Next Article in Journal
Relationship between Physical Fitness and Cyberbullying Patterns (Cybervictimization and Cyberperpetration) in Spanish Adolescents
Next Article in Special Issue
Exposure to the Death of Others during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Growing Mistrust in Medical Institutions as a Result of Personal Loss
Previous Article in Journal
Predictors of Vaccination Intentions and Behaviour during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Italy
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Impacts of Leaders’ Influence Tactics on Teleworkers’ Job Stress and Performance: The Moderating Role of Organizational Support in COVID-19
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Habitual Mask Wearing as Part of COVID-19 Control in Japan: An Assessment Using the Self-Report Habit Index

Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto University, Yoshida-Konoe-cho, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(11), 951; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110951
Submission received: 11 October 2023 / Revised: 10 November 2023 / Accepted: 16 November 2023 / Published: 19 November 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Health Psychology and Behaviors during COVID-19)

Abstract

:
Although the Japanese government removed mask-wearing requirements in 2023, relatively high rates of mask wearing have continued in Japan. We aimed to assess psychological reasons and the strength of habitual mask wearing in Japan. An Internet-based cross-sectional survey was conducted with non-random participant recruitment. We explored the frequency of mask usage, investigating psychological reasons for wearing masks. A regression analysis examined the association between psychological reasons and the frequency of mask wearing. The habitual use of masks was assessed in the participant’s most frequently visited indoor space and public transport using the self-report habit index. The principal component analysis with varimax rotation revealed distinct habitual characteristics. Among the 2640 participants surveyed from 6 to 9 February 2023, only 4.9% reported not wearing masks at all. Conformity to social norms was the most important reason for masks. Participants exhibited a slightly higher degree of habituation towards mask wearing on public transport compared to indoor spaces. The mask-wearing rate was higher in females than in males, and no significant difference was identified by age group. Daily mask wearing in indoor spaces was characterized by two traits (automaticity and behavioral frequency). A high mask-wearing frequency has been maintained in Japan during the social reopening transition period. Mask wearing has become a part of daily habit, especially on public transport, largely driven by automatic and frequent practice.

1. Introduction

The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) is a respiratory disease caused by severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2), which has resulted in over 680 million confirmed deaths as of April 2023 [1,2]. The virus is transmitted mainly via droplets and aerosols [1,3,4,5,6]. Secondary transmission is known to occur even during the incubation period (i.e., pre-symptomatic stage), accounting for approximately half of all secondary transmissions [7], and transmission from asymptomatic or mildly infected individuals can also occur [8]. To control the spread of COVID-19, a concerted effort that combines both pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical interventions was carried out across the world. While none of those interventions perfectly prevented transmission or severe illness, as part of a suppression policy to curb epidemic waves and slow transmission rates, many nations across the world implemented a range of public health and social measures (PHSMs), especially before the completion of the primary series vaccination [9]. The PHSMs included but were not limited to the restriction of contact (including lockdowns), contact tracing, social distancing, and personal protections, including mask-wearing [9,10,11,12]. While the vaccines contributed to greatly reducing the disease burden [13], especially by elevating the indirect protection of the vaccinated population, the fatality risk among vulnerable people remained high [14]. Moreover, post-COVID-19 sequelae are highly prevalent [15,16].
Although many of the PHSMs that greatly reduced contact were temporarily effective in reducing the incidence [17], the PHSMs can lead to pandemic fatigue, resulting in diminished effectiveness and responsiveness among citizens [18,19]. Ideally, sustainable countermeasures would be able to control the epidemic for an extended time period without causing fatigue. While the individual benefit of wearing a mask in protecting oneself is yet debated, mask wearing has been considered a cost-effective preventive measure [20]. During the pandemic, mask wearing has sometimes been recommended or mandated for entire populations, because secondary transmission from asymptomatic infected individuals can occur without recognition [6,21,22,23]. The wearing of non-woven fabric masks by infected individuals contributed to a reduction in the risk of secondary transmission [8]. A systematic review of 21 papers on this subject found that community-wide recommendations to wear masks were associated with reduced numbers of new infections, hospital admissions, and deaths [22]. While compliance with mask wearing is a potential practical issue, mask wearing is considered to be more sustainable than other non-pharmaceutical interventions, such as staying at home or maintaining physical distance [18]. Many countries have promoted de-masking as a policy symbol of downgrading COVID-19 control and fully reopening society [18,24,25]. Japan has not been an exception, and the government announced on 13 March 2023 that people may actively decide for themselves whether to wear masks, even in indoor spaces [26].
Given the circumstances described above, it is important to understand the psychological reasons and extent of the habituation of mask wearing. Published studies have frequently examined the reasons why people wear masks using various methodological approaches [27,28,29,30,31,32]. Nakayachi et al. suggested that in Japan, mask wearing in the early stages of the pandemic was driven by peer pressure rather than personal choice [30]. Little is known about the potential for habituation of mask wearing, even three years after the onset of this pandemic. Wood et al. conducted a literature review and attempted to propose several mechanisms of habituation [33], concluding that nonhabitual behaviors tend to induce greater levels of stress, whereas habitual behaviors employed in daily tasks reduced stress and increased the likelihood of success in continuing the behavior [34].
Do the Japanese still wear the mask due to peer pressure and to what extent is the mask-wearing behavior already habitual among the Japanese? The purposes of the present study were to understand the mask-wearing rate and its psychological reasons and also to measure the strength of mask-wearing habits among Japanese adults. To achieve the second objective, we used the self-report habit index (SRHI), originally developed by Verplanken and Orbell to assess habit strength [35], and applied it to mask wearing in Japan. The SRHI has been widely used across many health fields [36] and translated into Japanese, and its reliability has been confirmed elsewhere [37]. The SRHI assesses three aspects of habit: automaticity, frequency of the behavior, and self-identity [35,36,38].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data Source

