The Relationship between Language and Technology: How Screen Time Affects Language Development in Early Life—A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Material and Methods
2.1. Data Sources and Search Strategy
2.2. Study Selection
3. Results
3.1. Characteristic of the Included Studies
3.2. Screen Time and Language Development
3.3. Screen Time and Global Development of Children
4. Discussion
4.1. Television and Language Development: How Television Affects Language Acquisition in Children
4.2. Passive Screen Time vs. Active Screen Time: Do All Media Have the Same Effect on Child Language Development?
4.3. Do All Media Affect Language Development? Discordant Studies on the Negative Relationship between Screen Time and Language Acquisition
4.4. Child–Parent Interaction: How Caregivers Can Mediate the Effects of Screen Time on Language Development
4.5. Impact of Screen Time on Children with Delayed Language Development
4.6. Brain and Prolonged Screen Time: Structural Alterations in White Matter and Influence on Language Acquisition
4.7. Association between Global Cognitive Development and Screen Time across Ages and Developmental Domains
4.8. Impact of Screen Time on Attentional and Executive Functions
4.9. Screen Time and Development of Social Skills
5. Practical Fallouts
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Bozzola, E.; Spina, G.; Ruggiero, M.; Memo, L.; Agostiniani, R.; Bozzola, M.; Corsello, G.; Villani, A. Media devices in pre-school children: The recommendations of the Italian pediatric society. Ital. J. Pediatr. 2018, 44, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Canadian Paediatric Society; Digital Health Task Force. Screen time and young children: Promoting health and development in a digital world. Paediatr. Child Health 2017, 8, 461–477.
- Sweetser, P.; Johnson, D.; Ozdowska, A.; Wyeth, P. Active versus passive screen time for young children. Australas. J. Early Child. 2012, 37, 94–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, R.; Lauricella, A.; Zack, E.; Calvert, S.L. Infant and early childhood exposure to adult-directed and child-directed television programming: Relations with cognitive skills at age four. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2010, 56, 21–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomopoulos, S.; Dreyer, B.P.; Berkule, S.; Fierman, A.H.; Brockmeyer, C.; Mendelsohn, A.L. Infant media exposure and toddler development. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2010, 164, 1105–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zimmerman, F.J.; Christakis, D.A. Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: A longitudinal analysis of national data. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2005, 159, 619–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Council on Communications and Media; Brown, A. Media use by children younger than 2 years. Pediatrics 2011, 128, 1040–1045. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Barnes, J.D.; Cameron, C.; Carson, V.; Chaput, J.P.; Faulkner, G.E.; Janson, K.; Janssen, I.; Kramers, R.; LeBlanc, A.G.; Spence, J.C.; et al. Results from Canada’s 2016 ParticipACTION Report Card on Physical Activity for Children and Youth. J. Phys. Act. Health 2016, 13, 110–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canadian Radio-Television and Telecommunications Commission. Communications Monitoring Report. Ottawa, Ontario. 2016. Available online: www.crtc.gc.ca/eng/publications/reports/PolicyMonitoring/2016/cmr.pdf (accessed on 14 November 2023).
- Common Sense Media. Zero to Eight: Children’s Media Use in America 2013: A Common Sense Research Study. 2013. Available online: www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-childrensmedia-use-in-america-2013 (accessed on 14 November 2023).
- Cheung, C.H.M.; Vota, W. LSE Department of Media and Communications. What Are the Effects of Touchscreens on Toddler Development? 2016. Available online: http://blogs.lse.ac.uk/parenting4digitalfuture/2016/12/28/what-are-the-effects-oftouchscreens-on-toddlerdevelopment/ (accessed on 14 November 2023).
- Dusi, E. Bambini, già a un anno con il cellulare, La Repubblica. 5 January 2017. Available online: https://www.repubblica.it/salute/2016/12/22/news/bambini_gia_a_un_anno_con_il_cellulare-154682280/ (accessed on 14 November 2023).
