; and (h) dehumanization, depriving people of their human qualities to facilitate abusing them.
The moral disengagement mechanisms allow explaining immoral acts from genocide (Bandura 1999) to daily acts of corruption (Martínez-González et al. 2020; Zhao et al. 2019) such as academic cheating (Barbaranelli et al. 2018) or peer victimization (Meter et al. 2019). It was found that students more sensitive to social pressure are more likely to academically cheat than those who give greater emphasis to their moral identity (Wowra 2007).
Clear rules established justly and equitably positively impact students’ moral identity development and auto-understanding (Ramberg and Modin 2019; Riekie et al. 2017). In contrast, dishonest acts, distrust, and manipulation have been associated with bullying behaviors (Andreou 2004), and these generate internalizing problems as well as health problems (Arana et al. 2018; Dirks et al. 2017; Iyer-Eimerbrink and Jensen-Campbell 2019; Joseph and Stockton 2018).
These behaviors have been related to anxiety, which could fluctuate in different situations or evolutionary stages of life, presenting women with higher anxiety levels and internalizing problems, especially in adolescents (Hernandez Rodriguez et al. 2020; Storch et al. 2003). The incidence of state anxiety has been associated with maturational and reproductive processes, social pressure in adolescence, negative affect with stress, anxiety, and depression (McLean and Anderson 2009; Mercader-Yus et al. 2018). Many of these situations occur at school, where young conflicts find their natural scenario.
Social pressure at school could nudge the students to resort to academic cheating in contexts where this is adopted as a practice legitimized by their peers (Barbaranelli et al. 2018; Farnese et al. 2011; Griebeler 2019) but also could be a situation that generates conflicts between students. In this line, children’s expectations about peers play an important role when they face school conflicts (Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019).
On the other hand, violence in the family and community as a legitimate way to solve conflicts has been associated with early violence legitimation (Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Martínez-González et al. 2016). Those environments reduce prosocial behaviors (Galán Jiménez 2018), expose children to the risk of reproducing violence in their daily relationships (Kim et al. 2019a, 2019b) and in the society that they will constitute in adulthood (Goodman et al. 2020), and also expose them to chronic stress, compromising their health (Finegood et al. 2020).
We proposed the present research to analyze gender, anxiety, and psychological inflexibility differences of high school students’ behaviors in a simulated situation of peer coercion into academic cheating.
Our hypotheses were (i) gender differences would be present in the behavior facing peer coercion into academic cheating; (ii) higher anxiety and inflexibility would be present in those participants that reacted aggressively facing the situation; (iii) higher anxiety and inflexibility would be associated with the rejection of moral disengagement mechanisms; (iv) higher anxiety and inflexibility would be associated with peers’ perception as sanctioners or opposed to the decision made facing the situation.

2. Results

This study aimed to analyze gender, anxiety, and psychological flexibility differences in teenagers’ behavior facing a simulated situation of peer coercion into academic cheating. Hypothesis i was confirmed, since the gender of participants and the gender of the offenders was associated with the behaviors facing the situation; Hypothesis ii was partially confirmed since higher state anxiety and inflexibility were present in those participants that avoided aggressive behaviors facing the situation, but trait anxiety was present in those who reacted aggressively; Hypothesis iii was rejected since higher trait anxiety and inflexibility were associated with the used moral disengagement mechanisms. Hypothesis iv was accepted, considering that higher trait anxiety and inflexibility were associated with perceiving witnesses as sanctioners. Next, we will explain these results.
Differences were found in the reactions associated with the gender of the participant and the peer who pressed academic fraud (p = 0.10). Most of the female participants were inclined to avoid conflict or assertively resolve the situation. The males resolved it assertively and, secondly, avoided the conflict. A more significant proportion of males presented aggressive behavior compared to females (Table 1).
When the peer that pressured someone into academic fraud was female, significant differences were found in their responses associated with the gender of the peer that pressured someone into academic fraud (p = 0.017). There were more aggressive responses when the peer was a man and more assertive responses when the peer was a woman (Table 2).
No significant differences were found in state anxiety (p = 0.086), trait anxiety (p = 0.341), and psychological inflexibility (p = 0.301) related to gender when facing coercion into academic fraud. However, these variables showed significant differences between those who decided to attack and those who did not. Although state anxiety was higher than trait anxiety, it was higher in participants who decided not to attack. They also evidenced high psychological inflexibility. Trait anxiety was higher in participants who reacted aggressively (Table 3).
The state anxiety did not show significant differences associated with the different mechanisms of moral disconnection. In contrast, trait anxiety presented significant differences when moral disengagement mechanisms, such as moral justification, distorting the consequences, victim blaming, and dehumanization were used. The psychological inflexibility showed significant differences related to the use of advantageous comparison and dehumanization. The participants who showed higher trait anxiety considered their reaction as morally correct. The same happened to the participants who considered that they did not hurt the offender and those who did not blame the peer for initiating the situation. The participants who showed greater inflexibility avoided comparing their behavior with a worse one to justify their reaction. Finally, trait anxiety evidenced significant differences regarding witness peers’ expectations, being higher in those who perceived them as sanctioning or legitimizing their reaction facing the situation. A greater inflexibility was found in the participants who perceived the witness peers as neutral or opposed to their reaction to the situation presented (Table 4).