A snapshot survey of Japanese adults aged over 20 years old was carried out from 6 to 9 February 2023. The participants were non-randomly recruited from registered users of Mellinks Ltd., Tokyo, Japan, a Japanese internet search company. The respondents did not receive financial remuneration, but upon completing the survey, they earned points from the company that could be later redeemed for valuable goods. Area sampling was performed, proportionally recruiting participant samples according to prefectural population size, with even weighting for age group (20–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, 60–69, and over 70 years) and sex (female and male). While sampling people in different geographic areas and ages, the recruited people are restricted to those who can manage their time to correspond to the survey request.

2.2. Data Collection and Definitions

The self-reported internet survey consisted of five sections, as follows: (i) demographic characteristics (age, sex, marital status, parental status), (ii) frequency of mask usage, (iii) the psychological reasons for mask wearing, (iv) self-recognition associated with habitual use of masks at the participant’s most frequently visited indoor space other than their household, (v) and self-recognition associated with habitual use of masks on public transport. In the second section, the frequency of mask wearing in indoor spaces (other than household) was examined, using a three-point scale (1 = I have not worn one at all, 2 = I have sometimes worn one, and 3 = I have usually worn one). In the third section, referring to a published study in early 2020 [30], we explored six psychological reasons for wearing masks. That is, we explored six psychological items that represent possible reasons to wear a mask, as follows: (i) perceived severity of disease (severity), (ii) wearing a mask to protect oneself (protection), (iii) wearing a mask to prevent spread (prevention) (iv), conforming to a norm to wear masks (norm), (v) feeling relief when mask wearing (relief), and (vi) impulse to take whatever actions are necessary (impulsion), via a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all to 5 = very much).
The fourth section gathered information about the respondents’ most frequently visited indoor spaces and the strength of mask-wearing habits among respondents in that particular indoor space. To measure the strength of habit, we used the Japanese version of the SRHI [37] using a seven-point Likert scale (1 = agree to 7 = disagree). The fifth section gathered information on the most frequently used public transport and the strength of mask-wearing habits on public transport. Again, we used the Japanese version of the SRHI. English-translated survey questions are available as a Supplemental File S1.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out in two steps. In the first step, we explored the frequency of mask wearing among participants and explored its relationship with psychological reasons. Subsequently, we investigated the strength of mask-wearing habits. Sex, marital status, parental status, age groups, most frequently visited indoor space, and most frequently used public transport were dealt with as categorical variables, and all other variables were dealt with as continuous variables. The level of significance was set at α = 0.05.
To summarize the results, we first clarified the descriptive features of the frequency of mask wearing (not at all, sometimes, and usually). To explore differences in the mask-wearing rate by demographic variables, multinomial logistic regression was used. Multiple linear regression was then used to model the relationship between the frequency of mask wearing and the six psychological reasons. The 12 items of the SRHI were assessed via principal component analysis (PCA) using maximum likelihood extraction and varimax rotation. The item loadings were derived from the pattern matrix. Subsequently, the SRHI item frequencies were compared to those in component 1 (the first principal component) and those in component 2 (the second principal component). All statistical data were analyzed using JMP Pro statistical software, version 17.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

2.4. Ethical Considerations

The participants received an explanation about the background and purpose of the study before enrollment and were given an explicit right to withdraw from the study. They were then asked to review the consent document prior to responding to our survey, and only those who agreed via the webpage were included. After the survey, Mellinks compiled anonymously processed information that could not be linked to any personally identifying information. This study was approved by the Medical Ethics Board of the Graduate School of Medicine at Kyoto University (R3863).