- Leonard, L.B. Children with specific language impairment and their contribution to the study of language development. J. Child Lang. 2014, 1, 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeong, H.; Li, P.; Suzuki, W.; Sugiura, M.; Kawashima, R. Neural mechanisms of language learning from social contexts. Brain Lang. 2021, 212, 104874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Breitenstein, C.; Jansen, A.; Deppe, M.; Foerster, A.F.; Sommer, J.; Wolbers, T.; Knecht, S. Hippocampus activity differentiates good from poor learners of a novel lexicon. Neuroimage 2005, 25, 958–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, J.; Gates, K.; Molenaar, P.; Li, P. Neural changes underlying successful second language word learning: An fMRI study. J. Neurolinguistics 2015, 33, 29–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raboyeau, G.; Marcotte, K.; Adrover-Roig, D.; Ansaldo, A. Brain activation and lexical learning: The impact of learning phase and word type. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 2850–2861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karani, N.F.; Sher, J.; Mophosho, M. The influence of screen time on children’s language development: A scoping review. S. Afr. J. Commun. Disord. 2022, 69, 825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jordan, A.B. Learning to Use Books and Television: An Exploratory Study in the Ecological Perspective. Am. Behav. Sci. 2005, 48, 523–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balton, S.; Uys, K.; Alant, E. Family-based activity settings of children in a low-income African context. Afr. J. Disabil. 2019, 8, 364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, F.J.; Christakis, D.A.; Meltzoff, A.N. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2007, 161, 473–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chonchaiya, W.; Pruksananonda, C. Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97, 977–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christakis, D.A.; Gilkerson, J.; Richards, J.A.; Zimmerman, F.J.; Garrison, M.M.; Xu, D.; Gray, S.; Yapanel, U. Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: A population-based study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2009, 163, 554–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ling, L.Y.; Cherng, R.J.; Chen, Y.J.; Chen, Y.J.; Yang, H.M. Effects of television exposure on developmental skills among young children. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015, 38, 20–26. [Google Scholar]
- Perdana, S.; Medise, B.; Purwaningsih, E. Duration of watching TV and child language development in young children. Paediatr. Indones. 2017, 57, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hinkley, T.; Brown, H.; Carson, V.; Teychenne, M. Cross sectional associations of screen time and outdoor play with social skills in preschool children. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0193700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Madigan, S.; Browne, D.; Racine, N.; Mori, C.; Tough, S. Association Between Screen Time and Children’s Performance on a Developmental Screening Test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019, 173, 244–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heffler, K.F.; Sienko, D.M.; Subedi, K.; McCann, K.A.; Bennett, D.S. Association of Early-Life Social and Digital Media Experiences With Development of Autism Spectrum Disorder-Like Symptoms. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, 690–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hutton, J.S.; Dudley, J.; Horowitz-Kraus, T.; DeWitt, T.; Holland, S.K. Associations Between Screen-Based Media Use and Brain White Matter Integrity in Preschool-Aged Children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, e193869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinot, P.; Bernard, J.Y.; Peyre, H.; De Agostini, M.; Forhan, A.; Charles, M.A.; Plancoulaine, S.; Heude, B. Exposure to screens and children’s language development in the EDEN mother-child cohort. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 11863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vohr, B.R.; McGowan, E.C.; Bann, C.; Das, A.; Higgins, R.; Hintz, S.; Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Neonatal Research Network. Association of High Screen-Time Use With School-age Cognitive, Executive Function, and Behavior Outcomes in Extremely Preterm Children. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, 1025–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nobre, J.N.P.; Santos, J.N.; Santos, L.R.; Guedes, S.D.C.; Pereira, L.; Costa, J.M.; Morais, R.L.S. Determining factors in children’s screen time in early childhood. Ciência Saúde Coletiva 2021, 26, 1127–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Z.; Adamo, K.B.; Ogden, N.; Goldfield, G.S.; Okely, A.D.; Kuzik, N.; Crozier, M.; Hunter, S.; Predy, M.; Carson, V. Associations between screen time and cognitive development in preschoolers. Paediatr. Child Health 2021, 27, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kerai, S.; Almas, A.; Guhn, M.; Forer, B.; Oberle, E. Screen time and developmental health: Results from an early childhood study in Canada. BMC Public Health 2022, 22, 310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwarzer, C.; Grafe, N.; Hiemisch, A.; Kiess, W.; Poulain, T. Associations of media use and early childhood development: Cross-sectional findings from the LIFE Child study. Pediatr. Res. 2022, 91, 247–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yang, S.; Saïd, M.; Peyre, H.; Ramus, F.; Taine, M.; Law, E.C.; Dufourg, M.N.; Heude, B.; Charles, M.A.; Bernard, J.Y. Associations of screen use with cognitive development in early childhood: The ELFE birth cohort. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Takahashi, I.; Obara, T.; Ishikuro, M.; Murakami, K.; Ueno, F.; Noda, A.; Onuma, T.; Shinoda, G.; Nishimura, T.; Tsuchiya, K.J.; et al. Screen Time at Age 1 Year and Communication and Problem-Solving Developmental Delay at 2 and 4 Years. JAMA Pediatr. 2023, 177, 1039–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rai, J.; Predy, M.; Wiebe, S.A.; Rinaldi, C.; Zheng, Y.; Carson, V. Patterns of preschool children’s screen time, parent-child interactions, and cognitive development in early childhood: A pilot study. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2023, 9, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferguson, C.A. Baby talk in six languages. Am. Anthr. 1964, 66, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Locke, J.L. The Child’s Path to Spoken Language; Revised Edition; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 236–238. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, C.E. Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition: Beginning from Baby Talk: Twenty Years of Research on Input in Interaction; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1994; pp. 3–12. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, D.N.; Spieker, S.; Barnett, R.K.; MacKain, K. The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes. J. Child Lang. 1983, 1, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stern, D.N. The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Rice, M.L.; Haight, P.L. “Motherese” of Mr. Rogers: A description of the dialogue of educational television programs. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 1986, 51, 282–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radesky, J.S.; Schumacher, J.; Zuckerman, B. Mobile and interactive media use by young children: The good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics 2015, 135, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Council on Communications and Media. Media and Young Minds. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20162591. [Google Scholar]
- Nobre, J.N.P.; Vinolas Prat, B.; Santos, J.N.; Santos, L.R.; Pereira, L.; Guedes, S.D.C.; Ribeiro, R.F.; Morais, R.L.S. Quality of interactive media use in early childhood and child development: A multicriteria analysis. J. Pediatr. 2020, 96, 310–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reich, S.M.; Yau, J.C.; Warschauer, M. Tablet-Based eBooks for Young Children: What Does the Research Say? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2016, 37, 585–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maddison, R.; Mhurchu, C.N.; Jull, A.; Jiang, Y.; Prapavessis, H.; Rodgers, A. Energy expended playing video console games: An opportunity to increase children’s physical activity? Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 2007, 19, 334–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lieberman, D.A.; Chamberlin, B.; Medina, E., Jr.; Franklin, B.A.; Sanner, B.M.; Vafiadis, D.K. Power of Play: Innovations in Getting Active Summit Planning Committee. The power of play: Innovations in Getting Active Summit 2011: A science panel proceedings report from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2011, 123, 2507–2516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Li, X.; Atkins, M.S. Early childhood computer experience and cognitive and motor development. Pediatrics 2004, 113, 1715–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spence, I.; Feng, J. Video Games and Spatial Cognition. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2010, 14, 92–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christakis, D.A.; Zimmerman, F.J.; DiGiuseppe, D.L.; McCarty, C.A. Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics 2004, 113, 708–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linebarger, D.L.; Walker, D. Infants’ and Toddlers’ Television Viewing and Language Outcomes. Am. Behav. Sci. 2005, 48, 624–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guryan, J.; Hurst, E.; Kearney, M. Parental Education and Parental Time with Children. J. Econ. Perspect. 2008, 22, 23–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hinkley, T.; Salmon, J.; Okely, A.D.; Trost, S.G. Correlates of sedentary behaviours in preschool children: A review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010, 7, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poulain, T.; Vogel, M.; Neef, M.; Abicht, F.; Hilbert, A.; Genuneit, J.; Körner, A.; Kiess, W. Reciprocal Associations between Electronic Media Use and Behavioral Difficulties in Preschoolers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kirkorian, H.L.; Pempek, T.A.; Murphy, L.A.; Schmidt, M.E.; Anderson, D.R. The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 1350–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, D.R.; Subrahmanyam, K. Cognitive Impacts of Digital Media Workgroup. Digital Screen Media and Cognitive Development. Pediatrics 2017, 140, 57–61. [Google Scholar]
- Strouse, G.A.; Troseth, G.L.; O’Doherty, K.D.; Saylor, M.M. Co-viewing supports toddlers’ word learning from contingent and noncontingent video. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 166, 310–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myers, L.J.; Crawford, E.; Murphy, C.; Aka-Ezoua, E.; Felix, C. Eyes in the room trump eyes on the screen: Effects of a responsive co-viewer on toddlers’ responses to and learning from video chat. J. Child. Media 2018, 12, 275–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, D.R.; Pempek, T.A. Television and very young children. Am. Behav. Sci. 2005, 48, 505–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villani, V.S.; Olson, C.K.; Jellinek, M.S. Media literacy for clinicians and parents. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2005, 14, 523–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wartella, E.; Robb, M. Young children, new media. J. Child. Media 2007, 1, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strasburger, V.C. First do no harm: Why have parents and pediatricians missed the boat on children and media? J. Pediatr. 2007, 151, 334–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biedinger, N. The influence of education and home environment on the cognitive outcomes of preschool children in Germany. Child Dev. Res. 2011, 2011, 916303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramírez, N.F.; Lieberman, A.M.; Mayberry, R.I. The initial stages of first-language acquisition begun in adolescence: When late looks early. J. Child Lang. 2013, 40, 391–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oxford, M.; Spieker, S. Preschool language development among children of adolescent mothers. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 27, 165–182. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Schmidt, M.E.; Rich, M.; Rifas-Shiman, S.L.; Oken, E.; Taveras, E.M. Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort. Pediatrics 2009, 123, 370–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jordan, A.B.; Hersey, J.C.; McDivitt, J.A.; Heitzler, C.D. Reducing children’s television-viewing time: A qualitative study of parents and their children. Pediatrics 2006, 118, 1303–1310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soares, J.M.; Marques, P.; Alves, V.; Sousa, N. A hitchhiker’s guide to diffusion tensor imaging. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sousa, S.S.; Amaro, E., Jr.; Crego, A.; Gonçalves, Ó.F.; Sampaio, A. Developmental trajectory of the prefrontal cortex: A systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018, 12, 1197–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romeo, R.R.; Segaran, J.; Leonard, J.A.; Robinson, S.T.; West, M.R.; Mackey, A.P.; Yendiki, A.; Rowe, M.L.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Language Exposure Relates to Structural Neural Connectivity in Childhood. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 7870–7877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, J.E.; Grohs, M.N.; Dewey, D.; Lebel, C. Global and regional white matter development in early childhood. Neuroimage 2019, 196, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thiebaut de Schotten, M.; Cohen, L.; Amemiya, E.; Braga, L.W.; Dehaene, S. Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 4, 989–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smits, M.; Jiskoot, L.C.; Papma, J.M. White matter tracts of speech and language. Semin. Ultrasound CT MRI 2014, 35, 504–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dodson, C.K.; Travis, K.E.; Borchers, L.R.; Marchman, V.A.; Ben-Shachar, M.; Feldman, H.M. White matter properties associated with pre-reading skills in 6-year-old children born preterm and at term. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2018, 60, 695–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yeatman, J.D.; Dougherty, R.F.; Rykhlevskaia, E.; Sherbondy, A.J.; Deutsch, G.K.; Wandell, B.A.; Ben-Shachar, M. Anatomical properties of the arcuate fasciculus predict phonological and reading skills in children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011, 11, 3304–3317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Borchers, L.R.; Bruckert, L.; Dodson, C.K.; Travis, K.E.; Marchman, V.A.; Ben-Shachar, M.; Feldman, H.M. Microstructural properties of white matter pathways in relation to subsequent reading abilities in children: A longitudinal analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 2019, 2, 891–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Domingues-Montanari, S. Clinical and psychological effects of excessive screen time on children. J. Paediatr. Child Health 2017, 4, 333–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Felix, E.; Silva, V.; Caetano, M.; Ribeiro, M.V.V.; Fidalgo, T.M.; Rosa Neto, F.; Sanchez, Z.M.; Surkan, P.J.; Martins, S.S.; Caetano, S.C. Excessive Screen Media Use in Preschoolers Is Associated with Poor Motor Skills. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2020, 6, 418–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Madigan, S.; McArthur, B.A.; Anhorn, C.; Eirich, R.; Christakis, D.A. Associations Between Screen Use and Child Language Skills: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020, 174, 665–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walsh, J.J.; Barnes, J.D.; Tremblay, M.S.; Chaput, J.P. Associations between duration and type of electronic screen use and cognition in US children. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 108, 106312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radesky, J.; Miller, A.L.; Rosenblum, K.L.; Appugliese, D.; Kaciroti, N.; Lumeng, J.C. Maternal mobile device use during a structured parent-child interaction task. Acad. Pediatr. 2015, 2, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pempek, T.A.; Kirkorian, H.L.; Anderson, D.R. The effects of background television on the quantity and quality of child-directed speech by parents. J. Child. Media 2014, 8, 211–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, F.J.; Christakis, D.A. Associations between content types of early media exposure and subsequent attentional problems. Pediatrics 2007, 120, 986–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lo, C.B.; Waring, M.E.; Pagoto, S.L.; Lemon, S.C. A television in the bedroom is associated with higher weekday screen time among youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADD/ADHD). Prev. Med. Rep. 2015, 2, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ahn, J.S.; Min, S.; Kim, M.H. The Role of Uncontrolled Eating and Screen Time in the Link of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder with Weight in Late Childhood. Psychiatry Investig. 2017, 6, 808–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tandon, P.S.; Sasser, T.; Gonzalez, E.S.; Whitlock, K.B.; Christakis, D.A.; Stein, M.A. Physical Activity, Screen Time, and Sleep in Children With ADHD. J. Phys. Act. Health 2019, 16, 416–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Radesky, J.S.; Christakis, D.A. Increased Screen Time: Implications for Early Childhood Development and Behavior. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2016, 63, 827–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Christakis, D.A.; Ramirez, J.S.B.; Ferguson, S.M.; Ravinder, S.; Ramirez, J.M. How early media exposure may affect cognitive function: A review of results from observations in humans and experiments in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 9851–9858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zanto, T.P.; Gazzaley, A. Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies optimal working memory performance. J. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 3059–3066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kostyrka-Allchorne, K.; Cooper, N.R.; Simpson, A. The relationship between television exposure and children’s cognition and behaviour: A systematic review. Dev. Rev. 2017, 44, 19–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, R. Growing up in the digital age: Early learning and family media ecology. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 4, 341–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitzpatrick, C.; Cristini, E.; Bernard, J.Y.; Garon-Carrier, G. Meeting preschool screen time recommendations: Which parental strategies matter? Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1287396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, S.; Maeda, T.; Yoichi, S.; Yamagata, Z.; Tomiwa, K.; Japan Children’s Study Group. Early television exposure and children’s behavioral and social outcomes at age 30 months. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 20, 482–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mistry, K.B.; Minkovitz, C.S.; Strobino, D.M.; Borzekowski, D.L. Children’s television exposure and behavioral and social outcomes at 5.5 years: Does timing of exposure matter? Pediatrics 2007, 4, 762–769. [Google Scholar]
- Christakis, D.A. Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: Time to rethink the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline? JAMA Pediatr. 2014, 168, 399–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Telama, R. Moral development. In Psychology for Physical Educators; Durand, M., Bakker, F., Vanden Auweele, Y., Eds.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1999; pp. 321–342. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida, M.L.; Garon-Carrier, G.; Cinar, E.; Frizzo, G.B.; Fitzpatrick, C. Prospective associations between child screen time and parenting stress and later inattention symptoms in preschoolers during the COVID-19 pandemic. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1053146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vanderloo, L.M.; Carsley, S.; Aglipay, M.; Cost, K.T.; Maguire, J.; Birken, C.S. Applying Harm Reduction Principles to Address Screen Time in Young Children Amidst the COVID-19 Pandemic. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2020, 41, 335–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Author | Year | Study Design | Number of Cases | Materials | Main Findings | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Zimmerman F.J. & Christakis D.A. [23] | 2007 | Longitudinal study | 560 children’s parents aged 0 to 35 months. 407 children’s parents aged 4 to 5 years. |
| Early television viewing is associated with subsequent attention problems specifically for non-educational content and viewing before the age of 3. |
2 | Chonchaiya W. & Pruksananonda C. [24] | 2008 | Pilot study of a case-control study | 56 patients with language delay and 110 normal children, aged 15–48 months. |
| There is a relationship between early onset (at less than 12 months of age) and high frequency of television viewing (>2 h per day) and language delay. |
3 | Christakis D.A. et al. [25] | 2009 | Prospective, population-based observational study | 329 children from 2 to 48 months |
| Sound television is associated with less exposure to discernible human speech of adults and less speech of children, contributing to the delay in language development. |
4 | Ling-Yi L. et al. [26] | 2015 | Cross-sectional study | 75 children frequently exposed to television and 75 children not frequently exposed to television, between 15 and 35 months of age. |
| Children under 35 months of age exposed to television for 2 h a day are more likely to develop delayed language, cognitive, and motor development. |
5 | Perdana S.A. et al. [27] | 2017 | Cross-sectional study | 84 children from 18 month to 3 years. |
| Children who watch television more than 4 h a day and who watch television programmes in both English and their native language (Indonesian) have a higher risk of developing a language delay. |
6 | Hinkley T. et al. [28] | 2018 | Cross-sectional study | 958 children from 3 to 5 years |
| Television/DVD/video viewing may be negatively associated, and outdoor play favourably associated, with the social skills of pre-school children. |
7 | Madigan S. et al. [29] | 2019 | Longitudinal cohort study | 2441 mothers and children. Data were available when children were aged 24, 36, and 60 months. |
| Higher levels of time spent in front of the screen in 24- and 36-month-old children are associated with a delay in reaching the developmental milestones of children at 36 and 60 months, respectively. |
8 | Heffler K.F. et al. [30] | 2020 | Prospective cohort study | 2152 children at 12 and 18 months of age |
| Excessive daily exposure to television/video and little interactive play time between caregiver and child is significantly associated with more frequent ASD symptoms. |
9 | Hutton J.S. et al. [31] | 2020 | Cross-sectional study | 69 children between the ages of 3 and 5 years old |
| Excessive screen use is associated with worse results on cognitive assessments and lower measures of the microstructural organisation and myelinisation of brain white matter tracts that support language and literacy skills. |
10 | Martinot P. et al. [32] | 2021 | Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses | 1562 children aged 2 to 5–6 years old |
| Screen time in 2-year-old children is negatively associated with language development; exposure to television during family meals is negatively associated with language test scores at all ages. |
11 | Vohr B.R. et al. [33] | 2021 | Prospective cohort study | 414 children from 6 years 4 months to 7 years 2 months |
| High screen time is negatively associated with language, cognitive, behavioural, and executive function outcomes in extremely preterm children (EPT) at the age of 6–7 years. |
12 | Nobre J.R. et al. [34] | 2021 | Cross-sectional, descriptive, and exploratory study | 172 children aged 24 to 42 months and 15 days |
| Daily exposure > 2 h to television appears to be associated with delayed language, difficulties in social interactions, sedentary lifestyle, and low stimulation for creative thinking. |
13 | Zhang Z. et al. [35] | 2022 | Cross-sectional study | 97 preschoolers (36 to 60 months) |
| Children under the age of 60 months who interact with the screen for more than one hour a day have a higher risk of having a worse working memory than those who do not exceed this limit. |
14 | Kerai S. et al. [36] | 2022 | Cross-sectional study | 2983 children, the mean age of children was 5.2 |
| Children with a screen time of more than one hour per day, compared with those with a lower screen time, were more likely to develop vulnerabilities in the following domains: Social skills, emotional maturity, physical and mental well-being, language, and cognitive development. |
15 | Schwarzer C. et al. [37] | 2022 | Cross-sectional cohort study | 296 healthy 2–5-year-old preschoolers and 224 mothers |
| Daily screen time > 1 h in children and lower levels of parent–child interaction are negatively associated with language, cognitive, and socio-emotional abilities. |
16 | Yang S. et al. [38] | 2023 | Longitudinal cohort study | 13.763 children aged 2–5.5 years old |
| Family mealtime use of television in 2-year-olds is negatively associated with expressive language and general cognitive development. |
17 | Takahashi et al. [39] | 2023 | Prospective cohort study | 7097 children from 2 to 4 years |
| Daily screen time > 4 h in children is associated with delays in the development of communication and problem solving skills. |
18 | Rai J. et al. [40] | 2023 | Cross-sectional study | 44 children aged 3 years and their parents |
| Screen use is not totally negative. Total time spent on the screen and specific video/show/movie viewing are negatively correlated with working memory and co-use of mobile devices is negatively correlated with self-control, while viewing educational content has a positive correlation with children’s inhibitory control. |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Massaroni, V.; Delle Donne, V.; Marra, C.; Arcangeli, V.; Chieffo, D.P.R. The Relationship between Language and Technology: How Screen Time Affects Language Development in Early Life—A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010027
Massaroni V, Delle Donne V, Marra C, Arcangeli V, Chieffo DPR. The Relationship between Language and Technology: How Screen Time Affects Language Development in Early Life—A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(1):27. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010027
Chicago/Turabian StyleMassaroni, Valentina, Valentina Delle Donne, Camillo Marra, Valentina Arcangeli, and Daniela Pia Rosaria Chieffo. 2024. "The Relationship between Language and Technology: How Screen Time Affects Language Development in Early Life—A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 14, no. 1: 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010027
APA StyleMassaroni, V., Delle Donne, V., Marra, C., Arcangeli, V., & Chieffo, D. P. R. (2024). The Relationship between Language and Technology: How Screen Time Affects Language Development in Early Life—A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences, 14(1), 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14010027