3. Discussion

Analyzing the gender differences in coercion into academic fraud, female adolescents were more avoidant, while male adolescents were more aggressive, coinciding with previous studies about peer aggression in adolescence (McNaughton Reyes et al. 2019). We found how the reaction to the situation was more aggressive if the offender was a man and more assertive if the partner was a woman. In this regard, boys suffered more significant and different types of victimization than girls (Hernandez Rodriguez et al. 2020; Joseph and Stockton 2018). These trends in gender differences are linked to the way interpersonal relationships are built in childhood from an early age, as well as the expectations of behavior associated with each gender (Card et al. 2008; Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Martínez-González et al. 2016; Martínez-González et al. 2021). In consequence, the use of conciliatory strategies, seeking help from adults, or avoiding confrontation may become a factor of greater risk of victimization by peers if these alternatives are used by male teenagers (Dirks et al. 2017), even when the group rejects peer victimization (Guimond et al. 2018). One reason is that assertiveness and assent to dialogue are associated with female stereotypes (Mehta and Dementieva 2017). However, men receive more social pressure from peers and adults to use violence as an accepted way of solving conflicts in their daily relationships (Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Farrell et al. 2011; Kim et al. 2019a, 2019b). Thus, assertiveness is a strategy generally seen as socially skillful and would be associated with less victimization, but only for youth who experience less aggression from their peers (Dirks et al. 2017).
Regarding avoidance as a reaction of female adolescents facing coercion into academic cheating, new evidence indicates that women can assume avoidant attitudes or maintain silence when facing moral dilemmas that benefit or even disadvantage them (Bossuyt and Van Kenhove 2018). In this line, several studies evidenced that girls tend to suffer more social manipulation from their peer group than boys (Hernandez Rodriguez et al. 2020; Joseph and Stockton 2018). The position of avoidance and silence in situations of victimization may be related to greater social need, fear of abandonment, difficulty in managing emotions, and more binding relationships (Benenson et al. 2014). These situations place the women at risk of several future forms of violence (Pokharel et al. 2020) with implications even for their moral functioning (Bossuyt and Van Kenhove 2018). Therefore, it is important to design interventions at this stage for the students to be aware of this type of behavior and the implications it has for their individual future and the way they will interact socially.
Concerning state anxiety and psychological inflexibility results, it was found that both were higher in participants who decided not to attack. In contrast, trait anxiety was significantly higher in the participants who reacted aggressively. These characteristics of the population that responded aggressively can be related to the conditions of the Colombian context, where there is a high exposure to violence in everyday relationships (Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Martínez-González et al. 2016). These responses are socially legitimized as the correct procedure and are seen as usual (Martínez-González et al. 2016).
In this regard, people with high reactivity to fear in the face of threats have shown more evasive responses to a threat or provocation in a conflict situation (Beyer et al. 2017; Vogel and Schwabe 2019), while people characterized by traits with a high level of motivation were more likely to participate in aggressive interactions when provoked (Beyer et al. 2017). The fight or escape elections in a conflict situation have been mainly driven by the possibility of retaliating against the aggressor (Beyer et al. 2017). Thus, anxious symptoms are associated with less support for aggressive responses to challenging hypothetical peer situations and peer victimization (Dirks et al. 2017; Wright et al. 2010), in contraposition to the trait anxiety.
High trait anxiety causes a higher reactivity to any threat; therefore, it will be easier to show an attacking or aggressive behavior to face a threat. However, the state anxiety, a feeling that participant will experience when the controllability perception of the environment is low, and the certainty of the lack of resources to face any eliciting situation that appears, will provoke a flight reaction (Bustamante-Sánchez et al. 2020; Tornero-Aguilera et al. 2020). In this line, psychological inflexibility produces a similar response. Lower psychological flexibility would be related to lower cognitive adaptability to the changing environment, eliciting a larger number of uncontrollable feelings, especially when a stressor occurs (e.g., aggression). Previous authors have shown how psychological inflexibility is linked to an increased modulation of the sympathetic autonomic nervous system, which regulates the stress response (Beltrán-Velasco et al. 2021; Sánchez-Conde et al. 2019). In this physiological situation, it has also been possible to verify how information processing decreases and more irrational decisions tend to be made (Delgado-Moreno et al. 2019; Tornero-Aguilera and Clemente-Suárez 2018). Therefore, if we combine high psychological inflexibility, a decrease in information processing, and high state anxiety, it is predictable that the expected response would be an avoidance rather than a confrontational or attacking behavior to face the eliciting situation.
The participants who showed greater trait anxiety avoided aggression and considered this reaction to be morally correct, without consequences to the offender, and did not blame him for causing the situation. Furthermore, trait anxiety was also more significant in those adolescents who expected sanction or legitimation from witnesses. These teenagers could tend toward group acceptance and avoiding conflict. They may be more likely to yield to academic cheating by being more sensitive to social pressure when fraud is a legitimized practice by peers (Barbaranelli et al. 2018; Farnese et al. 2011; Griebeler 2019; Wowra 2007). Consequently, children’s and adolescents’ expectations of their peers as legitimizers of transgressive behaviors play an important role when facing school conflicts (Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Martínez-González et al. 2021) and require attention to implement intervention programs.
Regarding the participants who showed greater inflexibility, they avoided comparing their behavior with a worse one to justify their reaction and considered the witnesses as neutral or in opposition to this. These teenagers could exhibit more consistent and rigid moral behaviors, thus avoiding falling into fraud (Wowra 2007). However, they may also have more difficulties adapting to the group rules and experiencing more loneliness, which could lead to the appearance of internalizing symptoms if they are victims of intimidation and social victimization (Arana et al. 2018; Iyer-Eimerbrink and Jensen-Campbell 2019). Negative experiences of friendship, aggressive conflicts, and low reciprocal friendship levels exacerbate the adverse effects of maladaptive coping associated with a higher feeling of school loneliness and continuous victimization experiences among peers (Gardner 2019). While higher anxiety symptoms are associated with less aggressive responses, higher levels of depressive symptoms have been related to aggressive behavior and, for girls, the reduction of assertive strategies (Cuttini 2017; Dirks et al. 2014). Those findings suggest that anxiety may be associated with a less problematic response than depression facing peer victimization (Cuttini 2017).
These gender differences consistently evident in peer victimization literature have implications for preventing and reducing social bullying in its different manifestations. Specific intervention at school, at home, and other social interventions are needed to stop the increasing tendency in bullying cases. The information provided in the present research could be used to design and apply this program with the teenage population.
Moral disengagement may serve as an important risk indicator for the appearance of more aggression in conflict situations among adolescents, characterized by the perception of injustice and sustained victimization over time. Identifying how adolescents handle situations involving moral dilemmas and their perception of the offenders and witnesses could help develop effective interventions to promote the resolution of conflicts using socially acceptable strategies. Interventions to promote empathy and prosociality are crucial for victims, offenders, and witnesses who can advocate against victimization (Meter et al. 2019). Preserving clear rules established fairly and impartially may help adolescents’ moral behavior and help them understand themselves (Ramberg and Modin 2019; Riekie et al. 2017).