3. Results

From 6 to 9 February 2023, a total of 2640 participants responded to our survey. The demographic characteristics of participants are summarized in Table 1. Sex and age were evenly distributed by sampling, and 45.5% of participants were unmarried. Offices (n = 1305; 49.4%) were the most frequently visited indoor spaces other than households, followed by shopping malls (n = 935; 35.4%) and healthcare facilities (n = 170; 6.4%). Trains (n = 1709; 64.7%) were the most frequently used public transport, followed by buses (n = 499; 18.9%).
Table 2 shows the frequency of mask wearing. Overall, 83.7% of the participants reported that they usually wore a mask, and only 4.9% reported that they did not wear a mask at all. Female participants reported wearing masks more frequently than male participants (88.0% vs. 79.4% of female and male participants, respectively, responded “usually”), but no clear differences were identified by marital status. We did not identify significant differences in mask-wearing rates by age group.
Exploring psychological reasons for wearing masks, all six psychological reasons were positively correlated with the frequency of mask usage (Supplementary Table S1), which is consistent with previous reports [27,30]. Table 3 shows the associations between the frequency of mask wearing and six psychological reasons. According to the standardized coefficient, conformity to mask-wearing norms (0.22, p < 0.0001) was the most important reported psychological reason and was the only reason exhibiting a statistically significant association with the frequency of mask wearing. This association was also validated in different subgroups (Supplementary Tables S2–S13).
Figure 1 shows the distribution of the SRHI scores in indoor spaces and on public transport, stratified by sex and age group. The total SRHI score of all the participants in indoor spaces was 64.9 (±15.7). When stratified by sex, female participants scored 68.2 (±14.2) and male participants scored only 61.5 (±16.5). Additionally, older participants (aged 65 years and older) scored 70.0 (±14.0), while younger participants were less habituated to mask wearing, with a score of 63.1 (±15.9). The total SRHI score on public transport was 67.2 (±18.4), which was greater than that in indoor space. Stratifying by sex, female participants scored 70.8 (±16.1) and male participants scored 65.6 (±19.8). Additionally, older participants scored 71.3 (±18.8), while younger participants scored 65.8 (±18.0). In most subgroups, the SRHI scores on public transport were higher than those in indoor spaces (Supplementary Table S14).
Table 4 shows the results of PCA with varimax rotation of SRHI. For mask wearing in indoor space, the 12 items were underpinned by two components (component 1: eigenvalue 7.9, 66.1% variance explained, and component 2: eigenvalue 1.0, 8.1% variance explained). Component 1 captured 10 items, while component 2 captured nine items. The 12 items related to mask wearing on public transport were grouped under one component (component 1), which had an eigenvalue of 9.2 and explained 76.7% of the variance. Stratified by demographic characteristics, the results of the PCA of the SRHI were consistent in most subgroups (Supplementary Tables S15 and S16). For both indoor spaces and public transport, items 2, 3, 5, and 8 are known to reflect the automaticity of mask wearing behavior, yielding higher coefficient values than other items. Moreover, items 4, 6, 9, and 11 reflect self-identity, and small coefficient values for this category indicate that mask wearing had little to do with self-identification.

4. Discussion

Japan is considered to have a higher level of cultural tightness compared with Western countries [39]. We conducted a snapshot survey to investigate the prevalence of mask wearing in February 2023 in the Japanese cultural context. Of the 2640 participants, 83.7% reported that they usually wear a mask, and the behavior was found to be regulated mainly by social norms. Additionally, we assessed whether mask wearing had become a daily habit and found that respondents wear masks more regularly and unconsciously on public transport compared with indoor spaces. A daily habit of mask wearing was more likely to be reported by women and older people (65 years and older). PCA revealed that items associated with automaticity of behavior yielded high coefficient values, while mask wearing had little to do with self-identity.
Even shortly before the government legally downgraded the category of COVID-19 to be comparable to that of seasonal influenza from May 2023, mask wearing was maintained at a very high level in Japan, exceeding that at the early stages of the pandemic in 2020 [30]. Consistent with previous studies [29,40,41], female participants reported wearing masks more frequently than male participants and exhibited more adherence to personal protective behaviors, whereas some male respondents reported feeling ashamed of regular mask wearing [41,42,43]. COVID-19 was reported to change personal protective behaviors, and the rate of mask wearing did not differ by age. Positive emotional appraisals of masks were previously reported to be associated with the frequency and duration of mask wearing [44], the interpretation of which is linked to the theory of micro-valences [45]. From 2020 to 2023, mask wearing in Japan remained a strong social norm, and because of the emotional value of social norms, people in Japan were more likely to wear masks. Moreover, the collectivism that stems from social harmony and cohesiveness helped to positively promote mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic [28,41].
According to our participants, conformity to social norms was the most important reason for mask wearing, and the involvement of peer pressure has not changed since early 2020 [30]. Our finding regarding this point is consistent with the results of another study [27], which revealed that wearing masks was what people tended to express serious concern about non-mask-wearing behavior in the Japanese cultural context. This phenomenon is thought to be partially attributable to social anxiety toward COVID-19, and the fundamental need to be accepted by others [46]. Engagement in mask wearing appears to be affected by social judgments from others [47,48]. One previous study [46] referred to this phenomenon as a “situational norm”.
After spending more than 3 years under a state of emergency during the pandemic, mask-wearing behavior became a daily habit for many people, as assessed by SRHI items [35]. We found that people exhibited stronger habituation to mask wearing on public transport compared with indoor places. The need to wear masks on public transport is in line with social norms, and the finding is also partially explainable by ecological psychology theory [49,50], in which people’s behaviors and social thoughts are thought to be determined by time, physical environment, and the duration of time spent in a particular environment. That is, during weekdays, people spend longer time in indoor spaces than on public transport, and thus, may be more familiar and feel safer with indoor spaces, leading them to wear masks less frequently. Women have been found to wear masks more frequently than men, have more positive attitudes regarding their effectiveness, and perceive a higher level of risk than men [29,40,41]. All these factors may have contributed to a stronger habitual tendency for mask wearing in women compared with men. A previous study [51] reported that older people wore masks more often to avoid serious risks, possibly because they are more vulnerable to severe complications. In the long term, older people are likely to continue to habitually wear masks. Ongoing habituation in the presence of social norms is of course preferable news for public health, but not necessarily good news to the future society with downgraded COVID-19 control. Even provided that the risk of infection is lowered at some point in the future time, there might be strong social norms on public transport, and people who clearly do not wish to wear the mask need to maintain a non-habitual behavior of wearing the mask even in the society with reduced risk of infection.
Our PCA results indicated a one-dimensional structure, suggesting that the Japanese version of the SRHI can be used to assess the strength of habitual mask usage. We found that two characteristics underlie the habituation of wearing masks in indoor spaces, but only one characteristic applies to public transport. Based on previous studies [35,36,38], we interpreted the two rotated components as capturing the different characteristics of habit: automaticity, and frequency of the behavior, separately. When people spend time in public spaces, their behaviors are influenced by social norms, and 3 years of practice during the pandemic led to mask-wearing behavior being exercised unconsciously. With strong social norms, people tend to feel safer mimicking the behaviors of others and obeying guidance from others, so their social behavioral effectiveness can be maximized [46]. However, according to ecological psychology [50], we regard that people who spend most of their time in indoor spaces feel more at ease and less constrained and are more likely to realize that the behavior suits them and that this feeling allows them to be diverse and flexible in their behaviors, which leads to various characteristics of their behavior. As an important policy implication, governmental organizations should indicate specific indoor settings where habitual mask-wearing behavior is recommended in the long run (and where such indication can be relaxed).
The current study involved several limitations. First, the present study was cross-sectional, and we were not able to clarify the causal associations between mask wearing and dynamic features of collective behavior. Second, our participants were recruited using convenience sampling. This method may have led to bias in the sampling frequency. For example, individuals who receive care in a nursing home or hospital may have been less likely to participate in our study and may wear masks more frequently than the individuals who responded. Due to the convenient nature of sampling, those who do not regularly wear the mask might have been less likely to be recruited, and thus, we might have potentially overestimated mask-wearing coverage. Third, the detailed characteristics of the principal components we identified are still controversial, and the survey results regarding daily habitual behavior should be explored longitudinally. We plan to implement serial cross-sectional surveys in the future to address this issue. Fourth, the strength of habitual mask usage was assessed using a self-report method. Because individuals’ behaviors dynamically change, it will be important to consistently assess the strength of habitual mask use in future studies.
Despite these limitations, the current results demonstrate that the frequency of mask wearing remained very high in Japan in February 2023, and that this can be partially explained by the existence of strong social norms. Public transport was shown to have the potential to make mask wearing part of daily habit, while habitual use of masks was less common in the most frequently used indoor space. These findings may be helpful for informing future personal protective behaviors during the ongoing health impacts of endemic COVID-19.

5. Conclusions

In this study, we conducted an Internet-based cross-sectional survey between 6–9 February 2023. We revealed that a high mask-wearing frequency has been maintained in Japan during the social reopening transition period. Using regression analysis, we also found the main psychological reason for wearing masks is still conformity to social norms. And mask wearing has become a part of daily habits, especially on public transport, largely because of automaticity and behavioral frequency.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs13110951/s1, Supplementary Tables S1–S16, Table S1: Correlations among variables regarding mask usage, Table S2: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in female participants, Table S3: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in male participants, Table S4: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in married participants, Table S5: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in unmarried participants, Table S6: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants who have at least one child, Table S7: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants who do not have children, Table S8: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged 20–29 years old, Table S9: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged 30–39 years old, Table S10: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged 40–49 years old, Table S11: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged 50–59 years old, Table S12: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged 60–69 years old, Table S13: Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons in participants aged ≥ 70 years old, Table S14: Self-report habit index scores stratified in different groups, Table S15: Principal component analysis of self-report habit index in indoor spaces, Table S16: Principal component analysis of self-report habit index on public transport and Supplementary File S1.

Author Contributions

H.N. conceived the study. T.L., M.F., K.H., A.A. and H.N. conceptualized the study design. T.L. and M.F. performed statistical analyses. T.L. and H.N. drafted early versions of the manuscript and figures. T.L., M.F., K.H., A.A. and H.N. contributed to revising the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the Japan Science and Technology Agency RISTEX program for Science of Science, Technology and Innovation Policy (JPMJRS22B4). H.N. received funding from Health and Labour Sciences Research Grants (20CA2024, 20HA2007, 21HB1002, 21HA2016, and 23HA2005), the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (JP23fk0108685 and JP23fk0108612), JSPS KAKENHI (21H03198 and 22K19670), the Environment Research and Technology Development Fund (JPMEERF20S11804) of the Environmental Restoration and Conservation Agency of Japan, Kao Health Science Research, the Daikin GAP fund program of Kyoto University, and the Japan Science and Technology Agency SICORP program (JPMJSC20U3 and JPMJSC2105). M.F. received funding from JSPS KAKENHI. K.H. received funding from JSPS KAKENHI (20K18953) and The Health Care Science Institute (IKEN). A.A. received funding from JSPS KAKENHI (22KJ1852). The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Medical Ethics Board of the Graduate School of Medicine at Kyoto University (R3863). The participants received an explanation regarding their background and purpose before enrollment and were given an explicit right to withdraw from the study. They were then asked to review a consent document prior to responding to the survey, and only those who agreed on the webpage were included. After the survey, Mellinks compiled anonymously processed information that could not be linked to any personally identifying information.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Sharma, A.; Ahmad Farouk, I.; Lal, S.K. COVID-19: A review on the novel coronavirus disease evolution, transmission, detection, control and prevention. Viruses 2021, 13, 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. World Health Organization. WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard. Available online: https://covid19.who.int (accessed on 5 April 2023).
  3. Vardoulakis, S.; Sheel, M.; Lal, A.; Gray, D. COVID-19 environmental transmission and preventive public health measures. Aust. NZ J. Public Health 2020, 44, 333–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. World Health Organization. Modes of Transmission of Virus Causing COVID-19: Implications for IPC Precaution Recommendations. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/commentaries/detail/modes-of-transmission-of-virus-causing-covid-19-implications-for-ipc-precaution-recommendations (accessed on 5 April 2023).
  5. Azuma, K.; Yanagi, U.; Kagi, N.; Kim, H.; Ogata, M.; Hayashi, M. Environmental factors involved in SARS-CoV-2 transmission: Effect and role of indoor environmental quality in the strategy for COVID-19 infection control. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2020, 25, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Deng, W.; Sun, Y.; Yao, X.; Subramanian, K.; Ling, C.; Wang, H.; Chopra, S.S.; Xu, B.B.; Wang, J.X.; Chen, J.F.; et al. Masks for COVID-19. Adv. Sci. 2022, 9, 2102189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Nishiura, H.; Linton, N.M.; Akhmetzhanov, A.R. Serial interval of novel coronavirus (COVID-19) infections. Int. J. Infect. Dis. 2020, 93, 284–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Johansson, M.A.; Quandelacy, T.M.; Kada, S.; Parasad, P.V.; Steele, M.; Brooks, J.T.; Slayton, R.B.; Biggerstaff, M.; Butler, J.C. SARS-CoV-2 transmission from people without COVID-19 symptoms. JAMA Netw. Open 2021, 4, e2035057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blavatnik School of Government, COVID-19 Government Response Tracker. Available online: https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/research/covid-19-government-response-tracker (accessed on 5 April 2023).
  10. Chu, I.Y.-H.; Alam, P.; Larson, H.J.; Lin, L. Social consequences of mass quarantine during epidemics: A systematic review with implications for the COVID-19 response. J. Travel Med. 2020, 27, taaa192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mendez-Brito, A.; El Bcheraoui, C.; Pozo-Martin, F. Systematic review of empirical studies comparing the effectiveness of non-pharmaceutical interventions against COVID-19. J. Infect. 2021, 83, 281–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wenham, C.; Hale, T.; Green, K.; Andretti di Melo, B.; Furst, R.; Kamenkovich, N.; Petherick, A.; Philips, T.; Torness, W.; Laping, S.; et al. What Would a Data Framework for Policy Responses to Pandemic Diseases Look Like? Blavatnik School of Government, University of Oxford: Oxford, UK, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  13. Lewnard, J.A.; Hong, V.X.; Patel, M.M.; Kahn, R.; Lipsitch, M.; Tartof, S.Y. Clinical outcomes associated with SARS-CoV-2 Omicron (B.1.1.529) variant and BA.1/BA.1.1 or BA.2 subvariant infection in Southern California. Nat. Med. 2022, 28, 1933–1943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mefsin, Y.M.; Chen, D.; Bond, H.S.; Lin, Y.; Cheung, J.K.; Wong, J.Y.; Ali, S.T.; Lau, E.H.; Wu, P.; Leung, G.M.; et al. Epidemiology of infections with SARS-CoV-2 omicron BA.2 variant, Hong Kong, January–March 2022. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 2022, 28, 1856–1858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mantovani, A.; Morrone, M.C.; Patrono, C.; Gabriella Santoro, M.; Schiaffino, S.; Remuzzi, G.; Bussolati, G.; Cappuccinelli, P. Long covid: Where we stand and challenges ahead. Cell Death Differ. 2022, 29, 1891–1900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Brodin, P.; Casari, G.; Townsend, L.; O’Farrelly, C.; Tancevski, I.; Löffler-Ragg, J.; Mogensen, T.H.; Casanova, J.L.; Abel, L.; Aiuti, A.; et al. Studying severe long COVID to understand post-infectious disorders beyond COVID-19. Nat. Med. 2022, 28, 879–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Haug, N.; Geyrhofer, L.; Londei, A.; Dervic, E.; Desvars-Larrive, A.; Loreto, V.; Pinior, B.; Thurner, S.; Klimek, P. Ranking the effectiveness of worldwide COVID-19 government interventions. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 1303–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Petherick, A.; Goldszmidt, R.; Andrade, E.B.; Furst, R.; Hale, T.; Pott, A.; Wood, A. A worldwide assessment of changes in adherence to COVID-19 protective behaviours and hypothesized pandemic fatigue. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 1145–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Du, Z.; Wang, L.; Shan, S.; Lam, D.; Tsang, T.K.; Xiao, J.; Gao, H.; Yang, B.; Ali, S.T.; Pei, S.; et al. Pandemic fatigue impedes mitigation of COVID-19 in Hong Kong. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2022, 119, e2213313119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Li, H.; Yuan, K.; Sun, Y.K.; Zhen, Y.B.; Xu, Y.Y.; Su, S.Z.; Zhang, Y.-X.; Zhong, Y.; Wang, Y.-J.; Tian, S.-S.; et al. Efficacy and practice of facemask use in general population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Transl. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Boutzoukas, A.E.; Zimmerman, K.O.; Inkelas, M.; Brookhart, M.A.; Benjamin, D.K.; Butteris, S.; Koval, S.; DeMuri, G.P.; Manuel, V.G.; Smith, M.J.; et al. School masking policies and secondary SARS-CoV-2 transmission. Pediatrics 2022, 149, e2022056687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ford, N.; Holmer, H.K.; Chou, R.; Villeneuve, P.J.; Baller, A.; Kerkhove, M.V.; Allegranzi, B. Mask use in community settings in the context of COVID-19: A systematic review of ecological data. eClinicalMedicine 2021, 38, 101024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Abaluck, J.; Kwong, L.H.; Styczynski, A.; Haque, A.; Kabir, A.; Bates-Jefferys, E.; Crawford, E.; Benjamin-Chung, J.; Raihan, S.; Rahman, S.; et al. Impact of community masking on COVID-19: A cluster-randomized trial in Bangladesh. Science 2022, 375, eabi9069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. MacIntyre, C.R.; Nguyen, P.Y.; Chughtai, A.A.; Trent, M.; Gerber, B.; Steinhofel, K.; Seale, H. Mask use, risk-mitigation behaviours and pandemic fatigue during the COVID-19 pandemic in five cities in Australia, the UK and USA: A cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Infect. Dis. 2021, 106, 199–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hržič, R.; Peixoto, V.R.; Mason-Jones, A.J.; McCallum, A. Is it really time to ditch the mask? BMJ 2022, 377, o1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. Wearing a Mask. Available online: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/stf/seisakunitsuite/bunya/kansentaisaku_00001.html (accessed on 9 April 2023).
  27. Saint, S.A.; Moscovitch, D.A. Effects of mask-wearing on social anxiety: An exploratory review. Anxiety Stress Coping 2021, 34, 487–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lu, J.G.; Jin, P.; English, A.S. Collectivism predicts mask use during COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2021793118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. English, A.S.; Li, X. Mask use depends on the individual, situation, and location—Even without COVID-19 transmission: An observational study in Shanghai. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 754102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Nakayachi, K.; Ozaki, T.; Shibata, Y.; Yokoi, R. Why do Japanese people use masks against COVID-19, even though masks are unlikely to offer protection from infection? Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 1918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Karimi, S.M.; Salunkhe, S.S.; White, K.B.; Little, B.B.; McKinney, W.P.; Mitra, R.; Chen, Y.; Adkins, E.R.; Barclay, J.A.; Ezekekwu, E.; et al. Prevalence of unmasked and improperly masked behavior in indoor public areas during the COVID-19 pandemic: Analysis of a stratified random sample from Louisville, Kentucky. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0248324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Yang, L.; Constantino, S.M.; Grenfell, B.T.; Weber, E.U.; Levin, S.A.; Vasconcelos, V.V. Sociocultural determinants of global mask-wearing behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2022, 119, e2213525119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wood, W.; Rünger, D. Psychology of habit. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016, 67, 289–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Wood, W.; Quinn, J.M.; Kashy, D.A. Habits in everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 83, 1281–1297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Verplanken, B.; Orbell, S. Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 1313–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Orbell, S.; Verplanken, B. The strength of habit. Health Psychol. Rev. 2015, 9, 311–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Takami, K. What is habit as a psychological construct in health-related behaviour? Annu. Conv. Jpn. Assoc. Health Psychol. 2020, 33, 33. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gardner, B.; Mainsbridge, C.P.; Rebar, A.L.; Cooley, P.D.; Honan, C.; O’Brien, J.; Pedersen, S.J. Breaking the habit? Identifying discrete dimensions of sitting automaticity and their responsiveness to a sitting-reduction intervention. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gelfand, M.J.; Jackson, J.C.; Pan, X.; Nau, D.; Pieper, D.; Denison, E.; Dagher, M.; Lange, P.A.M.V.; Chiu, C.-Y.; Wang, M. The relationship between cultural tightness–looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: A global analysis. Lancet Planet. Health 2021, 5, e135–e144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Alagili, D.E.; Bamashmous, M. The Health Belief Model as an explanatory framework for COVID-19 prevention practices. J. Infect. Public Health 2021, 14, 1398–1403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Zhang, W.; Chen, S.F.; Li, K.K.; Liu, H.; Shen, H.C.; Zhang, X.C. Mask-wearing behavior during COVID-19 in China and its correlation with e-health literacy. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 930653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bekele, F.; Sheleme, T.; Fekadu, G.; Bekele, K. Patterns and associated factors of COVID-19 knowledge, attitude, and practice among general population and health care workers: A systematic review. SAGE Open Med. 2020, 8, 2050312120970721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Al-Hanawi, M.K.; Angawi, K.; Alshareef, N.; Qattan, A.M.N.; Helmy, H.Z.; Abudawood, Y.; Alqurashi, M.; Kattan, W.M.; Kadasah, N.A.; Chirwa, G.C.; et al. Knowledge, attitude and practice toward COVID-19 among the public in the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia: A cross-sectional study. Front. Public Health 2020, 8, 217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Dudarev, V.; Manaligod, M.G.; Enns, J.T.; Todd, R.M. In the hands of the beholder: Wearing a COVID-19 mask is associated with its attractiveness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2022, 75, 598–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Manaligod, M.G.M. Attentional Biases by Induced Microvalence in Novel Objects: An Emphasis on the Role of Experience. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  46. Aarts, H.; Dijksterhuis, A. The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 18–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Betsch, C.; Korn, L.; Sprengholz, P.; Felgendreff, L.; Eitze, S.; Schmid, P.; Böhm, R. Social and behavioral consequences of mask policies during the COVID-19 pandemic. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 21851–21853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Burgess, A.; Horii, M. Risk, ritual and health responsibilisation: Japan’s “safety blanket” of surgical face mask-wearing. Sociol. Health Illn. 2012, 34, 1184–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Rosa, E.M.; Tudge, J. Urie Bronfenbrenner’s theory of human development: Its evolution from ecology to bioecology. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 2013, 5, 243–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Meagher, B.R. Ecologizing social psychology: The physical environment as a necessary constituent of social processes. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 24, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Asri, A.; Asri, V.; Renerte, B.; Föllmi-Heusi, F.; Leuppi, J.D.; Muser, J.; Nüesch, R.; Schuler, D.; Fischbacher, U. Wearing a mask: For yourself or for others? Behavioral correlates of mask wearing among COVID-19 frontline workers. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0253621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Distribution of Self-Report Habit Index (SRHI) scores. This figure shows the distribution of SRHI scores of different subgroups in indoor space and public transport. The horizontal axis represents the different subgroups, and the vertical axis represents the SRHI score. The SRHI score represents the strength of the habit of mask wearing, ranging from 12 to 84. Higher SRHI scores indicate stronger habits. (A) Distribution of SRHI scores of all participants in indoor space and on public transport; (B,C) Distribution of the SRHI scores by sex in indoor space and on public transport; (D,E) Distribution of SRHI scores by age group in indoor space and on public transport. The box plot ranges from lower to upper quartiles, and the middle line represents the median value. Whiskers extend to minimum and maximum scores.
Figure 1. Distribution of Self-Report Habit Index (SRHI) scores. This figure shows the distribution of SRHI scores of different subgroups in indoor space and public transport. The horizontal axis represents the different subgroups, and the vertical axis represents the SRHI score. The SRHI score represents the strength of the habit of mask wearing, ranging from 12 to 84. Higher SRHI scores indicate stronger habits. (A) Distribution of SRHI scores of all participants in indoor space and on public transport; (B,C) Distribution of the SRHI scores by sex in indoor space and on public transport; (D,E) Distribution of SRHI scores by age group in indoor space and on public transport. The box plot ranges from lower to upper quartiles, and the middle line represents the median value. Whiskers extend to minimum and maximum scores.
Behavsci 13 00951 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of participants (n = 2640).
Table 1. Characteristics of participants (n = 2640).
CharacteristicsEstimate
SexMale1320 (50.0)
Marital StatusUnmarried1200 (45.5)
Have at least one child or notYes1247 (47.2)
Age group (years, all age groups have the same sample size of 440)20–2924.7 ± 2.8
30–3934.6 ± 2.9
40–4944.5 ± 2.9
50–5954.5 ± 2.9
60–6964.6 ± 2.9
≥7074.7 ± 4.0
Indoor space that is most frequently visitedOffice1305 (49.4)
School98 (3.7)
Healthcare Facility170 (6.4)
Shopping Mall935 (35.4)
Public Facility91 (3.5)
Other Indoor Places41 (1.6)
Public transportation is most frequently usedTrain1709 (64.7)
Bus499 (18.9)
Aircraft47 (1.8)
Boat9 (0.3)
Other Public Transportation376 (14.2)
Mean ± standard deviation; n (%).
Table 2. Distribution of frequency of mask usage (n = 2640).
Table 2. Distribution of frequency of mask usage (n = 2640).
Frequency of Mask Wearing
Not at allSometimesUsually
n (%)n (%)n (%)
Total130 (4.9)301 (11.4)2209 (83.7)
Sex *Male82 (6.2)190 (14.4)1048 (79.4)
Female48 (3.6)111 (8.4)1161 (88.0)
Marital StatusMarried72 (6.0)134 (11.2)994 (82.8)
Unmarried58 (4.0)167 (11.6)1215 (84.4)
Have at least one child or not **Yes 51 (4.1)156 (12.5)1040 (83.4)
No79 (5.7)145 (10.4)1169 (83.9)
Age (years)20–2929 (6.6)40 (9.1)371 (84.3)
30–3920 (4.6)43 (9.8)377 (85.7)
40–4927 (6.1)53 (12.0)360 (81.8)
50–5920 (4.6)54 (12.3)366 (83.2)
60–6918 (4.1)44 (10.0)378 (85.9)
≥7016 (3.6)67 (15.2)357 (81.1)
* p < 0.0001, ** p = 0.05. The test level is α = 0.05; the p-value was calculated by multinomial logistic regression analysis.
Table 3. Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons (n = 2640).
Table 3. Association between mask wearing and psychological reasons (n = 2640).
VariableStandardized Coefficientp-Value
Severity0.02 (0.02)0.41
Protection0.04 (0.03)0.15
Prevention0.02 (0.02)0.36
Impulsion0.05 (0.03)0.05
Norm0.22 (0.03)<0.0001
Relief0.01 (0.03)0.59
Frequency of mask usage as explained by six variables. Dependent variable: the frequency of mask wearing; independent variable: six psychological reasons variables. Standard errors are shown in brackets. R2 = 0.10, R2(adj) = 0.09, root mean square error (RMSE) = 0.49. The test level is α = 0.05; the p-value is calculated by multiple linear regression.
Table 4. Principal component analysis of Self-Report Habit Index (n = 2640).
Table 4. Principal component analysis of Self-Report Habit Index (n = 2640).
Mask Wearing IndoorsMask Wearing on Public Transport
Before RotationAfter Rotation
Component 1Component 2Component 1Component 2Component 1
Eigenvalue7.91.04.83.49.2
Variance explained (%)66.078.0540.3028.3476.70
Item loadings
  1. I do frequently.0.82−0.320.78-0.87
  2. I do automatically.0.85−0.220.80-0.91
  3. I do without having to consciously remember.0.88−0.230.84-0.93
  4. that makes me feel weird if I do not do it.0.820.290.470.700.88
  5. I do without thinking.0.88−0.090.740.470.93
  6. that would require effort not to do it.0.610.60-0.620.74
  7. that belongs to my (daily, weekly, monthly) routine.0.84−0.220.720.410.90
  8. I start doing before I realize I’m doing it.0.81−0.040.620.480.86
  9. I would find hard not to do.0.840.260.460.750.90
  10. I have no need to think about doing.0.79−0.050.590.480.87
  11. that’s typically “me”.0.750.41-0.750.80
  12. I have been doing for a long time.0.85−0.160.690.460.89
Component 1: the first principal component; Component 2: the second principal component; Rotation: maximum likelihood extraction and varimax rotation method. Rotated component loading values < 0.4 were not reported.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, T.; Fujimoto, M.; Hayashi, K.; Anzai, A.; Nishiura, H. Habitual Mask Wearing as Part of COVID-19 Control in Japan: An Assessment Using the Self-Report Habit Index. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 951. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110951

AMA Style

Li T, Fujimoto M, Hayashi K, Anzai A, Nishiura H. Habitual Mask Wearing as Part of COVID-19 Control in Japan: An Assessment Using the Self-Report Habit Index. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(11):951. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110951

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Tianwen, Marie Fujimoto, Katsuma Hayashi, Asami Anzai, and Hiroshi Nishiura. 2023. "Habitual Mask Wearing as Part of COVID-19 Control in Japan: An Assessment Using the Self-Report Habit Index" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 11: 951. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110951

APA Style

Li, T., Fujimoto, M., Hayashi, K., Anzai, A., & Nishiura, H. (2023). Habitual Mask Wearing as Part of COVID-19 Control in Japan: An Assessment Using the Self-Report Habit Index. Behavioral Sciences, 13(11), 951. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110951

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop