Effects of Digitalized Front-of-Package Food Labels on Healthy Food-Related Behavior: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Eligibility Criteria
3.2. Search Strategy
3.3. Selection Process
3.4. Data Collection Process
3.5. Synthesis of Results
3.6. Study Risk of Bias Assessment
4. Results
4.1. Study Selection
4.2. Study Characteristics
4.3. The Effects of Physical and Digitalized FOP Food Labels
4.4. Risk of Bias in Articles
5. Discussion
5.1. General Interpretation
5.2. Limitation of Evidence Based on Articles and Review
5.3. Implications and Further Research
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Article | Year of Publication | Author(s) | Number of Observations | Unit of Analysis | Percentage Female Participants | Research Design | Controlled or Field Setting | Dependent Variable(s) | Independent Variable(s) | Comparison of Data Method | Univariate or Multivariate Independent Variable(s) |
1 | 2019 | Finkelstein et al. [48] | 147 | Participants | 68.8% | Within-participant design | Field setting | Purchase | Multiple traffic light label, Nutri-score label | First difference model | Multivariate independent variables |
2 | 2019 | Reyes et al. [49] | Study 1: 600 Study 2: 700 | Participants | 100% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | 15 warning labels | Study 1: ANOVA, Bonferroni Study 2: Chi-square, t-test | Multivariate independent variables |
3 | 2020 | Finkelstein et al. [50] | 146 | Participants | 78.8% | Within-participant design | Field setting | Purchase, self-report | “Lower calorie” labeling on within-category products, “Lower calorie” labeling on across category products | First difference approach | Multivariate independent variables |
4 | 2014 | Koenigstorfer et al. [51] | 152 | Participants | 63.5% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Health mark label, traffic lights color-coding label | Two-factorial ANOVA | Multivariate independent variables |
5 | 2019 | Siegrist et al. [52] | 780 | Participants | 51.5% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Healthy choice label, nutritional information label | Kruskal–Wallis test, Mann–Whitney U test | Multivariate independent variables |
6 | 2021 | Miklavec et al. [53] | 1000 | Participants | 49% | Within-participant design, between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report, hypothetical choice | Type of product, brand name with labels, health claim | Not specified for conjoint analysis, t-test | Multivariate independent variables |
7 | 2020 | Alcantara et al. [54] | 1232 | Participants | 52% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Health logo, nutritional warning label | Generalized linear model, Tukey’s test | Multivariate independent variables |
8 | 2020 | Deliza et al. [55] | Study 2: 1932 | Participants | 51% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | Guideline daily amount, traffic light system, black magnifier, red magnifier, red circle, black triangle, and black octagon label | ANOVA, generalized linear model, t-test | Multivariate independent variables |
9 | 2019 | Lima et al. [56] | 141 | Participants | 60% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Types of dairy products, traffic light system, familiar brands | ANOVA, Tukey’s test, exploratory factor analysis, parallel analysis, maximum likelihood estimation, pro max | Multivariate independent variables |
10 | 2011 | Vyth et al. [57] | Study 1: 25 Study 2: 368 | Study 1: Cafeterias Study 2: Participants | Not collected | Within-participant design, between-participant design | Field setting | Purchase, self-report | Choices logo on sandwiches, choices logo on soups, sandwiches, and soups | Generalized estimation equation analysis, t-test | Multivariate independent variables |
11 | 2019 | Machin et al. [58] | 199 | Participants | 66% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report, hypothetical choice | Calorie, added sugar, total fat, saturated fat, and sodium-based warning labels | Chi-square test, t-test | Multivariate independent variables |
12 | 2018 | Kim et al. [59] | 95 | Participants | 54.7% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Numeric, color-coded, and physical activity-based labels | Chi-square test, one-way ANOVA, Tukey test | Multivariate independent variables |
13 | 2015 | Antunez et al. [60] | 54 | Participants | 53% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Color without text, color with text, monochromatic without text, monochromatic with text | ANOVA | Multivariate independent variables |
14 | 2020 | Blitstein et al. [61] | 1452 | Participants | 83.5% | Between-participant design, within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | Summary label, nutrient-specific label, hybrid label, 10-min time limit to shop | Tukey–Kramer adjustment, ANCOVA, | Multivariate independent variables |
15 | 2020 | Gustafson & Zeballos [62] | 633 | Participants | 53% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Calorie information for each ingredient, calorie information relative to highest calorie item, calorie information relative to lowest calorie item | T-test, linear regression analysis, chi-square tests, Bonferroni Correction | Multivariate independent variables |
16 | 2020 | Hagmann & Siegrist [63] | 1313 | Participants | 53.9% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | Nutritional information label, multiple traffic light, Nutri-score, Nutri-score on half of the products | Welch’s analysis of variance (ANOVA), Games-Howell post hoc test, Kruskal–Wallis test, Pearson’s r, one-way ANOVA, exploratory t-tests independent samples | Multivariate independent variables |
17 | 2018 | Menger-Ogle & Graham [64] | 239 | Participants | 59% | Within-participant design | Field setting | Self-report | “Low fat”, “> 100% RDA of vitamin C”, “35% RDA of vitamin A”, “Low fat, low sugar, transfat free”, “zero cholesterol, zero trans fat, gluten-free, MSG-free” labels | Mediational model, t-test, | Multivariate independent variables |
18 | 2019 | Vizcaino & Velasco [65] | Study 1: 133 Study 2: 837 Study 3: 181 Study 4: 201 | Participants | Study 1: 38%Study 2: 53%Study 3: 46.7% Study 4: 71.1% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Traffic light labels, brand familiarity | ANOVA, mediation analysis, multiple moderation regression, moderated-mediation analysis | Multivariate independent variables |
19 | 2020 | Gabor et al. [66] | 76 | Participants | 55% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Nutri-score, multiple traffic lights, guideline daily amount | ANOVA, Tukey’s test | Multivariate independent variables |
20 | 2021 | Finkelstein et al. [67] | 106 | Participants | 66% | Within-participant design | Field setting | Purchase | Healthier choice, physical activity equivalent, and nutritional information label | First-differenced regressions | Multivariate independent variables |
21 | 2021 | Fagerstrøm et al. [68] | 30 | Participants | 46.7% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice | Correspondence of healthy food labels and nutritional information | Share of correct choice | Univariate independent variable |
22 | 2021 | Folkvord et al. [69] | 192 | Participants | 63% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Nutri-score | Chi-square test, MANOVA, Bayesian ANCOVA | Univariate independent variable |
23 | 2018 | Lima et al. [70] | Sample 1: 318Sample 2: 278 | Participants | Sample 1: 83% Sample 2: 49% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Daily guideline amounts, traffic light systems, and warning systems | ANOVA, Tukey’s test | Multivariate independent variables |
24 | 2017 | Yoo et al. [71] | 646 | Participants | 56% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Dairy product, sugar reduction claim, traffic light system | ANOVA, generalized linear model | Multivariate independent variables |
25 | 2020 | Shin et al. [15] | 125 | Participants | 72% | Between-participant design | Field setting | Purchase, self-report | Nutri-score, physical activity equivalents, calorie, sugar, sodium, saturated fat, and total fat per serving and percentage daily recommended intake dynamic with real-time feedback labels | First difference regression | Multivariate independent variables |
26 | 2018 | Acton et al. [33] | 675 | Participants | 53.9% | Between-participant design | Field setting | Self-report | Numeric rating, health star rating, traffic light symbol | Chi-square test, logistic regression model | Multivariate independent variables |
27 | 2020 | Rojas-Rivas et al. [72] | 498 | Participants | 73% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report | Sodium warning, type of bread, brand, price | Exploratory factor analyses, maximum likelihood and Promax rotation method, mixed logit model | Multivariate independent variables |
28 | 2021 | Yang et al. [73] | 1215 | Participants | 62.2% | Within-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | Health label, low-carbon label, the proportion of brown rice, pre-treatment, price | Mixed logit model, | Multivariate independent variables |
29 | 2021 | Mauri et al. [74] | Study 1: 200 Study 2: 272 | Participants | Study 1: 66.8% Study 2: 71.7% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Hypothetical choice, self-report | Sugar teaspoons, traffic lights, | ANOVA, Mediation testing | Multivariate independent variables |
30 | 2020 | Jin et al. [75] | Study 1: 123 Study 2: 144 Study 3: 241 | Participants | Study 1: 51.2% Study 2: 49.3% Study 3: 46.9% | Between-participant design | Controlled setting | Self-report, consumption | Physical activity equivalent to calorie label | Moderated regression analysis | Multivariate independent variables |
Article | Year of Publication | Author(s) | Physical or Digitalized FOP Food Label | Static, Interactive, or Technology-Enabled | Type of FOP Food Label |
1 | 2019 | Finkelstein et al. [48] | Digitalized | Interactive | Graded nutrient-specific label, graded summary label |
2 | 2019 | Reyes et al. [49] | Physical | Singe nutrient-specific label | |
3 | 2020 | Finkelstein et al. [50] | Digitalized | Interactive | Singe nutrient-specific label |
4 | 2014 | Koenigstorfer et al. [51] | Physical | Graded nutrient-specific label, single summary label, combined label | |
5 | 2019 | Siegrist et al. [52] | Digitalized | Interactive | Single summary label |
6 | 2021 | Miklavec et al. [53] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label |
7 | 2020 | Alcantara et al. [54] | Digitalized | Static | Single nutrient-specific label |
8 | 2020 | Deliza et al. [55] | Digitalized | Static | Percentage nutrient-specific label, graded nutrient-specific label, single nutrient-specific labels |
9 | 2019 | Lima et al. [56] | Physical | Graded nutrient-specific label | |
10 | 2011 | Vyth et al. [57] | Physical | Single summary label | |
11 | 2019 | Machin et al. [58] | Physical | Single nutrient-specific label | |
12 | 2018 | Kim et al. [59] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label |
13 | 2015 | Antunez et al. [60] | Digitalized | Static | Percentage nutrient-specific label, graded nutrient-specific label |
14 | 2020 | Blitstein et al. [61] | Digitalized | Interactive | Graded summary label, graded nutrient-specific label, combined label |
15 | 2020 | Gustafson & Zeballos [62] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label, percentage nutrient-specific label |
16 | 2020 | Hagmann & Siegrist [63] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label, graded summary label |
17 | 2018 | Menger-Ogle & Graham [64] | Digitalized | Static | Single nutrient-specific label |
18 | 2019 | Vizcaino & Velasco [65] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label, single nutrient-specific label |
19 | 2020 | Gabor et al. [66] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label, graded summary label, percentage nutrient-specific label |
20 | 2021 | Finkelstein et al. [67] | Digitalized | Interactive | Single nutrient-specific label, graded nutrient-specific label |
21 | 2021 | Fagerstrøm et al. [68] | Digitalized | Static | Single summary label |
22 | 2021 | Folkvord et al. [69] | Digitalized | Static | Graded summary label |
23 | 2018 | Lima et al. [70] | Digitalized | Static | Percentage nutrient-specific label, graded nutrient-specific label, single nutrient-specific labels |
24 | 2017 | Yoo et al. [71] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label |
25 | 2020 | Shin et al. [15] | Digitalized | Technology-enabled | Graded summary label, graded nutrient-specific label |
26 | 2018 | Acton et al. [33] | Physical | Graded summary label | |
27 | 2020 | Rojas-Rivas et al. [72] | Digitalized | Static | Single nutrient-specific label |
28 | 2021 | Yang et al. [73] | Digitalized | Static | Single summary label |
29 | 2021 | Mauri et al. [74] | Digitalized | Static | Graded nutrient-specific label |
30 | 2020 | Jin et al. [75] | Physical | Graded nutrient-specific label |
Article | Year of Publication | Author(s) | Findings |
1 | 2019 | Finkelstein et al. [48] | Consumers shopped at online grocery stores and were exposed to (a) Multiple traffic light labels, (b) Nutri-score labels, or (c) no FOP food labels conditions. The results show that both labels increased the purchase of healthy products with no difference between labels. In contrast, the Nutri-score label increased average Nutri-score selection higher than multiple traffic light labels and no label. |
2 | 2019 | Reyes et al. [49] | Study 1: Participants were instructed to select between 2 out of 15 yogurts with different warning labels and asked to rate visibility, understanding, intend purchase score, ability to modify intend to purchase, and socio-demographical information. The results show that five distinct labels were associated with greater visualization, intended purchase score, and ability to modify intended purchase. Study 2: New participants were instructed to select one out of two warning labels which were based on the five warning labels in a previous study, and used the same measurement as the previous study. The results show that the stop sign label was associated with a greater effect on all ratings. |