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Participants

A total of 1147 volunteer adolescents participated, aged between 12 and 18 years (Men: N = 479; M = 16.3; SD = 1.1; Women: N = 668; M = 16.2; SD = 0.9) with a stratified random sampling of simple affixation in which the sample was collected from schools with different socioeconomic levels in the city of Barranquilla (Colombia). All the procedures were conducted following the Helsinki Declaration (as revised in Brazil, 2013) and approved by the University Ethical Committee (approval code 094). The data were collected anonymously. Before participation, all participants, parental or guardian, and their professors were informed about the experimental procedures, indicating the right to withdraw from the study at any time and providing written informed consent.

4.2. Procedure

This study relied on the use of animated video that simulated a cheating coercion situation at school with a different combination of peer and teacher genders as the authority figure to evaluate different behaviors from participants, as well as the influence of gender, anxiety, and psychological inflexibility on the high school students’ behavior. The use of simulated scenarios of violence has been effectively used by previous specific researchers (Hyatt et al. 2019; Martínez-González et al. 2019; Cardozo-Rusinque et al. 2019; Anderson et al. 2016; Rayburn et al. 2007). The teenagers were contacted in several high schools, and the sample was formed from students who accepted, and had parents who accepted, their participation. Groups of 30 participants completed the task in a computer room, sitting randomly according to their gender. We prepared the computers with the situations according to gender, two lines for males and two lines for females, varying the gender of the aggressor. Then, when students entered the classroom, we asked them to sit in the line corresponding to their gender identity. First, they read the objectives of the research and consented to participation. Next, the instructions and demographic questions appeared; they watched the video and answered the questions proposed to analyze their behavior. Finally, the participants filled out an anxiety questionnaire.

4.3. Instruments

4.3.1. Test Used to evaluate Anxiety

Anxiety was measured by the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory for children and adolescents (Castrillón-Moreno and Borrero-Copete 2005). It is composed of two scales, the first to measure State Anxiety, with 20 items (reliability: McDonald’s ω = 0.72 on this study), and the second to measure Trait Anxiety, with 20 items (reliability: McDonald’s ω = 0.91 on this study).

4.3.2. Test Used to Evaluate Psychological Inflexibility

This variable was measured by the Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (AFQ-Y). It is a 7-item questionnaire where the participant must respond on a 1 to 7 scale (Valdivia-Salas et al. 2017) (reliability: McDonald’s ω = 0.87 on this study).

