Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Generating the Relevant Questions
3. Humans as a Reference?
4. Human–Animal Companionship: Is It Social Abuse?
5. Vertebrate Brain Evolution as the Basis for a Common Social Mindset
6. The Common Social Toolbox
6.1. The “Conservative” Social Brain
6.2. The “Modern” Social Brain
6.3. Social Physiology
6.4. Common Principles of Behavioural Organisation
6.5. Anthropomorphising
6.6. Dogs Are Special—But How “Unique” Are They?
7. Discussion
7.1. The Social Environment as a Major Factor in Dog Welfare
7.2. What Kind of Relationships Support Dog Welfare?
7.3. Anthropomorphising and Mentalising: Is It OK?
8. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Serpell, J.A. In the Company of Animals; Basil Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, D.E. Human Universals; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Christakis, N.A. Blueprint. The Evolutionary Origins of a Good Society; Little, Brown Sparks: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- World Animal Foundation. Available online: https://worldanimalfoundation.org/dogs/ (accessed on 15 July 2023).
- Kotrschal, K. Mensch. Woher Wir Kommen, Wer Wir Sind, Wohin Wir Gehen; Brandstätter: Vienna, Austria, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Fromm, E. Die Seele des Menschen. Ihre Fähigkeit zum Guten und zum Bösen; Ullstein Hc: München, Germany, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis; Islands Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache, J.S.; Pickard, M.B.; LoBue, V. How very young children think about animals. In How Animals Affect Us: Examining the Influences of Human-Animal Interaction on Child Development and Human Health; McCune, S., Griffin, S.J.A., Maholmes, V., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2011; pp. 85–99. [Google Scholar]
- Wedl, M.; Kotrschal, K. Social and Individual components of animal contact in preschool children. Anthrozoös 2009, 22, 383–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kotrschal, K. Einfach Beste Freunde. Warum Menschen und Andere Tiere Einander Verstehen; Brandstätter: Vienna, Austria, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Kotrschal, K. Do companion animals support social, emotional and cognitive development of children? In The Social Neuroscience of Human-Animal Interaction; Mc Cune, S., Esposito, L., Gee, N.R., McCardle, P., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; pp. 73–86. [Google Scholar]
- Bar-On, Y.M.; Phililips, R.; Milo, R. The biomass distribution on Earth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 6506–6511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strassburg, B.B.N.; Iribarrem, A.; Beyer, H.L.; Cordeiro, C.L.; Crouzeilles, R.; Jakovac, C.C.; Braga Junqueira, A.; Lacerda, E.; Latawiec, A.E.; Balmford, A.; et al. Global priority areas for ecosystem restoration. Nature 2020, 586, 724–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Available online: https://worldanimalfoundation.org/advocate/how-many-cows-are-in-the-world/ (accessed on 15 July 2023).
- Tinbergen, N. On aims and methods of ethology. Z. Tierpsychol. 1963, 20, 410–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodson, J.L. The vertebrate social behavior network: Evolutionary themes and variations. Horm. Behav. 2005, 48, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Connell, L.A.; Hofmann, H.A. The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: A comparative synthesis. Comp. Neurol. 2011, 519, 3599–3639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Porges, S.W. The polyvagal theory: New insights into adaptive reactions of the autonomic nervous system. Clevel. Clin. J. Med. 2009, 76, 86–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DeVries, A.C.; Glasper, E.R.; Detillion, C.E. Social modulation of stress responses. Physiol. Behav. 2003, 79, 399–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grahn, P.; Ottosson, J.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. The oxytocinergic system as a mediator of anti-stress and instorative effects induced by nature: The calm and connection theory. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 617814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousn. Cogn. 2005, 14, 30–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, M.S. What behavioral abilities emerged at key milestones in human brain evolution? 13 hypotheses on the 600-million-year phylogenetic history of human intelligence. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 685853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bennett, M.S. A Brief History of Intelligence; Harper Collins: New York, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Güntürkün, O. The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005, 15, 686–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowlby, J. The bowlby-ainsworth attachment theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 1979, 2, 637–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hinde, R.A.; Stevenson-Hinde, J. Attachment: Biological, cultural and individual desiderata. Hum. Dev. 1990, 33, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clayton, N.S.; Griffiths, D.P.; Emery, N.J.; Dickinson, A. Elements of episodic-like memory in animals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2001, 356, 1483–1491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, T.A.; Fortin, N.J. The evolution of episodic memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 10379–10386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kotrschal, K. Emotions Are at the Core of Individual Social Performance. In Emotions of Animals and Humans: Comparative Perspectives; Watanabe, S., Kucvzaj, S.K., Eds.; The Science of the Mind; Springer: Tokyo, Japan, 2013; pp. 3–21. [Google Scholar]
- Kotrschal, K.; Scheiber, I.B.R.; Hirschenhauser, K. Individual Performance in Complex Social Systems: The Greylag Goose Example. In Animal Behaviour: An Evolutionary Perspective; Kappeler, P., Ed.; Springer: Heidelberg, Germany, 2021; pp. 121–148. [Google Scholar]
- Kordig, C.R. Self-Reference and Philosophy. Am. Philos. Q. 1983, 20, 207–216. [Google Scholar]
- Von Bartheld, C.S.; Bahney, J.; Herculano-Houzel, S. The search for true numbers of neurons and glial cells in the human brain: A review of 150 years of cell counting. J. Comp. Neurol. 2016, 524, 3865–3895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsuboi, M.; Husby, A.; Kotrschal, A.; Hayward, A.; Buechel, S.D.; Zidar, J.; Løvlie, H.; Kolm, N. Comparative support for the expensive tissue hypothesis: Big brains are correlated with smaller gut and greater parental investment in Lake Tanganyika cichlids. Evolution 2014, 69, 190–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lesch, R.; Kotrschal, K.; Kitchener, A.C.; Fitch, W.T.; Kotrschal, A. The expensive-tissue hypothesis may help explain brain-size reduction during domestication. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2022, 15, 190–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Louv, R. Last Child in the Woods: Saving Our Children from Nature-Deficit Disorder; Alonquin Books: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Headey, B.; Na, F.; Zheng, R. Pet Dogs Benefit Owners’ Health: A ‘Natural Experiment’ in China. Soc. Indicators Res. 2008, 87, 481–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Julius, H.; Beetz, A.; Kotrschal, K.; Turner, D.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. Attachment to Pets. In An Integrative View of Human-Animal Relationships with Implications for Therapeutic Practice; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Urquiza-Haas, E.G.; Kotrschal, K. The mind behind anthropomorphic thinking: Attribution of mental states to other species. Anim. Behav. 2015, 109, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Urquiza-Haas, E.G.; Kotrschal, K. Human-animal similarity and the imageability of mental state concepts for mentalizing animals. J. Cogn. Cult. 2022, 22, 220–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Striedter, G.F.; Northcutt, R.G. Brains through Time. A Natural History of Vertebrates; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Curley, J.P.; Keverne, E.B. Genes, brains and mammalian social bonds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005, 20, 561–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olkowicza, S.; Kocoureka, M.; Lucana, R.K.; Porteša, M.; Fitch, W.T.; Herculano-Houzel, S.; Nemec, P. Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 7255–7260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Güntürkün, O.; Bugnyar, T. Cognition without Cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 291–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Russell, B. Unpopular Essays; Simon & Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Adkins-Regan, E. Neuroendocrinology of social behavior. ILAR J. 2009, 50, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nesse, R.M.; Bhatnagar, S.; Ellis, B. Evolutionary origins and functions of the stress response system. In Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 95–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berns, G. How Dogs Love Us: A Neuroscientist and His Adopted Dog Decode the Canine Brain; New Harvest/Hughton Mifflin Harcourt: Boston, MA, USA; New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Marsha, N.; Scheelea, D.; Feinstein, J.S.; Gerhardtd, H.; Strange, S.; Maier, W.; Hurlemann, R. Oxytocin-enforced norm compliance reduces xenophobic outgroup rejection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 9314–9319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Handlin, L.; Hydbring-Sandberg, E.; Nilsson, A.; Ejdebäck, M.; Jansson, A.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K. Short-term interaction between dogs and their owners—Effects on osytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate—An exploratory study. Anthrozoös 2011, 24, 301–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, X.; Li, J.; Xie, F.; Chen, X.; Xu, W.; Hudson, N.W. The relationship aetween adult attachment and mental health: A meta-analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2022, 123, 1089–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beetz, A.; Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Julius, H.; Kotrschal, K. Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human-animal interactions: The possible role of oxytocin. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kotrschal, K. Hund und Mensch; Brandstätter: Vienna, Austria, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. Basic emotions. Handb. Cogn. Emot. 1999, 98, 45–60. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals; D. Appleton and Company: New York, NY, USA, 1897. [Google Scholar]
- Lorenz, K. Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung. Grundlagen der Ethologie; Springer: Vienna, Austria; New York, NY, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Tinbergen, N. The Study of Instinct; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1951. [Google Scholar]
- Levita, L.; Bois, C.; Healey, A.; Smyllie, E.; Papakonstantinou, E.; Hartley, T.; Lever, C. The Behavioural Inhibition System, anxiety and hippocampal volume in a non-clinical population. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 2014, 4, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Waal, F.C. Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes; John Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, ND, USA; London, UK, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Range, F.; Marshall-Pescini, S. Wolves and Dogs. Between Myth and Science; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Urban-Lurain, M.; Weinshank, D.J. I do and I understand” mastery model learning for a large non-major course. ACM SIGCSE Bull. 1999, 31, 150–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jablonka, E.; Lamb, M. Evolution in Four Dimensions; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amodio, D.M. Social cognition 2.0: An interactive memory systems account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 21–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Serpell, J.; Duffy, D.L.; Jagoe, J.A. Becoming a dog: Early experience and behaviour. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People, 2nd ed.; Serpell, J., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2017; pp. 93–117. [Google Scholar]
- Müller, C.A.; Schmitt, K.; Barber, A.L.; Huber, L. Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 601–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Range, F.; Horn, L.; Viranyi, Z.; Huber, L. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 340–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heberlein, M.T.E.; Turner, D.C.; Range, F.; Viranyi, Z. The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Anim. Behav. 2016, 122, 59–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heberlein, M.T.E.; Manser, M.B.; Turner, D.C. Deceptive-like behaviour in dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 511–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Topal, J.; Byrne, R.W.; Miklosi, A.; Csanyi, V. Reproducing human actions and action sequences: “Do as I Do!” in a dog. Anim. Cogn. 2006, 9, 355–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilkins, A.S.; Wrangham, R.W.; Fitch, W.T. The “Domestication syndrome” in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics 2014, 197, 795–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jardim-Messeder, D.; Lambert, K.; Noctor, S.; Pestana, F.M.; de Castro Leal, M.E.; Bertelsen, M.F.; Alagaili, A.N.; Mohammad, O.B.; Manger, P.R.; Herculano-Houzel, S. Dogs have the most neurons, though not the largest brain: Trade-off between body mass and number of neurons in the cerebral cortex of large carnivoran species. Front. Neuroanat. 2017, 11, 118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mariti, C.; Pierantoni, L.; Sighieri, C.; Gazzano, A. Guardians’ perceptions of dogs’ Welfare and behaviors related to visiting the veterinary clinic. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2017, 20, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wascher, C.A.F.; Scheiber, I.B.R.; Kotrschal, K. Heart rate modulation in bystanding geese watching social and non-social events. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 275, 1653–1659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunter, L.M.; Feuerbacher, E.N.; Gilchrist, R.J.; Wynne, C.D. Evaluating the effects of a temporary fostering program on shelter dog welfare. Peer J. 2019, 7, e6620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hennessy, M.B.; Davis, H.N.; Williams, M.T.; Mellott, C.; Douglas, C.W. Plasma cortisol levels of dogs at a county animal shelter. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 62, 485–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schleidt, W. How “Fixed” is the Fixed Action Pattern? Z. Tierpsychol. 1974, 36, 184–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. Grundriss der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung; Piper: München, Germany; Zürich, Switzerland, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Von Uexküll, J. Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1909. [Google Scholar]
- Shipman, P. The Invaders. How Humans and Their Dogs Drove Neanderthals to Extinction; Belknap Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Kotrschal, K. How wolves turned into dogs and how dogs are valuable in meeting human social needs. People Anim. Int. J. Res. Pract. 2018, 1, 6. [Google Scholar]
- Kotrschal, K. Der Wolf und Wir. Wie Aus Ihm Unser Erstes Haustier Wurde—und Warum Seine Rückkehr Chancen Bietet; Brandstätter: Vienna, Austria, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Belyaev, D.K. Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. Heredity 1972, 70, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frantz, L.A.F.; Mullin, V.E.; Pionnier-Capitan, M.; Lebrasseur, O.; Ollivier, M.; Perri, A.; Linderholm, A.; Mattiangeli, V.; Teasdale, M.D.; Dimopoulos, E.A.; et al. Genomic and archaeological evidence suggest a dual origin of domestic dogs. Science 2016, 352, 1228–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, B.; Woods, V. The Genius of Dogs: Discovering the Unique Intelligence of Man’s Best Friend; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/448169/deadliest-creatures-in-the-world-by-number-of-human-deaths/ (accessed on 20 June 2023).
- Mech, L.D.; Boitani, L. (Eds.) Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA; London, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Scott, J.P.; Fuller, J.L. The Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA; London, UK, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Jean-Joseph, H.; Kortekaas, K.; Range, F.; Kotrschal, K. Context-specific arousal during resting in wolves and dogs: Effects of domestication? Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 568199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jean-Joseph, H.; Dooey, G.; Kotrschal, K. Diurnal activity patterns of equally socialized and kept wolves, Canis lupus, and dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Anim. Behav. 2022, 190, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Available online: https://sites.google.com/view/doglabiiserkolkata/home (accessed on 20 June 2023).
- Available online: https://www.vetmeduni.ac.at/klivv/forschung/projekte/free-ranging-dogs (accessed on 20 June 2023).
- Available online: https://www.wolfscience.at (accessed on 25 July 2023).
- Vasconcellos, A.S.; Virányi, Z.; Range, F.; Ades, C.; Scheidegger, J.K.; Möstl, E.; Kotrschal, K. Training reduces stress in human-socialised wolves to the same degree as in dogs. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0162389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunbar, R.I.M. Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 2003, 32, 163–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Range, F.; Virányi, Z. Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: The “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis”. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5, 1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cordoni, G.; Palagi, E. Back to the future: A glance over wolf social behavior to understand dog-human relationship. Animals 2019, 9, 991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kotrschal, K. Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools. Animals 2023, 13, 2729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172729
Kotrschal K. Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools. Animals. 2023; 13(17):2729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172729
Chicago/Turabian StyleKotrschal, Kurt. 2023. "Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools" Animals 13, no. 17: 2729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172729
APA StyleKotrschal, K. (2023). Wolf–Dog–Human: Companionship Based on Common Social Tools. Animals, 13(17), 2729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13172729