3 | 2020 | Finkelstein et al. [50] | Consumers shopped at online grocery stores and were exposed to (a) “Lower calorie” label on 20% of foods within a product category, (b) “Lower calorie” label on 20% of all foods, or (c) no FOP food labels conditions. The results show that within-category labeling increased purchase of labeled foods compared to no label, no differences in within- and across- category labeling, and no evidence of a decrease in calories purchased in the latter. |
4 | 2014 | Koenigstorfer et al. [51] | Participants were randomly assigned to either (a) health mark and traffic light labels present, (b) health mark present and traffic light absent, (c) health mark absent and traffic light present, and (d) no labels. The results show that participants exposed to both labels selected healthier foods compared to other conditions. |
5 | 2019 | Siegrist et al. [52] | Participants were randomly assigned to (a) combined healthy choice and nutritional information label, (b) healthy choice label, (c) nutritional information label, and (d) no label condition; they were presented pairs of food and instructed to select the healthiest product. Healthy choice labels were presented on healthier options and nutritional information labels on both options. The results show that the nutritional information label slightly improved healthy choices, combined labels produced similar results, and that healthy choice labels alone and no label produced similar results. |
6 | 2021 | Miklavec et al. [53] | Participants were presented with fictive products with different combinations of types of products (soft drink, yogurt, and chocolate), brand name (three neutral circles, one heart, or three hearts), and claim (no claim, general claim, specific claim), and asked to rate how healthy the product was. Later, participants were assigned in (a) a heart symbol label condition and (b) no heart symbol label condition, presented for four existing brands of water bottles where one of the products in the former condition had a heart symbol, and instructed to rate how healthy the product was compared to tap water. The results show that the type of product, claims, and brand name had the highest impact in that order, and that the water bottle with heart symbols had the highest impact on the rating. |
7 | 2020 | Alcantara et al. [54] | Participants were allocated to either (a) health logo condition, (b) nutritional warning condition, and (c) no label condition, presented with three products each from a different category, and instructed to select which one they would like to buy. The results show that nutritional warnings were more effective than health logos in increasing product selection in all product categories, although both increased product selection compared to the no-label condition. |
8 | 2020 | Deliza et al. [55] | Participants were allocated to either (a) guideline daily amount, (b) traffic-light system, (c) black magnifier, (d) red magnifier, (e) red circle, (f) black triangle, and (g) black octagon label condition, and were presented with three products each having a different label and product category. Half of the participants were instructed to select the healthy product while the other half selected the unhealthy product. Later, participants were instructed to rate how healthy the product was. The results show that traffic-light, red circle, black triangle, black octagon, black magnifier, and guideline daily amount labels from, most to least in that order, were associated with healthier responses. Guideline daily amounts were associated with higher ratings of healthfulness than warning labels. |
9 | 2019 | Lima et al. [56] | Participants were presented with food products with different combinations of dairy products (yogurt, cheese, and chocolate-flavored milk), traffic light system (yes vs. no), and brand (well-known vs. unknown), were presented one product at a time, and instructed to rate how healthy the product was. The result shows the relative impact on ratings, from most to least, were the type of dairy product, traffic light system, and brand. The yogurt with the traffic light system and a familiar brand was on average, rated as healthier. |
10 | 2011 | Vyth et al. [57] | Consumers in 25 different cafeterias were exposed for cycles of nine weeks of (a) baseline condition with no Choices logo, (b) Choices logo on sandwiches, soups, and fresh fruits, and (c) postintervention period without the label, each condition lasting three weeks. Cycles were repeated three times. Consumers’ purchases and employees near the cafeterias self-reports regarding attitudes, self-efficacy, intention, and whether they used the logo, were measured. The intervention did not significantly affect employees’ lunchtime food choices. However, the results show that fruits sales were higher in the logo condition compared to its absence, while purchases of sandwiches and soups were similar across the conditions. |
11 | 2019 | Machin et al. [58] | Participants were allocated to (a) warning label condition or (b) no label condition; they were exposed to 15 snack products based on six food categories (fruit, alfajor, cereal bar, cracker, cookies, and peanuts; and were instructed to select a snack they would like to consume. The results show that participants allocated to the warning label condition selected fewer products that were excessive in at least one nutrient than participants in the no label condition. |
12 | 2018 | Kim et al. [59] | Participants were allocated to (a) color-coded calorie label condition, (b) physical activity-based condition, or (c) numeric calorie label condition; they were instructed to choose one burger/sandwich, snack/sides, and beverage; each category had six options. The results show that participants being exposed to the physical activity-based label, color-coded label, and numeric label led from most to least, to fewer calories selected. |
13 | 2015 | Antunez et al. [60] | Participants were exposed to a series of three products with a combination of the type of label (color or no color; text or no text) and a number of excess nutrients (all medium; one excessive nutrient content) and were asked to indicate which of three labels were low-fat and classify which product had lowest salt content. The results show that the percentage of correct responses was higher during color-coded than monochromatic labels for low-fat ratings but not for low salt ratings. |
14 | 2020 | Blitstein et al. [61] | Participants were allocated to (a) summary label condition, (b) nutrient-specific label condition, (c) hybrid label condition, or (d) no label condition; participants had either a time constraint of 10 min for shopping or were without such time constraints; and were instructed to select the six healthiest products for their family. The results show that the participants exposed to summary or hybrid labels made healthier choices than those exposed to nutrient-specific labels. The time constraint led to less healthier choices than no time constraint for participants exposed to summary or hybrid labels but not for nutrient-specific labels. |
15 | 2020 | Gustafson & Zeballos [62] | Participants were allocated to (a) calorie information condition, (b) calorie information relative to highest calorie item, (c) calorie information relative to lowest calorie item, and (d) no label condition; they were instructed to select which items they would like for constructing a fictive sandwich. The results show that participants exposed to relative calorie labels had fewer selected calories than those with no label. There were no significant differences between the calorie label and no label condition. |
16 | 2020 | Hagmann & Siegrist [63] | Participants were allocated to (a) nutritional information condition, (b) multiple traffic light condition, (c) Nutri-score condition, (d) Nutri-score on half of products condition, and (e) no label condition; they were presented with two products and instructed to select the healthiest option. The results show that the proportion of correct choices was higher for participants exposed to the Nutri-score label than respondents in other conditions. |
17 | 2018 | Menger-Ogle & Graham [64] | Participants were exposed to two of four possible food products with labels, instructed to rate how likely they were to purchase the product, the influence of such messages on their own purchase behavior, the healthfulness of the product, and the truthfulness of the message being presented. The results based on mediation models show that labels such as “healthful for children” influenced product perceptions while labels like “tasty” influenced purchase intention. |
18 | 2019 | Vizcaino & Velasco [65] | Study 1: Participants were allocated in either (a) traffic light condition or (b) no label condition, were presented a yogurt, and asked to rate how likely they were to purchase. Study 2: Participants were allocated in either (a) familiar brand and traffic light label, (b) unfamiliar brand and label, (c) familiar brand and no label, and (d) unfamiliar brand and no label, and were instructed to rate the trustworthiness of the product. Study 3: Used the same procedure as the second study but used different food categories.Study 4: Used the same procedure as study 2, used different foods and performed a moderation-mediation model.The results of these studies show a higher degree of purchase ratings when exposed to traffic lights, that familiar brands with traffic lights did produce interaction effects, and that brand trust may mediate the interaction of traffic lights and brand familiarity. |
19 | 2020 | Gabor et al. [66] | Participants were allocated to (a) multiple traffic light conditions, (b) Nutri-score condition, and (c) guideline daily amounts label condition; presented food products, instructed to rate how healthy the product is and how often they consumed such products. The results show that Nutri-score, multiple traffic lights, and guideline daily amounts condition produced high to low ratings of healthfulness and consumption frequency in that order. |
20 | 2021 | Finkelstein et al. [67] | Consumers shopped at an online grocery store and were exposed to (a) healthier choice label condition, (b) healthier choice and physical activity equivalent labels condition, and (c) no label condition; while their purchases were recorded and used to derive total calories, Grocery Purchase Quality Index 2016, weighted average Nutri-scores, sugar, sodium, saturated fat, and calories per dollar spent. The results show that healthier choices increased purchases of labeled products. In contrast, healthier choice labels combined with physical activity equivalents did not lead to a greater increase in healthy food purchases. |
21 | 2021 | Fagerstrøm et al. [68] | Participants were exposed to (a) correspondence between healthy food labels and nutritional information labels, (b) non-correspondence between healthy food labels, (c) healthy food labels on both products, and (d) no label conditions; presented with two food products with their respective nutritional information, and instructed to select the healthier product. The results show that approximately two-thirds of the participants chose the healthier option when there was a correspondence. Approximately one-third of participants continued to select the healthier option when there was non-correspondence. |
22 | 2021 | Folkvord et al. [69] | Participants were allocated in either (a) Nutri-score condition or (b) no label condition; they were instructed to rate how appealing the product looked, how tasty the product looked, and how likely they were to purchase the product. The results show that the participants’ ratings of appeal, ratings of tastiness, and purchase intention did not differ between the groups. |
23 | 2018 | Lima et al. [70] | Participants (parents and children) were allocated to (a) guideline daily amounts, (b) traffic light systems, or (c) warning system conditions; and instructed to rate how healthy the product was and how often they consume such products. The results show that ratings of healthfulness were lower for participants in the traffic light and warning systems condition, than in the guideline daily system. Based on the parents, the guideline daily amount system was associated with higher healthfulness scores, and warning labels had more impact on ratings than other labels, while the labels influenced children less. |
24 | 2017 | Yoo et al. [71] | Participants were exposed to several fictive products with different combinations of (a) dairy products (yogurt, chocolate-flavored milk, and vanilla milk dessert), (b) sugar reduction claim (present or absent), and (c) traffic light system (present or absent); presented with the products one at a time and instructed to rate how much they would like the product. The results show that participants were influenced by product type, sugar reduction claim, and traffic light system from most to least in that order. |
25 | 2020 | Shin et al. [15] | Consumers shopped in an online grocery store and were exposed to either (a) dynamic food labels with real-time feedback based on selected products in a virtual basket or (b) no food label condition; consumers’ purchases were used to derive weighted average Nutri-score per serving, total calories and sugar purchased, calories per dollar purchased, and average servings of calories, sugar, sodium, total fat, and saturated fat. The results show that the average Nutri-score was higher, and all other measures were lower for participants in the label condition than the participants in the no label condition. |
26 | 2018 | Acton et al. [33] | Participants were allocated to (a) numeric rating food label, (b) health star rating, (c) simplified traffic light symbol or, (d) no label condition; presented with three beverages with varying degrees of healthiness, all of them which had the label corresponding to the condition; and were instructed to rate the healthiness of the product. The results show that participants exposed to health star rating were more likely to select moderately healthy for moderately healthy beverages, than when exposed to other labels. |
27 | 2020 | Rojas-Rivas et al. [72] | Participants were exposed to pairs of products with different combinations of type of bread (white or whole wheat), brand (unknown or known), sodium warning (present or absent), and price (75, 85, or 100USD); and instructed to select between the two products or a “none of these breads” option. The results show that participants were influenced by sodium warning, brand, and type of bread from most to least in that order, although the type of bread was not statistically significant. |
28 | 2021 | Yang et al. [73] | Participants were exposed to two food products with different combinations of (a) types of health labels, (b) types of low-carbon labels, (c) proportions of brown to white rice, (d) cooking method, and (e) price; and were instructed to select which one they prefer. The results show that health and low-carbon labels in the form of symbols were associated with a higher willingness to pay than brief text claims about health or low-carbon. All labels had higher impact on choice than cooking method but lower than the proportion of brown to white rice. |
29 | 2021 | Mauri et al. [74] | Study 1: Participants were exposed to two foods with (a) two or six sugar teaspoons labels and (b) red or green traffic light labels and were instructed to select the product that best reflects their preference. Study 2: Participants were allocated in either (a) traffic light label condition, (b) sugar teaspoons label condition, or (c) no label condition; they were instructed to select one of three products that they would like to buy. After the selection, the ingredients of the products with different degrees of simplicity were shown. Participants were instructed to rate their preference for labels, ingredient information, and degree of the healthiness of the product. These studies show that labels had a small increase in healthier food selection. Participants exposed to sugar teaspoon labels chose, on average, a higher proportions of healthy foods than participants exposed to traffic light labels. In addition, the effects of sugar teaspoons are also impacted by food category, and ingredient composition influences ratings of healthiness. |
30 | 2020 | Jin et al. [75] | Study 1: Participants were allocated to either (a) physical activity equivalent calorie label condition or (b) no label condition; instructed to rate how hungry they were, exposed to a food product, instructed to consume the product, rate how much they liked the product and their dieting tendencies, and to run on a treadmill for as long or as intensely as they chose. Study 2: Participants were allocated similarly to in study 1, used another food product, were instructed to complete a lexical decision task consisting of non-words, neutral words unrelated to energy balance, target energy balance-related words, and were later instructed to run on a treadmill as in study 1. Study 3: Participants were allocated to (a) presence of label and energy-balance tasks, (b) presence of label and absence of energy-balance task, (c) absence of label and presence of energy-balance task, or (d) absence of label and energy-balance task conditions; instructed to conduct a language test, to construct a four-word sentence and, similarly as to previous studies, taste food and run on a treadmill. These studies show that participants with high dietary tendencies consumed fewer calories and burned more calories in the treadmill condition compared to non-dieters, that such effects were greater for dieters compared to non-dieters, that such labels affect response time to energy balance-related words in a higher degree to dieters compared to non-dieters, and that dieters in the presence of label and absence of energy-balance conditions had higher energy expenditures compared to absent of energy-balance condition. |
Physical | Digitalized | |||
Dependent Variable Was Affected by the FOP Format | Static | Interactive | Technology-Enabled | |
Full | 83.3% (4, 9, 11, 26, 30) | 83.3% (6, 7, 12, 15, 16, 18, 24, 27, 28, 29) | 60% (1, 3, 14) | 100% (25) |
Partial | 16.7% (10) | 16.7% (17, 22) | 20% (20) | 0% |
No | 0% | 0% | 20% (5) | 0% |
Total number of articles | 6 | 12 | 5 | 1 |
Before Study | During Study | After Study | ||||||
Article | Study | Randomization Process | Deviations from Intended Interventions | Missing Outcome Data | Measurement of Outcome | Reporting of Results | Carryover or Timing Effects | Overall Risk of Bias |
1 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | |
3 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Low | Moderate | |
4 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | ||
5 | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | High | ||
6 | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | High | ||
7 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | ||
8 | Study 2 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | |
10 | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High | |
11 | Low | High | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High | ||
12 | Moderate | Moderate | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High | |
14 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | ||
15 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | ||
16 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | ||
18 | Study 2, 3, and 4 | Moderate | High | Low | Low | Moderate | High | |
19 | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | High | ||
20 | Low | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | |
22 | Moderate | High | Low | Low | Moderate | High | ||
23 | Moderate | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High | ||
25 | Low | Low | Low | High | Moderate | Low | High | |
26 | Moderate | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High | ||
29 | Study 2 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | High |
30 | Study 1, 2, and 3 | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate |
Article ID | Study | Temporal Order of Independent Variable and Effect | Participant Characteristics across Groups | Procedure for Interventions | Control Condition | Multiple Measurements of Outcome Pre- and Post-Intervention | Missing Data | Measurement of Outcome | Reliability of Outcome | Appropriate Analysis | Overall Risk |
2 | Study 1 and 2 | Low | Low | Low | High | High | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | High |
9 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | High | |
13 | Study 2 | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | High |
17 | Low | Low | Low | Low | High | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | High | |
18 | Study 1 | Low | Moderate | Low | Low | High | Low | Low | Low | Low | High |
21 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Moderate | |
24 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | |
27 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | |
28 | Low | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | Low | Low | Low | Moderate | High |
References
- Chooi, Y.C.; Ding, C.; Magkos, F. The epidemiology of obesity. Metabolism 2019, 92, 6–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poelman, M.; Strak, M.; Schmitz, O.; Hoek, G.; Karssenberg, D.; Helbich, M.; Ntarladima, A.-M.; Bots, M.; Brunekreef, B.; Grobbee, R. Relations between the residential fast-food environment and the individual risk of cardiovascular diseases in The Netherlands: A nationwide follow-up study. Eur. J. Prev. Cardiol. 2018, 25, 1397–1405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alaimo, K.; Olson, C.M.; Frongillo, E.A. Family food insufficiency, but not low family income, is positively associated with dysthymia and suicide symptoms in adolescents. J. Nutr. 2002, 132, 719–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Obesity. All countries off track to meet 2025 WHO targets on obesity. 2020. Available online: https://cdn.easo.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/03053339/World-Obesity-Day-Template-Press-Release-2020-FINAL-SB-edit-.pdf (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Campos, S.; Doxey, J.; Hammond, D. Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: A systematic review. Public health nutrition 2011, 14, 1496–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- World Health Organization. Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- An, R.; Shi, Y.; Shen, J.; Bullard, T.; Liu, G.; Yang, Q.; Chen, N.; Cao, L. Effect of front-of-package nutrition labeling on food purchases: A systematic review. Public Health 2021, 191, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, J.; Brown, M.K.; Tan, M.; MacGregor, G.A.; Webster, J.; Campbell, N.R.; Trieu, K.; Ni Mhurchu, C.; Cobb, L.K.; He, F.J. Impact of color-coded and warning nutrition labelling schemes: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2021, 18, e1003765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Temple, N.J. Front-of-package food labels: A narrative review. Appetite 2020, 144, 104485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ikonen, I.; Sotgiu, F.; Aydinli, A.; Verlegh, P.W. Consumer effects of front-of-package nutrition labeling: An interdisciplinary meta-analysis. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2020, 48, 360–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanjari, S.S.; Jahn, S.; Boztug, Y. Dual-process theory and consumer response to front-of-package nutrition label formats. Nutr. Rev. 2017, 75, 871–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hersey, J.C.; Wohlgenant, K.C.; Arsenault, J.E.; Kosa, K.M.; Muth, M.K. Effects of front-of-package and shelf nutrition labeling systems on consumers. Nutr. Rev. 2013, 71, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fagerstrøm, A.; Richartz, P.; Pawar, S.; Larsen, N.M.; Sigurdsson, V.; Eriksson, N. The relative importance of healthy food labels when shopping for groceries online. Procedia Comput. Sci. 2019, 164, 538–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fagerstrøm, A.; Eriksson, N.; Sigurdsson, V. Investigating the impact of Internet of Things services from a smartphone app on grocery shopping. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2020, 52, 101927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shin, S.; van Dam, R.M.; Finkelstein, E.A. The effect of dynamic food labels with real-time feedback on diet quality: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vyth, E.L.; Steenhuis, I.H.; Brandt, H.E.; Roodenburg, A.J.; Brug, J.; Seidell, J.C. Methodological quality of front-of-pack labeling studies: A review plus identification of research challenges. Nutr. Rev. 2012, 70, 709–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Talati, Z.; Egnell, M.; Hercberg, S.; Julia, C.; Pettigrew, S. Food choice under five front-of-package nutrition label conditions: An experimental study across 12 countries. Am. J. Public Health 2019, 109, 1770–1775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schuldt, J.P. Does green mean healthy? Nutrition label color affects perceptions of healthfulness. Health Commun. 2013, 28, 814–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roberto, C.A.; Shivaram, M.; Martinez, O.; Boles, C.; Harris, J.L.; Brownell, K.D. The Smart Choices front-of-package nutrition label. Influence on perceptions and intake of cereal. Appetite 2012, 58, 651–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Savoie, N.; Harvey, K.L.; Binnie, M.A.; Pasut, L. Consumer perceptions of front-of-package labelling systems and healthiness of foods. Can. J. Public Health 2013, 104, e359–e363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miklavec, K.; Pravst, I.; Raats, M.M.; Pohar, J. Front of package symbols as a tool to promote healthier food choices in Slovenia: Accompanying explanatory claim can considerably influence the consumer’s preferences. Food Res. Int. 2016, 90, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, J.; McMahon, E.; Brimblecombe, J. Point-of-sale nutrition information interventions in food retail stores to promote healthier food purchase and intake: A systematic review. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. Sensory expectations based on product-extrinsic food cues: An interdisciplinary review of the empirical evidence and theoretical accounts. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 40, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Granheim, S.I.; Løvhaug, A.L.; Terragni, L.; Torheim, L.E.; Thurston, M. Mapping the digital food environment: A systematic scoping review. Obes. Rev. 2022, 23, e13356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pitts, S.B.J.; Ng, S.W.; Blitstein, J.L.; Gustafson, A.; Niculescu, M. Online grocery shopping: Promise and pitfalls for healthier food and beverage purchases. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 3360–3376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nikolova, H.D.; Inman, J.J. Healthy choice: The effect of simplified point-of-sale nutritional information on consumer food choice behavior. J. Mark. Res. 2015, 52, 817–835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inman, J.J.; Nikolova, H. Shopper-facing retail technology: A retailer adoption decision framework incorporating shopper attitudes and privacy concerns. J. Retail. 2017, 93, 7–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mattilsynet. Nøkkelhullet. Available online: https://mattilsynet-xp7prod.enonic.cloud/api/_/attachment/inline/6e1b560e-8bb9-4fc0-95b8-1440abcbd484:85f2681b8ae3ba901d0cc3c65fae86242df2e821/Profilprogram%20for%20varemerket%20N%C3%B8kkelhullet%20-%20versjon%202021.pdf (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Ministère des Solidarités et de la Santé. Nutri-Score: Un Étiquetage Nutritionnel Pour Favoriser une Alimentation Équilibrée. Available online: https://solidarites-sante.gouv.fr/prevention-en-sante/preserver-sa-sante/nutrition/nutri-score/article/nutri-score-un-etiquetage-nutritionnel-pour-favoriser-une-alimentation (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Food and Drink Federation. GDA Labelling: A Tool to Help Improve Food Literacy of Consumers. Available online: https://www.gdalabel.org.uk/gda/front-of-pack-labelling.html (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- de Saud, M. Modifica Decreto Supermo N 977, de 1996, Reglamento Sanitario de Los Alimentos. Available online: https://www.minsal.cl/sites/default/files/decreto_etiquetado_alimentos_2015.pdf (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Department of Health. How to Use Health Star Ratings. Available online: http://www.healthstarrating.gov.au/internet/healthstarrating/publishing.nsf/Content/How-to-use-health-stars (accessed on 1 September 2022).
- Acton, R.B.; Vanderlee, L.; Hammond, D. Influence of front-of-package nutrition labels on beverage healthiness perceptions: Results from a randomized experiment. Prev. Med. 2018, 115, 83–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenigstorfer, J.; Groeppel-Klein, A.; Kamm, F. Healthful food decision making in response to traffic light color-coded nutrition labeling. J. Public Policy Mark. 2014, 33, 65–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Julia, C.; Blanchet, O.; Méjean, C.; Péneau, S.; Ducrot, P.; Allès, B.; Fezeu, L.K.; Touvier, M.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Singler, E. Impact of the front-of-pack 5-colour nutrition label (5-CNL) on the nutritional quality of purchases: An experimental study. International J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2016, 13, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raats, M.M.; Hieke, S.; Jola, C.; Hodgkins, C.; Kennedy, J.; Wills, J. Reference amounts utilised in front of package nutrition labelling; impact on product healthfulness evaluations. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015, 69, 619–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khandpur, N.; Sato, P.d.M.; Mais, L.A.; Martins, A.P.B.; Spinillo, C.G.; Garcia, M.T.; Rojas, C.F.U.; Jaime, P.C. Are front-of-package warning labels more effective at communicating nutrition information than traffic-light labels? A randomized controlled experiment in a Brazilian sample. Nutrients 2018, 10, 688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Egnell, M.; Boutron, I.; Péneau, S.; Ducrot, P.; Touvier, M.; Galan, P.; Buscail, C.; Porcher, R.; Ravaud, P.; Hercberg, S. Front-of-pack labeling and the nutritional quality of students’ food purchases: A 3-arm randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Public Health 2019, 109, 1122–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maubach, N.; Hoek, J.; Mather, D. Interpretive front-of-pack nutrition labels. Comparing competing recommendations. Appetite 2014, 82, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andrews, J.C.; Burton, S.; Kees, J. Is simpler always better? Consumer evaluations of front-of-package nutrition symbols. J. Public Policy Mark. 2011, 30, 175–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sacks, G.; Tikellis, K.; Millar, L.; Swinburn, B. Impact of ‘traffic-light’nutrition information on online food purchases in Australia. Aust. New Zealand J. Public Health 2011, 35, 122–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fuchs, K.L.; Lian, J.; Michels, L.; Mayer, S.; Toniato, E.; Tiefenbeck, V. Effects of Digital Food Labels on Healthy Food Choices in Online Grocery Shopping. Nutrients 2022, 14, 2044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, Y.; Kim, S.; Arora, N. GMO Labeling Policy and Consumer Choice. J. Mark. 2022, 86, 21–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2021, 10, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gusenbauer, M.; Haddaway, N.R. Which academic search systems are suitable for systematic reviews or meta-analyses? Evaluating retrieval qualities of Google Scholar, PubMed, and 26 other resources. Res. Synth. Methods 2020, 11, 181–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Higgins, J.P.; Savović, J.; Page, M.J.; Elbers, R.G.; Sterne, J.A. (Eds.) Assessing risk of bias in a randomized trial. In Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 2nd ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2019; pp. 205–228. [Google Scholar]
- Joanna Briggs Institute. Checklist for Quasi-Experimental Studies (Non-Randomized Experimental Studies): Critical Appraisal Tools for Use in JBI Systematic Reviews; JBI: Adelaide, Australia, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Finkelstein, E.A.; Ang, F.J.L.; Doble, B.; Wong, W.H.M.; van Dam, R.M. A randomized controlled trial evaluating the relative effectiveness of the multiple traffic light and nutri-score front of package nutrition labels. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reyes, M.; Garmendia, M.L.; Olivares, S.; Aqueveque, C.; Zacarías, I.; Corvalán, C. Development of the Chilean front-of-package food warning label. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Finkelstein, E.A.; Ang, F.J.L.; Doble, B. Randomized trial evaluating the effectiveness of within versus across-category front-of-package lower-calorie labelling on food demand. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koenigstorfer, J.; Wąsowicz-Kiryło, G.; Styśko-Kunkowska, M.; Groeppel-Klein, A. Behavioural effects of directive cues on front-of-package nutrition information: The combination matters! Public Health Nutr. 2014, 17, 2115–2121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C.; Lazzarini, G.A. Healthy choice label does not substantially improve consumers’ ability to select healthier cereals: Results of an online experiment. Br. J. Nutr. 2019, 121, 1313–1320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miklavec, K.; Hribar, M.; Kušar, A.; Pravst, I. Heart Images on Food Labels: A Health Claim or Not? Foods 2021, 10, 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Alcantara, M.; Ares, G.; de Castro, I.P.L.; Deliza, R. Gain vs. loss-framing for reducing sugar consumption: Insights from a choice experiment with six product categories. Food Res. Int. 2020, 136, 109458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deliza, R.; de Alcantara, M.; Pereira, R.; Ares, G. How do different warning signs compare with the guideline daily amount and traffic-light system? Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80, 103821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lima, M.; De Alcantara, M.; Rosenthal, A.; Deliza, R. Effectiveness of traffic light system on Brazilian consumers perception of food healthfulness. Food Sci. Hum. Wellness 2019, 8, 368–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vyth, E.L.; Steenhuis, I.H.; Heymans, M.W.; Roodenburg, A.J.; Brug, J.; Seidell, J.C. Influence of placement of a nutrition logo on cafeteria menu items on lunchtime food choices at Dutch work sites. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011, 111, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Machín, L.; Curutchet, M.R.; Giménez, A.; Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Ares, G. Do nutritional warnings do their work? Results from a choice experiment involving snack products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 77, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, E.; Tang, L.R.; Meusel, C.; Gupta, M. Optimization of menu-labeling formats to drive healthy dining: An eye tracking study. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 2018, 70, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antúnez, L.; Giménez, A.; Maiche, A.; Ares, G. Influence of interpretation aids on attentional capture, visual processing, and understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2015, 47, 292–299.e291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blitstein, J.L.; Guthrie, J.F.; Rains, C. Low-income parents’ use of front-of-package nutrition labels in a virtual supermarket. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2020, 52, 850–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gustafson, C.R.; Zeballos, E. The effect of presenting relative calorie information on calories ordered. Appetite 2020, 153, 104727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hagmann, D.; Siegrist, M. Nutri-Score, multiple traffic light and incomplete nutrition labelling on food packages: Effects on consumers’ accuracy in identifying healthier snack options. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menger-Ogle, A.D.; Graham, D.J. The influence of front-of-package nutrition claims on food perceptions and purchase intentions among Nepali consumers. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 66, 160–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vizcaíno, F.V.; Velasco, A. The battle between brands and nutritional labels: How brand familiarity decreases consumers’ alertness toward traffic light nutritional labels. J. Bus. Res. 2019, 101, 637–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gabor, A.M.; Stojnić, B.; Ostić, D.B. Effects of different nutrition labels on visual attention and accuracy of nutritional quality perception–Results of an experimental eye-tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 84, 103948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finkelstein, E.A.; Doble, B.; Ang, F.J.L.; Wong, W.H.M.; van Dam, R.M. A randomized controlled trial testing the effects of a positive front-of-pack label with or without a physical activity equivalent label on food purchases. Appetite 2021, 158, 104997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fagerstrøm, A.; Richartz, P.; Arntzen, E.; Sigurdsson, V. An explorative study on heuristic effects of healthy food labels in an online shopping situation. Procedia Comput. Sci. 2021, 181, 709–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Folkvord, F.; Bergmans, N.; Pabian, S. The effect of the Nutri-Score label on consumer’s attitudes, taste perception and purchase intention: An experimental pilot study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2021, 94, 104303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lima, M.; Ares, G.; Deliza, R. How do front of pack nutrition labels affect healthfulness perception of foods targeted at children? Insights from Brazilian children and parents. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 111–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yoo, H.-J.; Machín, L.; Arrúa, A.; Antúnez, L.; Vidal, L.; Giménez, A.; Curutchet, M.R.; Ares, G. Children and adolescents’ attitudes towards sugar reduction in dairy products. Food Res. Int. 2017, 94, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rojas-Rivas, E.; Antúnez, L.; Cuffia, F.; Otterbring, T.; Aschemann-Witzel, J.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Time orientation and risk perception moderate the influence of sodium warnings on food choice: Implications for the design of communication campaigns. Appetite 2020, 147, 104562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, X.; Chen, Q.; Xu, Z.; Zheng, Q.; Zhao, R.; Yang, H.; Ruan, C.; Han, F.; Chen, Q. Consumers’ preferences for health-related and low-carbon attributes of rice: A choice experiment. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 295, 126443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mauri, C.; Grazzini, L.; Ulqinaku, A.; Poletti, E. The effect of front-of-package nutrition labels on the choice of low sugar products. Psychol. Mark. 2021, 38, 1323–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jin, H.; Li, Y.a.; Li, D.; Zheng, J. The effects of physical activity calorie equivalent labeling on dieters’ food consumption and post-consumption physical activity. J. Consum. Aff. 2020, 54, 723–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huyghe, E.; Verstraeten, J.; Geuens, M.; Van Kerckhove, A. Clicks as a healthy alternative to bricks: How online grocery shopping reduces vice purchases. J. Mark. Res. 2017, 54, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Physical | Digitalized | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent Variable | Static | Interactive | Technology-Enabled | |
Purchase | 0% (10) | 66.67% (1, 3, 20) | 100% (25) | |
Consumption | 100% (30) | |||
Hypothetical choice | 100% (4, 11) | 100% (6, 7, 11, 12, 15, 16, 28, 29) | 50% (5, 14) | |
Self-reports | 33% (9, 10, 26) | 60% (16, 17, 18, 22, 24) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ljusic, N.; Fagerstrøm, A.; Pawar, S.; Arntzen, E. Effects of Digitalized Front-of-Package Food Labels on Healthy Food-Related Behavior: A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 363. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100363
Ljusic N, Fagerstrøm A, Pawar S, Arntzen E. Effects of Digitalized Front-of-Package Food Labels on Healthy Food-Related Behavior: A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(10):363. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100363
Chicago/Turabian StyleLjusic, Nikola, Asle Fagerstrøm, Sanchit Pawar, and Erik Arntzen. 2022. "Effects of Digitalized Front-of-Package Food Labels on Healthy Food-Related Behavior: A Systematic Review" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 10: 363. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100363
APA StyleLjusic, N., Fagerstrøm, A., Pawar, S., & Arntzen, E. (2022). Effects of Digitalized Front-of-Package Food Labels on Healthy Food-Related Behavior: A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences, 12(10), 363. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100363