4.3.3. Instrument to Assess Participant’s Behavior in the Academic Cheating Coercion at School

Participants saw a 15 s animated online video presenting peer coercion into an academic cheating situation. There were four different stimuli, varying the gender of the peer. The stimulus presented an exam staged in the classroom. A voice-over described the situation to generate identification with the main character, as follows: “During an evaluation, a classmate asks you for the answer to a question. When you refuse to tell him, he responds by insulting you. Given this, what do you do?”. There was no presence of authority figures in the scene. See the videos here:
The video is embedded in a Google form. After being played, the following response options appeared:
-
I respond the same, with rudeness (aggressive).
-
I ask the teacher to change my position (avoidant).
-
I call the teacher to come over (supportive).
-
I give him/her the answer to avoid being attacked (submissive).
-
I tell him/her that his/her evaluation could be canceled (assertive).
The answers were registered as assertive, avoidant, aggressive, submissive, and supportive behaviors. For this aim, the categories were integrated as “no attack” when the responses were alternatives to violence, and the aggressive responses were integrated as “attack”.
After this, questions inspired by moral disengagement mechanisms theory (Bandura et al. 1996; Bandura 1999, 2002) and questions about violence legitimation perceived from peers and adults were presented to analyze the participant behavior. These items were registered as nominal variables (Table 5):

4.4. Statistical Analysis

JASP statistical software was used to analyze the data. Chi-square test and ANOVA were used to analyze anxiety and gender differences related to the proposed situations’ behavior. The level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

5. Conclusions

In a situation of coercion into academic fraud, female adolescents were more avoidant, and male adolescents were more aggressive. The most aggressive students presented lower psychological inflexibility and stress and higher trait anxiety than those who tried to solve assertively. Non-aggressive behaviors were associated with higher psychological inflexibility and state anxiety. Finally, expectations about peers were shown to be relevant for decision making facing moral dilemmas and peer victimization.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.B.M.-G., C.P.A.-R., A.A.C.-R., A.R.M.-C., M.A.-R., Y.T.-P. and V.J.C.-S. methodology, M.B.M.-G. and C.P.A.-R.; formal analysis, M.B.M.-G.; investigation, M.B.M.-G., C.P.A.-R., A.A.C.-R., A.R.M.-C., M.A.-R., Y.T.-P. and V.J.C.-S.; resources, M.B.M.-G., C.P.A.-R., A.A.C.-R., A.R.M.-C., M.A.-R., Y.T.-P. and V.J.C.-S.; data curation, M.B.M.-G.; writing—original draft preparation, M.B.M.-G., C.P.A.-R., A.A.C.-R., A.R.M.-C., M.A.-R., Y.T.-P. and V.J.C.-S.; writing—review and editing, M.B.M.-G., C.P.A.-R., A.A.C.-R., A.R.M.-C., M.A.-R., Y.T.-P. and V.J.C.-S.; visualization M.B.M.-G.; supervision, M.B.M.-G.; project administration, M.B.M.-G.; funding acquisition, M.B.M.-G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Project funding was provided by the Universidad de la Costa.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University Ethical Committee (approval code 094).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data supporting presented results can be found by directly asking the first author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Anderson, RaeAnn E., Amanda M. Brouwer, Angela R. Wendorf, and Shawn P. Cahill. 2016. Women’s behavioral responses to the threat of a hypothetical date rape stimulus: A qualitative analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior 45: 793–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Andreou, Eleni. 2004. Bully/Victim Problems and Their Association with Machiavellianism and Self-Efficacy in Greek Primary School Children. British Journal of Educational Psychology 74: 297–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Arana, Allyson A., Erin Q. Boyd, Maria Guarneri-White, Priya Iyer-Eimerbrink, Angela Liegey Dougall, and Lauri Jensen-Campbell. 2018. The Impact of Social and Physical Peer Victimization on Systemic Inflammation in Adolescents. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 64: 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bandura, Albert. 1999. Moral Disengagement in the Perpetration of Inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review 3: 193–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Bandura, Albert. 2002. Selective Moral Disengagement in the Exercise of Moral Agency. Journal of Moral Education 31: 101–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bandura, Albert, Claudio Barbaranelli, Gian Vittorio Caprara, and Concetta Pastorelli. 1996. Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 364–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Barbaranelli, Claudio, Maria L. Farnese, Carlo Tramontano, Roberta Fida, Valerio Ghezzi, Marinella Paciello, and Philip Long. 2018. Machiavellian Ways to Academic Cheating: A Mediational and Interactional Model. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Beltrán-Velasco, Ana Isabel, Paula Sánchez-Conde, Domingo Jesús Ramos-Campo, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2021. Monitorization of autonomic stress response of nurse students in hospital clinical simulation. BioMed Research. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Benenson, Joyce F., Melissa N. Kuhn, Patrick J. Ryan, Anthony J. Ferranti, Rose Blondin, Michael Shea, Chalice Charpentier, Melissa Emery Thompson, and Richard W. Wrangham. 2014. Human Males Appear More Prepared Than Females to Resolve Conflicts with Same-Sex Peers. Human Nature 25: 251–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Beyer, Frederike, Macià Buades-Rotger, Marie Claes, and Ulrike M. Krämer. 2017. Hit or Run: Exploring Aggressive and Avoidant Reactions to Interpersonal Provocation Using a Novel Fight-or-Escape Paradigm (FOE). Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 11: 190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Bossuyt, Saar, and Patrick Van Kenhove. 2018. Assertiveness Bias in Gender Ethics Research: Why Women Deserve the Benefit of the Doubt: Marketing and Consumer Behavior. Journal of Business Ethics 150: 727–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bustamante-Sánchez, Alvaro, José Francisco Tornero-Aguilera, Valentín E. Fernández-Elías, Alberto J. Hormeño-Holgado, Athanasios A. Dalamitros, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2020. Effect of Stress on Autonomic and Cardiovascular Systems in Military Population: A Systematic Review. Cardiology Research and Practice. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Card, Noel A., Brian D. Stucky, Gita M. Sawalani, and Todd D. Little. 2008. Direct and Indirect Aggression During Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-Analytic Review of Gender Differences, Intercorrelations, and Relations to Maladjustment. Child Development 79: 1185–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cardozo-Rusinque, Aura Alicia, Marina B. Martínez-González, Adriana Angélica De La Peña-Leiva, Inírida Avedaño-Villa, and Tito José Crissien-Borrero. 2019. Factores Psicosociales Asociados al Conflicto Entre Menores En El Contexto Escolar. Educação & Sociedade 40: e0189140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Castrillón-Moreno, Diego Alonso, and Pablo Emilio Borrero-Copete. 2005. Validación del inventario de Ansiedad Estado-Rasgo (STAIC) en niños escolarizados entre los 8 y 15 años. Acta Colombiana de Psicología 8: 79–90. Available online: https://bit.ly/2QIuQyd (accessed on 8 July 2021).
  16. Cuttini, Laura A. 2017. Early Adolescents’ Reported Responses to Victimization by Peers: Links to Type of Provocation and Internalizing Symptoms. Ph.D. thesis, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. Available online: https://escholarship.mcgill.ca/concern/theses/pv63g270z?locale=en (accessed on 7 July 2021).
  17. Delgado-Moreno, Rosa, Jose Juan Robles-Pérez, Susana Aznar-Laín, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2019. Effect of experience and psychophysiological modification by combat stress in soldier’s memory. Journal of Medical Systems 43: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Dirks, Melanie A., Teresa A. Treat, and V. Robin Weersing. 2014. Youth’s Responses to Peer Provocation: Links to Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 36: 339–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dirks, Melanie A., Laura A. Cuttini, Addison Mott, and David B. Henry. 2017. Associations Between Victimization and Adolescents’ Self-Reported Responses to Peer Provocation Are Moderated by Peer-Reported Aggressiveness. Journal of Research on Adolescence 27: 436–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Farnese, Maria Luisa, Carlo Tramontano, Roberta Fida, and Marinella Paciello. 2011. Cheating Behaviors in Academic Context: Does Academic Moral Disengagement Matter? Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences 29: 356–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Farrell, Albert D., David B. Henry, Sally A. Mays, and Michael E. Schoeny. 2011. Parents as Moderators of the Impact of School Norms and Peer Influences on Aggression in Middle School Students: Parent Protective Factors for Aggression. Child Development 82: 146–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Fernández Villanueva, Icíar. 2009. Justificación y Legitimación de La Violencia En La Infancia: Un Estudio Sobre La Legitimación Social de Las Agresiones En Los Conflictos Cotidianos Entre Menores. Ph.D. thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Servicio de Publicaciones, Madrid, Spain. Available online: https://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/8436/ (accessed on 7 July 2021).
  23. Finegood, Eric D., Edith Chen, Jennifer Kish, Katherine Vause, Adam K.K. Leigh, Lauren Hoffer, and Gregory E. Miller. 2020. Community Violence and Cellular and Cytokine Indicators of Inflammation in Adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology 115: 104628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Galán Jiménez, Jaime Sebastián F. 2018. Exposición a La Violencia En Adolescentes: Desensibilización, Legitimación y Naturalización. Diversitas 14: 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gardner, Sarah. 2019. Coping with School-Based Peer-Victimisation: The Role of Peers. Ph.D. thesis, Nottingham University, Nottingham, UK. Available online: http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/37966/ (accessed on 7 July 2021).
  26. Goodman, Michael L., Andrea Hindman, Philip H. Keiser, Stanley Gitari, Katherine Ackerman Porter, and Ben G. Raimer. 2020. Neglect, Sexual Abuse, and Witnessing Intimate Partner Violence During Childhood Predicts Later Life Violent Attitudes Against Children Among Kenyan Women: Evidence of Intergenerational Risk Transmission From Cross-Sectional Data. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 35: 623–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Griebeler, Marcelo de C. 2019. “But Everybody’s Doing It!”: A Model of Peer Effects on Student Cheating. Theory and Decision 86: 259–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Guimond, Fanny-Alexandra, Mara Brendgen, Stephanie Correia, Lyse Turgeon, and Frank Vitaro. 2018. The Moderating Role of Peer Norms in the Associations of Social Withdrawal and Aggression with Peer Victimization’. Developmental Psychology 54: 1519–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Hernandez Rodriguez, Juventino, Samantha J. Gregus, James T. Craig, Freddie A. Pastrana, and Timothy A. Cavell. 2020. Anxiety Sensitivity and Children’s Risk for Both Internalizing Problems and Peer Victimization Experiences. Child Psychiatry & Human Development 51: 174–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hyatt, Courtland S., Amos Zeichner, and Joshua D. Miller. 2019. Laboratory aggression and personality traits: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Violence 9: 675–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Iyer-Eimerbrink, Priya A., and Lauri A. Jensen-Campbell. 2019. The Long-term Consequences of Peer Victimization on Physical and Psychological Health: A Longitudinal Study. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Joseph, Stephen, and Hannah Stockton. 2018. The Multidimensional Peer Victimization Scale: A Systematic Review. Aggression and Violent Behavior 42: 96–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kelman, Herbert C. 2001. Reflections on the social and psychological processes of legitimization and delegitimization. In The Psychology of Legitimacy: Emerging Perspectives on Ideology, Justice, and Intergroup Relations. Edited by John. T. Jost and Brenda Major. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 54–73. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kim, Jeongsuk, Bora Lee, and Naomi B. Farber. 2019a. Where Do They Learn Violence? The Roles of Three Forms of Violent Socialization in Childhood. Children and Youth Services Review 107: 104494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kim, Jeongsuk, Youn Kyoung Kim, and Naomi B. Farber. 2019b. Multiple Forms of Early Violent Socialization and the Acceptance of Interpersonal Violence Among Chinese College Students. Violence and Victims 34: 474–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Martínez-González, Marina B., Claudia Robles-Haydar, José Amar-Amar, and Fernando Crespo-Romero. 2016. Crianza y Desconexión Moral En Infantes: Su Relación En Una Comunidad Vulnerable de Barranquilla. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, Niñez y Juventud 14: 315–30. [Google Scholar]
  37. Martínez-González, Marina B., Claudia Robles-Haydar, José Amar-Amar, Raimundo Abello, Daladier Jabba, and Jairo Pimentel. 2019. Role-Playing Game as a Computer-Based Test to Assess the Resolution of Conflicts in Childhood. Interciencia. Available online: https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=33960068012 (accessed on 7 July 2021).
  38. Martínez-González, Marina Begoña, Claudia A. Robles-Haydar, and Judys Alfaro-Álvarez. 2020. Concepto de desconexion moral y sus manifestaciones contemporaneas/Moral disengagement concept and its contemporary manifestations. Utopía y Praxis Latinoamericana 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Martínez-González, Marina B., Yamile Turizo-Palencia, Claudia Arenas-Rivera, Mónica Acuña-Rodríguez, Yeferson Gómez-López, and Vicente J. Clemente-Suárez. 2021. Gender, Anxiety, and Legitimation of Violence in Adolescents Facing Simulated Physical Aggression at School. Brain Sciences 11: 458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. McLean, Carmen P., and Emily R. Anderson. 2009. Brave Men and Timid Women? A Review of the Gender Differences in Fear and Anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review 29: 496–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. McNaughton Reyes, H. Luz, Vangie A. Foshee, May S. Chen, and Susan T. Ennett. 2019. Patterns of Adolescent Aggression and Victimization: Sex Differences and Correlates. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma 28: 1130–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mehta, Clare M., and Yulia Dementieva. 2017. The Contextual Specificity of Gender: Femininity and Masculinity in College Students’ Same- and Other-Gender Peer Contexts. Sex Roles 76: 604–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mercader-Yus, Esther, M. Carmen Neipp-López, Pedro Gómez-Méndez, Fernando Vargas-Torcal, Melissa Gelves-Ospina, Laura Puerta-Morales, Alexandra León-Jacobus, Kattia Cantillo-Pacheco, and Malka Mancera-Sarmiento. 2018. Ansiedad, autoestima e imagen corporal en niñas con diagnóstico de pubertad precoz. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría 47: 229–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Meter, Diana J., Ting-Lan Ma, and Samuel E. Ehrenreich. 2019. Telling, Comforting, and Retaliating: The Roles of Moral Disengagement and Perception of Harm in Defending College-Aged Victims of Peer Victimization. International Journal of Bullying Prevention 1: 124–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Pokharel, Bijaya, Kathy Hegadoren, and Elizabeth Papathanassoglou. 2020. Factors Influencing Silencing of Women Who Experience Intimate Partner Violence: An Integrative Review. Aggression and Violent Behavior 52: 101422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ramberg, Joacim, and Bitte Modin. 2019. School Effectiveness and Student Cheating: Do Students’ Grades and Moral Standards Matter for This Relationship? ’ Social Psychology of Education 22: 517–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Rayburn, Nadine Recker, Lisa H. Jaycox, Daniel F. McCaffrey, Emilio C. Ulloa, Megan Zander-Cotugno, Grant N. Marshall, and Gene A. Shelley. 2007. Reactions to dating violence among Latino teenagers: An experiment utilizing the Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations paradigm. Journal of Adolescence 30: 893–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Riekie, Helen, Jill M. Aldridge, and Ernest Afari. 2017. The Role of the School Climate in High School Students’ Mental Health and Identity Formation: A South Australian Study. British Educational Research Journal 43: 95–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sánchez-Conde, Paula, Ana Isabel Beltrán-Velasco, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2019. Influence of psychological profile in autonomic response of nursing students in their first hospital clinical stays. Physiology & Behavior 207: 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Storch, Eric A., Marla R. Brassard, and Carrie L. Masia-Warner. 2003. The Relationship of Peer Victimization to Social Anxiety and Loneliness in Adolescence. Child Study Journal 33: 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tornero-Aguilera, José Francisco, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2018. Effect of experience, equipment and fire actions in psychophysiological response and memory of soldiers in actual underground operations. International Journal of Psychophysiology 128: 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tornero-Aguilera, José Francisco, José Juan Robles-Pérez, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2020. Could Combat Stress Affect Journalists’ News Reporting? A Psychophysiological Response. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 45: 231–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Valdivia-Salas, Sonsoles, José Martín-Albo, Pablo Zaldivar, Andrés S. Lombas, and Teresa I. Jiménez. 2017. Spanish validation of the Avoidance and Fusion Questionnaire for Youth (AFQ-Y). Assessment 24: 919–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Vogel, Susanne, and Lars Schwabe. 2019. Stress, Aggression, and the Balance of Approach and Avoidance. Psychoneuroendocrinology 103: 137–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Wowra, Scott A. 2007. Moral Identities, Social Anxiety, and Academic Dishonesty Among American College Students. Ethics & Behavior 17: 303–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Wright, Mark, Robin Banerjee, Willemijn Hoek, Carolien Rieffe, and Sheida Novin. 2010. Depression and Social Anxiety in Children: Differential Links with Coping Strategies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 38: 405–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Zhao, Huanhuan, Heyun Zhang, and Yan Xu. 2019. Effects of Perceived Descriptive Norms on Corrupt Intention: The Mediating Role of Moral Disengagement: Descriptive norms facilitate corruption. International Journal of Psychology 54: 93–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Stimulus simulating academic cheating coercion at school. In each box, the offender’s character is on the right and the participant’s character on the left.
Figure 1. Stimulus simulating academic cheating coercion at school. In each box, the offender’s character is on the right and the participant’s character on the left.
Socsci 10 00265 g001
Table 1. Gender differences in the participant’s behavior when faced with coercion to commit fraud.
Table 1. Gender differences in the participant’s behavior when faced with coercion to commit fraud.
Behavior When Faced with Coercion to Commit Fraud
Gender AggressiveSocial SupportAssertiveAvoidancePassiveTotal
MenCount59.00 106.0 153.0 147.0 14.00 479.0
% within row12.3% 22.1% 31.9% 30.7% 2.9% 100.0%
% within column 54.6% 40.9% 44.6% 36.4% 42.4% 41.8%
Women Count 49.00 153.0 190.0 257.0 19.00 668.0
% within row7.3% 22.9% 28.4% 38.5% 2.8% 100.0%
% within column45.4% 59.1% 55.4% 63.6% 57.6% 58.2%
TotalCount 108.00 259.0 343.0 404.0 33.00 1147.0
% within row9.4% 22.6% 29.9% 35.2% 2.9% 100.0%
% within column100.0% 100.0%100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
Chi-Squared Tests
Valuedfp
Χ213.3740.010
N1147
Table 2. Gender differences in the participant’s and aggressor’s behavior when faced with coercion to commit fraud.
Table 2. Gender differences in the participant’s and aggressor’s behavior when faced with coercion to commit fraud.
Behavior When Faced with Coercion to Commit Fraud
Participant GenderAggressor Gender AggressiveSocial SupportAssertiveAvoidancePassiveTotal
MenMenCount 34.00 67.00 84.00 87.00 6.00 278.0
% within row 12.2% 24.1% 30.2% 31.3% 2.2% 100.0%
% within column 57.6% 63.2% 54.9% 59.2% 42.9% 58.0%
WomenCount 25.00 39.00 69.00 60.00 8.00 201.0
% within row 12.4% 19.4% 34.3% 29.9% 4.0% 100.0%
% within column 42.4% 36.8% 45.1% 40.8% 57.1% 42.0%
Total Count 59.00 106.00 153.00 147.00 14.00 479.0
% within row 12.3% 22.1% 31.9% 30.7% 2.9% 100.0%
% within column 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
WomenMenCount 29.00 82.00 77.00 121.00 5.00 314.0
% within row 9.2% 26.1% 24.5% 38.5% 1.6% 100.0%
% within column 59.2% 53.6% 40.5% 47.1% 26.3% 47.0%
WomenCount 20.00 71.00 113.00 136.00 14.00 354.0
% within row 5.6% 20.1% 31.9% 38.4% 4.0% 100.0%
% within column 40.8% 46.4% 59.5% 52.9% 73.7% 53.0%
Total Count 49.00 153.00 190.00 257.00 19.00 668.0
% within row 7.3% 22.9% 28.4% 38.5% 2.8% 100.0%
% within column 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
Total MenCount 63.00 149.00 161.00 208.00 11.00 592.0
% within row 10.6% 25.2% 27.2% 35.1% 1.9% 100.0%
% within column 58.3% 57.5% 46.9% 51.5% 33.3% 51.6%
Women Count 45.00 110.00 182.00 196.00 22.00 555.0
% within row 8.1% 19.8% 32.8% 35.3% 4.0% 100.0%
% within column 41.7% 42.5% 53.1% 48.5% 66.7% 48.4%
Total Count 108.00 259.00 343.00 404.00 33.00 1147.0
% within row 9.4% 22.6% 29.9% 35.2% 2.9% 100.0%
% within column 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
Chi-Squared Tests
Participant Gender Valuedfp
Men Χ2 3.189 40.527
N 479
Women Χ2 12.052 40.017
N 668
Table 3. Differences in aggressive behaviors depending on the state anxiety, trait anxiety, and psychological inflexibility of participants.
Table 3. Differences in aggressive behaviors depending on the state anxiety, trait anxiety, and psychological inflexibility of participants.
ScaleBehavior Mean SD N F p
State AnxietyAttack 28.202.941 137 5.342 0.021
No attack 28.77 2.675 1010
Trait AnxietyAttack 26.29 4.613 137 6.5130.011
No attack 25.26 4.430 1010
Psychological InflexibilityAttack 15.50 3.567 137 6.859 0.009
No attack 16.33 3.446 1010
Table 4. Differences in moral disengagement mechanisms and expectations of peers by trait anxiety and psychological inflexibility.
Table 4. Differences in moral disengagement mechanisms and expectations of peers by trait anxiety and psychological inflexibility.
Moral Disengagement Mechanism and Expectative about PeersMeanSDN Fp
Trait AnxietyMoral justificationNeutral26.05 4.7521703.8090.022
Absence25.22 4.366947
Presence26.67 5.34630
Distorting the consequencesNeutral24.83 4.1731578.203<0.001
Absence25.12 4.176687
Presence26.25 5.090303
Victim blamingNeutral25.00 4.0822836.3330.002
Absence26.95 4.94084
Presence25.35 4.511780
Expectative about peersNeutral25.40 4.3555758.386<0.001
Reject25.04 4.386474
Legitimize26.67 4.93795
Sanction35.00 5.2923
Psychological InflexibilityAdvantageous comparison Neutral16.29 3.5148233.2430.039
Absence16.64 2.830128
Presence15.71 3.614196
Expectative about peersNeutral16.21 3.3285753.3250.019
Reject16.35 3.621474
Legitimize15.95 3.40695
Sanction10.33 3.5123
Table 5. Nominal variables to evaluate moral disengagement mechanisms (Bandura) and violence legitimation from peers and adults.
Table 5. Nominal variables to evaluate moral disengagement mechanisms (Bandura) and violence legitimation from peers and adults.
VariableDimensionQuestionResponse Options
Moral disengagement mechanismsMoral justificationDo you think your reaction was?
  • Good
  • Regular
  • Bad
Euphemistic labelingWhat have you done?
  • To save your bacon
  • You don’t know
  • Acting in the wrong way
Advantageous comparisonIf someone else was in your position
  • He/She would act worse than you
  • You don’t know how He/She would act
  • He/She would act the same as you
Displacement of responsibilityWho started the problem?
  • He/She did
  • You don’t know
  • You did
Diffusion of responsibility If your friends find out about the incident and decide to confront him/her, who would be responsible for the situation?
  • No one, because it would happen in a group
  • You do not know
  • Everyone, including you
Distorting the consequencesDo you think you hurt your classmate?
  • No, you don’t think you have hurt him/her
  • You don’t know
  • Yes, you think you have hurt him/her
Victim blamingDo you think your classmate deserved what you did?
  • Yes, because He/She was disrespectful
  • You don’t know
  • No, He/She didn’t deserve it
DehumanizationThe classmate who wanted to cheat is:
  • A donkey
  • You don’t know
  • Irresponsible
Legitimation perceivedFrom peersWhat would your classmates do when they found out about your reaction
  • They would support you.
  • They wouldn’t say anything.
  • They would criticize you.
  • They would reject the situation.
From adultsRealizing the situation, what will the teacher do?
  • He/She will punish your classmate.
  • He/She will do nothing.
  • He/She will get you a warning.
  • He/She would reject the situation.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Martínez-González, M.B.; Arenas-Rivera, C.P.; Cardozo-Rusinque, A.A.; Morales-Cuadro, A.R.; Acuña-Rodríguez, M.; Turizo-Palencia, Y.; Clemente-Suárez, V.J. Psychological and Gender Differences in a Simulated Cheating Coercion Situation at School. Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 265. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10070265

AMA Style

Martínez-González MB, Arenas-Rivera CP, Cardozo-Rusinque AA, Morales-Cuadro AR, Acuña-Rodríguez M, Turizo-Palencia Y, Clemente-Suárez VJ. Psychological and Gender Differences in a Simulated Cheating Coercion Situation at School. Social Sciences. 2021; 10(7):265. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10070265

Chicago/Turabian Style

Martínez-González, Marina Begoña, Claudia Patricia Arenas-Rivera, Aura Alicia Cardozo-Rusinque, Aldair Ricardo Morales-Cuadro, Mónica Acuña-Rodríguez, Yamile Turizo-Palencia, and Vicente Javier Clemente-Suárez. 2021. "Psychological and Gender Differences in a Simulated Cheating Coercion Situation at School" Social Sciences 10, no. 7: 265. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10070265

APA Style

Martínez-González, M. B., Arenas-Rivera, C. P., Cardozo-Rusinque, A. A., Morales-Cuadro, A. R., Acuña-Rodríguez, M., Turizo-Palencia, Y., & Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2021). Psychological and Gender Differences in a Simulated Cheating Coercion Situation at School. Social Sciences, 10(7), 265. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10070265

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop