Next Article in Journal
Genetically Predicted Association of 91 Circulating Inflammatory Proteins with Multiple Sclerosis: A Mendelian Randomization Study
Previous Article in Journal
Leveraging Artificial Intelligence to Optimize Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation for Long COVID Management: A Forward-Looking Perspective
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity and Hypnotizability in Motor Imagery

by
Eleonora Malloggi
1,2,†,
Žan Zelič
1,2,† and
Enrica Laura Santarcangelo
2,*
1
Department of Physics, University of Trento, 38122 Trento, Italy
2
Department of Translational Research and New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, Via San Zeno 31, 56127 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(8), 832; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080832
Submission received: 30 June 2024 / Revised: 11 August 2024 / Accepted: 17 August 2024 / Published: 19 August 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Behavioral Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
Motor imagery (MI) requires the mental representation of the body, obtained by integrating exteroceptive and interoceptive information. This study aimed to investigate the role of interoceptive sensitivity (IS) in MI performed through visual and kinesthetic modalities by participants with low (lows, N = 26; SHSS: A, M + SD: 1.00 + 1.52), medium (mediums, N = 11; SHSS: A, 6.00 + 0.77) and high hypnotizability scores (highs, N = 16; SHSS:A, 9.75 + 1.24), as measured by the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form A. The three groups displayed different MI abilities and IS levels. The efficacy of MI was measured using the chronometric index and self-reported experience, while IS was measured using the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) questionnaire. Alpha and beta power spectrum densities (PSDs) were extracted from the EEG signals acquired during baseline, actual movement and visually and kinesthetically imagined movements. The chronometric indices do not reveal significant differences between groups and imagery modalities. The self-report MI efficacy indicates better kinesthetic imagery in highs and mediums than in lows, and no modality difference among lows. The MAIA dimensions sustain the differences in subjective experience and almost all the EEG differences. The latter are slightly different in highs, mediums and lows. This is the first report of the major role played by IS in MI and strongly supports the theory of embodied cognition.

1. Introduction

Motor imagery (MI) is the mental simulation of a motor action without its actual execution [1]. It is used in neuro-rehabilitation therapies, including brain–computer interface interventions [2]. However, its efficacy is difficult to predict due to the large variability of motor imagery abilities in the general population [3,4].
Actual and imagined movements have been associated with different electroencephalographically recorded (EEG) patterns during their preparatory, execution and post-execution phases. Generally, alpha and beta desynchronization is observed during movement preparation and execution phases, whereas beta rebound occurs at the end of the movement [5,6]. Similar findings are reported for MI, with a decrease in EEG power (desynchronization), indicating increased cortical activity [7,8] and an increase representing the maintenance or inhibition of cortical activity [7,9,10].
According to the functional equivalence theory [11], the more similar the cortical activity during actual and imagined movement, the more efficacious the MI [12]. The difference between the duration of actual and imagined movements can also be used as a behavioral measure of the efficacy of MI (chronometric index). Indeed, actual and well-imagined actions have been shown to share time duration and autonomic responses in athletes [13] and in neurological patients [14,15]. In addition, the intensity of the experience of MI can be assessed by self-reports. Nonetheless, the correspondence between behavioral, EEG and subjective measures of MI is still debatable. It has been suggested that MI should be considered from multiple perspectives, including self-reports, chronometric indices, EEG activities and autonomic responses [16].

1.1. MI and Interoceptive Sensitivity

Efficacious MI requires a correct body schema, influenced by interoception [17], which is described as the »sense of the body« [18]. Interoception includes the dimensions of accuracy (IA, measured by the heartbeat counting task and heartbeat-evoked cortical potential (HEP)), sensitivity (IS, measured by self-report questionnaires) and awareness (the correspondence between IA and IS) [19]. IS is generally measured by the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA, [20]), which consists of eight scales (noticing, not distracting, not worrying, attention regulation, emotional awareness, self-regulation, body listening, trusting), indicating the mode of the individual interpretation of interoceptive signals.
The insular cortex is the brain structure most involved in interoception. It receives all sensory inputs, including somesthetic information, is involved in the sensory context relevant to voluntary movements [21] and participates in decision-making, social and risky behavior [22]. The activity of the anterior insula is involved in interoceptive accuracy [23], whereas its functional connectivity with the cerebellum, ventral striatum, brainstem and prefrontal cortex has been reported to be positively correlated with interoceptive sensitivity [24].
The body sensations experienced during motor imagery with different emotional content can increase or decrease the functional connectivity between the insula and the dorsomedial frontal cortex [22]. Additionally, an impaired body schema has been observed in patients with anorexia nervosa [25], who are also less accurate than control subjects in MI and less successful in the mental rotation of human figures [26]. Nonetheless, no study investigated the role of interoceptive sensitivity in motor imagery, despite the well-recognized role of the insula in this dimension of interoception.

1.2. Hypnotizability, MI and IS

Hypnotizability is a psychophysiological trait stable throughout life [27] and best-known for the ability of highly hypnotizable individuals to control pain through cognitive strategies [28]. It is measured by standardized scales classifying high (highs), medium (mediums) and low hypnotizable individuals (lows). Different hypnotizablity levels are associated with several behavioral and brain morpho-functional characteristics [29]. Among the former, there are differences in sensorimotor integration, vascular control and sensitivity of the opioid µ1 receptors [30]. Among the latter, the most important are the highs’ greater excitability of the motor cortex [31] and stronger functional equivalence (FE) between actual and imagined perception/action [32]. These differences make hypnotizability a good predictor of the efficacy of motor imagery [12].
Hypnotizability-related brain differences also include reduced grey matter volume in the insula and in the left cerebellum of highs compared to lows [33,34]. Accordingly, highs exhibit lower interoceptive accuracy [35,36] than lows, but »more adaptive« interoceptive sensitivity (IS)—that is, the tendency to trust their bodily signals and behave accordingly—compared to lows and mediums [37].

1.3. The Aim of This Study

Since both the efficacy of MI [32] and interoceptive sensitivity [37] differ according to hypnotizability, this study aimed to assess the role of hypnotizability and IS in the subjective experience, chronometric index and EEG correlates of visual and kinesthetic motor imagery in healthy highs, mediums and lows.
We hypothesize that interoceptive sensitivity sustains at least part of the EEG correlates of motor imagery, and that this occurs in highs and lows differentially. The expected results will highlight the role of interoceptive sensitivity in motor imagery for the first time.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Fifty-three right-handed (according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) healthy volunteers of both sexes (age range: 19–26 years) were recruited among the students at the University of Pisa. After signing the informed consent approved by the University Bioethics Committee (n.29/2022), the absence of medical, neurological, psychiatric disease, sleep and attention disorders and current pharmacological therapies was assessed by anamnestic interviews. Then, the participants were administered the Italian version of the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form A (SHSS: A, range: 0–12) [38], classifying them into groups of highs (score > 8 out of 12), mediums (score 5–7) and lows (score < 4). The SHSS scale consists of 12 behavioral items (for instance, postural instability, eyes closure, arms heaviness, rigidity and immobilization, hallucination of a mosquito, post-hypnotic command, amnesia). Each item is marked as passed/not passed depending on whether the hypnotist sees behavioral responses of fixed magnitude to suggestions within 10 s from the end of the suggestion. In the general population, mediums represent 70%, while highs and lows each represent 15% [39].

2.2. Experimental Design and Procedure

To prevent expectancy effects, questionnaire administration and the experimental session were performed at least two weeks after hypnotic assessment. The proneness to be deeply involved in cognitive tasks and interoceptive sensitivity were assessed through the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS, [40]) and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA, [20]). The experiments were conducted between 9 and 12 a.m. in a semi-dark and quiet room. Participants sat in a comfortable armchair with their eyes closed. Before the experimental session started, one of the experimenters demonstrated the movement to be performed and later imagined. The movement consisted of ten repetitions of flexion–extension of the left arm and hand, along with finger-to-thumb tapping. Its execution was visually inspected by an experimenter.
The experimental session consisted of a 2 min resting state (baseline), followed by two series of actual and imagined movements, performed through two different modalities (visual and kinesthetic). An actual movement (Mv) preceded each series of three repetitions of the same, visually imagined (MIv) and kinesthetically imagined movement (MIk). Each imagery series was also preceded by listening to a recorded script describing the movement (Supplementary Table S1). For visual imagery, the script read, “…Now please imagine doing the same movement you did a few minutes ago. You can see your left arm flexing up to the shoulder while your fingers touch your thumb one by one from the index to the little finger…”. For the kinesthetic imagery, the script read, “…Now, please imagine repeating the same movement you did a few minutes ago. You can feel the tension growing in your left biceps as it flexes up to the shoulder, while the muscles in your forearm start contracting and your fingers touch your thumb one by one from the index to the little finger …”.
The conditions were interspersed with 30 s rest periods. The duration of each condition was monitored. The imaginative visual and kinesthetic sequences were randomized among the subjects of each group (Figure 1). Each condition was initiated by a vocal command and ended when the experimenter saw that the real movement had ended (Mv, Mk), or when the subject declared that the imagery task had been completed (VI, KI). At the end of each imagery condition, the participants rated the experienced efficacy of imagery (range: 0–10).

2.3. Signal Acquisition and Analysis

The electrocardiogram (ECG) and electroencephalogram (EEG) were acquired by the g.tec’ smultipurpose wireless biosignal acquisition tool g. Nautilus (G.tec Medical Engineering, Graz, Austria). An EEG cap with 28 electrodes placed in standardized positions according to the modified 10-10 international system was used (FP1, FP2, AF3, AF4, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, C3, FC1, FC2, C4, T7, Cz, T8, CP5, CP1, CP2, CP6, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, PO3, PO4, Oz) and the reference was set to Cz. Two additional ECG electrodes were placed underneath the right and left clavicle. The sampling rate was set to 500 Hz and all impedances were kept below 30 kΩ.
EEG pre-processing was performed with the EEGLAB toolbox [41]. Signals were band-pass filtered (band pass: 0.5–45 Hz, two-way least-squares FIR filtering, according to the frequency bands analyzed, 8–33 Hz) and visually inspected to reject physiological and non-physiological artifacts. Individual channels showing quality decline (due to instability or loss of contact with the scalp during recordings) were visually identified and replaced with signals obtained via spline interpolation, a method generally implemented to maintain the spectral characteristics of the signal [42]. To remove residual artifacts, values exceeding the range of −70–70 mV were discarded. The retained EEG signals were downsampled to 256 Hz to reduce the required computing power. The signal was then submitted to Independent Component Analysis (ICA, [43]) to remove ocular, heart and muscular artifacts in each subject. We used the extended ICA algorithm and selected 20 components explaining the highest variance, since the optimal number of components should be between half and three quarters of the electrode number, thus balancing between identifying sufficient variance and avoiding overfitting [41]. Artifact components were identified by visual inspection of their time course, power spectrum and scalp maps. For every condition (B, M, MIv and MIk), the pre-processed EEG signal was subjected to Power Spectral Density (PSD) analysis using Welch’s method. PSD estimation was performed with a Hamming window of 4 s length, as a compromise between frequency and temporal resolution, and 50% overlap, to reduce spectral variability. The choice of using a Hamming window rather than a rectangular window was justified by the necessity to avoid spectral distortions due to border discontinuity. For F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, PO3, PO4 channels, the PSD was integrated over the alpha (8–12 Hz), low-beta (13–21 Hz) and high-beta (22–30 Hz) frequency bands and then log-transformed. PSD was averaged across the three visual and across the three kinesthetic imagery conditions, and across the two actual movement conditions. Signals from frontal and central, as well as parietal and occipital electrodes were averaged to obtain two regions (fronto–central, FC, and parieto–occipital, PO) for every condition. The heart rate was extracted by the open-source MATLAB toolbox EEG-Beats [44], which downsamples the signal to 128 Hz, applies FIR filtering (3 Hz high-pass, 20 Hz low-pass) and then uses a divide-and-conquer strategy to identify ECG peaks.

2.4. Variables

2.4.1. Preliminary Evaluation

Hypnotic Assessment

According to the SHSS: A, the participants were 16 highs (M + SD: 9.75 + 1.24; 9 females), 11 mediums (6.00 + 0.77; 7 females), 26 lows (1.00 + 1.52; 13 females), with mediums representing 70% of the general population and highs and lows each representing 15% [39]. SHSS scores were significantly different across the hypnotizability groups (F(2, 50) = 224.90, p < 0.001), with the highs’ scores higher than those of the mediums (p < 0.001) and lows (p < 0.001) and the mediums’ scores higher than those of the lows (p < 0.001).

Questionnaires

-
Absorption (Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS, [40]), measuring the level of proneness to be deeply involved in cognitive tasks (range: 0–34).
-
Interoceptive sensitivity (Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA [20]), consisting of 8 dimensions (noticing, not distracting, not worrying, attention regulation, emotional awareness, self-regulation, body listening, trusting), with each item scored between 0 (min) and 5 (max).

2.4.2. Experimental Session

-
For physiological assessment: EEG alpha and beta PSD and heart rate (HR) during the baseline condition (B), actual movement (M), visual and kinesthetic MI (VI, KI).
-
For psychophysiological assessment, visual (ΔV) and kinesthetic (ΔK) imagery duration normalized to movement duration computed as [(actual movement duration—imagined movement duration)/actual movement duration].
-
For the subjective experience of MI, self-report of the efficacy of visual (Ve) and kinesthetic (Ke) imagery (range: 0–10).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

To assess hypnotizability group differences, a univariate ANOVA was used for TAS scores and a multivariate ANOVA was applied to MAIA dimensions. Separate repeated measures ANOVAs (2 modalities × 3 groups) were applied to ΔHR, chronometry and efficacy variables. EEG log transformed signals were studied by repeated measures ANOVAs according to the following experimental design: 3 groups (highs, mediums, lows) × 4 conditions (B, M, V, K) × 2 hemispheric sides (left, right) × 2 brain regions (fronto–central, parieto–occipital).
The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used when the sphericity assumption was not met. ANCOVA was applied to test the differences in chronometry, efficacy and EEG using MAIA dimensions as covariates. Post-hoc comparisons were performed by contrast analysis and paired t-tests. Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied when necessary. Spearman correlation was used to study associations between chronometric, subjective measures of efficacy of each imagery modality and EEG alpha-/low-beta/high-beta power spectra. The significance level was set at α = 0.05.

3. Results

All the enrolled participants completed the study. Table 1 reports the mean values and standard deviations of all the studied variables. The TAS scores were significantly different between hypnotizability groups (F(2, 50) = 6.99, p = 0.002), with the highs’ (p = 0.006) and mediums’ scores (p = 0.019) higher than those of the lows, and no difference between highs and mediums. Among the MAIA dimensions of interoceptive sensitivity, only noticing differed among the hypnotizability groups (F(2, 50) = 3.86, p = 0.028), with highs’ scores > lows’ (p = 0.046).

3.1. Motor Imagery, Hypnotizability and Interoception

3.1.1. Heart Rate

Repeated measures ANOVA did not reveal a significant group effect for ΔHR. The changes in heart rate between visual imagery and actual movement (ΔHRv) were larger than the changes occurring between kinesthetic imagery and actual movement (ΔHRk) (F(1, 50) = 28.63, p < 0.001).

3.1.2. Subjective Efficacy (Ve, Ke)

Repeated measures ANOVA (3 groups × 2 MI modalities) revealed significant differences between imagery modalities (K > V, F(1, 50) = 16.82, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.252, α = 0.980) and a significant modality × group interaction (F(2, 50) = 3.918, p = 0.026, η2 = 0.135, α = 0.680). Its decomposition revealed a significant difference between imagery modalities in highs (K > V, F(1, 15) = 6.79, p = 0.020) and mediums (K > V, F(1, 10) = 7.56, p = 0.02) and no difference in lows. The kinesthetic efficacy was greater in mediums compared to lows (p = 0.046) and did not differ between highs and mediums and between highs and lows. The visual efficacy was similar in all three groups (Figure 2a). Controlling for MAIA dimensions in the ANOVA abolished all differences.

3.1.3. Chronometry (ΔV, ΔK)

A repeated measures ANOVA (3 groups × 2 MI modalities) did not reveal significant differences between groups and imagery modalities (Figure 2b). No difference was found by controlling for MAIA dimensions in the ANOVA.

3.1.4. Correlational Analysis

Chronometric (ΔK, ΔV) and subjective (Ke, Ve) measures of motor imagery did not correlate with each other for both modalities of imagery. Partial correlation controlling for SHSS and MAIA dimensions did not show any significant difference.

3.2. EEG Alpha and Beta PSD

Based on both the functional equivalence model [11] and on the literature showing lower desynchronization during imagery than during actual movement [45,46], we studied the conditions in which K and/or V power spectra were higher or non-significantly different from M, given the presence of significantly greater desynchronization during movement and imagery compared to baseline conditions.
The EEG power spectra of alpha, low-beta and high-beta showed significant differences between the baseline (B) and movement (M) conditions at all sites.
Figure 3. Topoplots of EEG alpha PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Figure 3. Topoplots of EEG alpha PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Brainsci 14 00832 g003
The side and condition effects were abolished by controlling for the MAIA dimensions. The region effect (F(1, 42) = 6.62, p = 0.014, η2 = 0.136 α = 0.710) and the region × condition × group interaction (F(6, 126) = 3.05, p = 0.013, η2 = 0.126 α = 0.854) survived. In highs, the decomposition of this interaction revealed significant region (FC < PO) and condition effects, which, however, did not involve differences between B, V and K (Supplementary Table S1). The significant main effects and interactions are reported in Table 2.
In mediums, Table 2 shows significant side, region and condition effects with M = V and M = K also observed (Figure 4, Supplementary Table S1).
In lows, the region × condition interaction revealed FC power < PO power in all conditions, B = K = V in FC, and M < K in PO (Supplementary Table S1).
No significant group effect was observed.
Figure 5. Topoplots of EEG low-beta PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Figure 5. Topoplots of EEG low-beta PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Brainsci 14 00832 g005
Low-beta showed significantly higher power in the right hemisphere, in the PO region and during V and K compared to M (Supplementary Table S2).
Table 3 shows the main effects and interactions for low and high-beta.
Figure 6. Topoplots of EEG high-beta PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Figure 6. Topoplots of EEG high-beta PSD during baseline, real movement, visual and kinesthetic imagery in lows (upper panel), mediums (middle panel) and highs (lower panel).
Brainsci 14 00832 g006
For high-beta (Figure 6), the significant side x group interaction (Table 3) revealed a significant side effect only in mediums (Supplementary Table S2). The significant region x condition interaction (Figure 7) indicated FC power < PO in all conditions, M < K in FC and no difference between conditions in PO (Supplementary Table S2).
All main effects and interactions described for low-beta and high-beta (Figure 7) became non-significant when controlling for MAIA.
No significant correlation was observed between EEG alpha/beta PSD and subjective or chronometric variables.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the role of hypnotizability and interoceptive sensitivity in motor imagery performed through the visual or kinesthetic sensory modality. The findings enable us to report, for the first time, a relevant role of interoceptive sensitivity in the subjective experience of movement imagery and its cortical correlates. In contrast, chronometric indices were not influenced by IS. Only alpha and high-beta PSD displayed a few hypnotizability-related differences.

4.1. Subjective and Behavioral Findings

The findings show that both interoceptive sensitivity and hypnotizability contribute to the difference in the subjective experience of visual and kinesthetic MI among the three groups, but not to their chronometric differences. The absence of a correlation between chronometry and subjective experience in both modalities of imagery was not unexpected, owing to hypnotizability-related differences in imagery abilities [32,47] and interoceptive characteristics [37], which might buffer each other differentially across the three groups. Subjective experience and chronometric indices of MI have been described, however, as measures of different components of motor imagery, whose effects may not be necessarily correlated with each other [48].
The subjective reports of the efficacy of visual imagery did not differ between groups, possibly due to the easiness of this modality of imagery. In a previous study, visual imagery was reported as easier than somesthetic imagery by lows when in standing position [49], thus possibly buffering the gap with highs, whereas highs and lows reported the same vividness and effort when seated [50].
Kinesthetic imagery was reported as more effective than visual imagery by mediums and highs, but not by lows, as previously observed in standing participants with different hypnotizability [49] and elite athletes not characterized by hypnotizability [48]. The proneness to absorption, higher in mediums and highs than in lows, might account for the difference between lows and mediums/highs.
As earlier shown in the general population [49], and in line with the suggested multivariate model of motor imagery [16], no group difference was found in chronometry, despite the hypnotizability-related differences in the efficacy of kinesthetic imagery.
The evaluation of heart rate made the results more robust, as the efficacy of MI requires a multidimensional approach, including autonomic activation (heart rate, electrodermal activity), subjective reports, EEG signals and chronometric measures [16].

4.2. EEG Findings

The EEG findings indicate that cortical activities—alpha, low-beta and high-beta—were largely sustained by interoceptive sensitivity, as controlling for the MAIA dimensions in the repeated measures ANOVAs abolished almost all the significant effects. This provides additional insight into the relationship between cognitive functions, such as imagery, and the perception and interpretation of bodily sensations, supporting the concept of embodied cognition [51].
EEG alpha spectral frequency, whose changes were sustained by interoceptive sensitivity, highlighted three different, hypnotizability-related modes of cortical elaboration of the imagery tasks. In highs, the cortical activities associated with both imagery tasks did not differ from the baseline, despite their subjective and behavioral correlates. This could be a side effect of the highs’ distributed rather than nested cortical activity, as indicated by earlier studies [32,50,52], which reported relatively few spectral changes in brain activities during sensory–cognitive imagery tasks in this group. Mediums, who represent the general population [39], exhibited the expected desynchronization [7,8], and lows desynchronized only in the parietal–occipital region, suggesting a visual rather than kinesthetic mechanism of motor imagery, independently of the requested modality. This group, similar to the highs, had the same frontocentral alpha power in baseline and imagery conditions. The interpretation of this finding in lows, however, is different from highs, as they could have paid less attention to the task, in line with their lower scores of absorption (TAS).
Low-beta and high-beta changes were sustained by interoceptive sensitivity, too. Low-beta changes (showing lower desynchronization during visual and kinesthetic imagery compared to the actual movement) were observed across the entire sample, as reported for the general population [53], and were sustained by IS. High-beta was modulated during both the kinesthetic and visual imagery in frontocentral regions and, counterintuitively, only during kinesthetic imagery at parieto–occipital sites. Nonetheless, this could be accounted for by the role suggested for beta as a large-scale communication mechanism between sensorimotor areas and other brain regions [54], as well as by the reported similar brain activations during visual and kinesthetic imagery in the general population [55]. The absence of cortical response to visual imagery suggests that, in line with the subjective experience reported by highs and mediums, kinesthetic motor imagery is more effective compared to visual motor imagery [56]. Indeed, the correlates of kinesthetic imagery are more similar to movement, whereas visual correlates better resemble action observation [9].
For high-beta, mediums exhibited hemispheric differences consisting of larger left than right desynchronization. In baseline conditions, highs have been reported to have higher cortical activity compared to lows in the frontal left region [57,58], and it is possible that in the present study, the bilateral activations reported for mental imagery [59] buffered the hemispheric difference related to cardiac asymmetric information in highs, and no differences were disclosed in lows. Nonetheless, the mediums’ predominant left activation could suggest a mechanism similar to that of the highs. As a matter of fact, few studies have enrolled mediums; thus, information about their difference from highs and lows is scarce [60].
As previously reported [16], and in line with the several discrepancies observed in behavioral and neural correlates of imagined and executed actions [45], no significant correlations were observed between subjective, chronometric and EEG correlates of the studied tasks. Regarding EEG, the debate should also consider the substantial absence of reliable indicators of covert cortical activities [61] and the different styles of information processing observed in participants with different hypnotizability [32,50]. Different methods of EEG signal analysis may likely provide further information. Furthermore, we might argue that the temporal equivalence between actual and imagined movements, which is expressed by the chronometric index, is not necessarily an index of performance accuracy. Indeed, a single reliable index has not been found and we agree with Collet and colleagues [16] on the necessity of a multidimensional evaluation of motor imagery: autonomic responses as indices of task-related arousal, EEG activities as indices of functional equivalence, reported efficacy as subjective experience and chronometry as an index of behavioral correspondence of imagined movement with the actual one.

Limitations

The limitations of this study are the absence of reports about how much participants imagined through the requested sensory modality, and the distribution of the participants’ hypnotizability, which was not in line with the hypnotizability distribution observed in the general population, which includes 15% of highs and lows and 70% of mediums [39].
Moreover, the experimental design included series of ten consecutive actual/imagined movements with no marker of each flexion/extension (impossible during imagery). Thus, we were unable to show EEG (de)synchronization for each movement within the series. The desynchronization observed during the entire movement/imagery tasks with respect to the baseline is a consequence of the predominant desynchronization associated with preparation and execution with respect to the synchronization associated with movement termination [5,6].
A strength of this study lies in its protocol, which involved a complex flexion–extension movement of the entire arm, rather than partially automatic, sequential movements limited to hands or fingers [62]. Moreover, the participants were seated rather than lying down [63,64], which was more suitable for performing the studied movement. Finally, the similar EEG correlates of visual and kinesthetic imagery could be accounted for by the fact that the two modalities of imagery were not exclusively used when participants were invited to use one of them, in line with earlier findings of imaging studies [56,64].

5. Conclusions

This is the first report of a major role of interoceptive sensitivity in the cortical activities associated with motor imagery. It shows hypnotizability-related differences in the influence of interoceptive sensitivity on motor imagery. From a general perspective, it highlights the close relationship between the individual experience of interoceptive information and cognitive activities, extending to motor imagery the role of the insula, in the light of the concept of embodied cognition. Our findings can be relevant to personalized neurorehabilitation training and to the improvement of sports performance by imagery training. The variable effects of BCI interventions [2,65,66] in post-stroke rehabilitation, in fact, could be attributed either to ineffective motor imagery or to altered interoceptive sensitivity. Moreover, since motor imagery training has also been found effective in improving the patients’ emotional experience and motivation to achieve a better quality of life [2], a holistic evaluation of candidates to BCI interventions, including both interoceptive sensitivity and mental imagery abilities, could better predict the intervention outcome.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci14080832/s1, Table S1; Table S2.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.L.S., E.M. and Ž.Z.; methodology, E.L.S., E.M. and Ž.Z.; formal analysis, E.L.S., E.M. and Ž.Z.; investigation, E.M. and Ž.Z.; resources, E.L.S.; data curation, E.M. and Ž.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, E.L.S., E.M. and Ž.Z.; writing—review and editing, E.L.S., E.M. and Ž.Z.; visualization, E.M. and Ž.Z.; supervision, E.L.S.; funding acquisition, E.L.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Pisa University (Santarcangelo, Ateneo, 2019).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Bioethical Committee of the University of Pisa (protocol code 29/2022, 29 June 2022) for studies involving humans.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The original data presented in this study are available upon request.

Acknowledgments

This article was produced by Eleonora Malloggi (CUP E63C22001340001) while attending the Ph.D. program in Space Science and Technology at the University of Trento, Cycle XXXVIII, with the support of a scholarship financed by the Ministerial Decree no. 352 of 9 April 2022, and by Žan Zelič (CUPE66E23000110001) while attending the PhD program in Space Science and Technology at the University of Trento, Cycle XXXIX, with the support of a scholarship financed by the Ministerial Decree no. 118 of 2 March 2023, based on the NRRP—funded by the European Union—NextGenerationEU—Mission 4 “Education and Research”, Component 1 “Enhancement of the offer of educational services: from nurseries to universities”—Investment 4.1 “Extension of the number of research doctorates and innovative doctorates for public administration and cultural heritage”.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of this study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Jeannerod, M.; Decety, J. Mental Motor Imagery: A Window into the Representational Stages of Action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1995, 5, 727–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Mane, R.; Chouhan, T.; Guan, C. BCI for Stroke Rehabilitation: Motor and Beyond. J. Neural Eng. 2020, 17, 041001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Jeunet, C.; N’Kaoua, B.; Lotte, F. Advances in User-Training for Mental-Imagery-Based BCI Control. In Progress in Brain Research; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2016; Volume 228, pp. 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Saha, S.; Baumert, M. Intra- and Inter-Subject Variability in EEG-Based Sensorimotor Brain Computer Interface: A Review. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2020, 13, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Leriche, R.B.; Jackson, N.A.; Peterson, K.; Aspandiar, Z.; Hufnagel, V.; Swann, N.C. Reduced Sensorimotor Beta Dynamics Could Represent a “Slowed Movement State” in Healthy Individuals. Neuropsychologia 2022, 172, 108276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Borra, D.; Fantozzi, S.; Bisi, M.C.; Magosso, E. Modulations of Cortical Power and Connectivity in Alpha and Beta Bands during the Preparation of Reaching Movements. Sensors 2023, 23, 3530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Steriade, M.; Llinás, R.R. The Functional States of the Thalamus and the Associated Neuronal Interplay. Physiol. Rev. 1988, 68, 649–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Opri, E.; Cernera, S.; Okun, M.S.; Foote, K.D.; Gunduz, A. The Functional Role of Thalamocortical Coupling in the Human Motor Network. J. Neurosci. 2019, 39, 8124–8134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Neuper, C.; Scherer, R.; Reiner, M.; Pfurtscheller, G. Imagery of Motor Actions: Differential Effects of Kinesthetic and Visual–Motor Mode of Imagery in Single-Trial EEG. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 25, 668–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Engel, A.K.; Fries, P. Beta-Band Oscillations—Signalling the Status Quo? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2010, 20, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Jeannerod, M. The Representing Brain: Neural Correlates of Motor Intention and Imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 1994, 17, 187–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Decety, J. Do Imagined and Executed Actions Share the Same Neural Substrate? Cogn. Brain Res. 1996, 3, 87–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Collet, C.; Guillot, A. Autonomic Nervous System Activities during Imagined Movements. In The Neurophysiological Foundations of Mental and Motor Imagery; Guillot, A., Collet, C., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 95–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Fontanelli, L.; Spina, V.; Chisari, C.; Siciliano, G.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Is Hypnotic Assessment Relevant to Neurology? Neurol. Sci. 2022, 43, 4655–4661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Malloggi, E.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Physiological Correlates of Hypnotizability: Hypnotic Behaviour and Prognostic Role in Medicine. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Collet, C.; Guillot, A.; Lebon, F.; MacIntyre, T.; Moran, A. Measuring Motor Imagery Using Psychometric, Behavioral, and Psychophysiological Tools. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2011, 39, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Raimo, S.; Di Vita, A.; Boccia, M.; Iona, T.; Cropano, M.; Gaita, M.; Guariglia, C.; Grossi, D.; Palermo, L. The Body across the Lifespan: On the Relation between Interoceptive Sensibility and High-Order Body Representations. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Craig, A.D. How Do You Feel? Interoception: The Sense of the Physiological Condition of the Body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 655–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Garfinkel, S.N.; Seth, A.K.; Barrett, A.B.; Suzuki, K.; Critchley, H.D. Knowing Your Own Heart: Distinguishing Interoceptive Accuracy from Interoceptive Awareness. Biol. Psychol. 2015, 104, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Mehling, W.E.; Acree, M.; Stewart, A.; Silas, J.; Jones, A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0208034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Tinaz, S.; Para, K.; Vives-Rodriguez, A.; Martinez-Kaigi, V.; Nalamada, K.; Sezgin, M.; Scheinost, D.; Hampson, M.; Louis, E.D.; Constable, R.T. Insula as the Interface Between Body Awareness and Movement: A Neurofeedback-Guided Kinesthetic Motor Imagery Study in Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Zhang, R.; Deng, H.; Xiao, X. The Insular Cortex: An Interface Between Sensation, Emotion and Cognition. Neurosci. Bull. 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural Systems Supporting Interoceptive Awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Fermin, A.S.R.; Sasaoka, T.; Maekawa, T.; Ono, K.; Chan, H.-L.; Yamawaki, S. Insula-Cortico-Subcortical Networks Predict Interoceptive Awareness and Stress Resilience. Asian J. Psychiatry 2024, 95, 103991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Beckmann, N.; Baumann, P.; Herpertz, S.; Trojan, J.; Diers, M. How the Unconscious Mind Controls Body Movements: Body Schema Distortion in Anorexia Nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2021, 54, 578–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Meregalli, V.; Tenconi, E.; Madan, C.R.; Somà, E.; Meneguzzo, P.; Ceccato, E.; Zuanon, S.; Sala, A.; Favaro, A.; Collantoni, E. Beyond Body Image: What Body Schema and Motor Imagery Can Tell Us about the Way Patients with Anorexia Nervosa Experience Their Body. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2023, 77, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Piccione, C.; Hilgard, E.R.; Zimbardo, P.G. On the Degree of Stability of Measured Hypnotizability over a 25-Year Period. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 289–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Houzé, B.; Streff, A.; Piché, M.; Rainville, P. Spinal and Supraspinal Modulation of Pain Responses by Hypnosis, Suggestions, and Distraction. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2021, 63, 329–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Santarcangelo, E.L. Physiological Correlates of Hypnotizability. In The Routledge International Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis; Linden, J.H., De Benedittis, G., Sugarman, L.I., Varga, K., Eds.; Taylor & Francis: Oxfordshire, UK, 2024; pp. 313–330. [Google Scholar]
  30. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Scattina, E. Complementing the Latest APA Definition of Hypnosis: Sensory-Motor and Vascular Peculiarities Involved in Hypnotizability. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2016, 64, 318–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Cesari, P.; Modenese, M.; Benedetti, S.; Emadi Andani, M.; Fiorio, M. Hypnosis-Induced Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability during Motor Imagery. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 16882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Ibáñez-Marcelo, E.; Campioni, L.; Manzoni, D.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Petri, G. Spectral and Topological Analyses of the Cortical Representation of the Head Position: Does Hypnotizability Matter? Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Landry, M.; Lifshitz, M.; Raz, A. Brain Correlates of Hypnosis: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analytic Exploration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 81, 75–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Picerni, E.; Santarcangelo, E.; Laricchiuta, D.; Cutuli, D.; Petrosini, L.; Spalletta, G.; Piras, F. Cerebellar Structural Variations in Subjects with Different Hypnotizability. Cerebellum 2019, 18, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Rosati, A.; Belcari, I.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Sebastiani, L. Interoceptive Accuracy as a Function of Hypnotizability. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2021, 69, 441–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Giusti, G.; Zelič, Ž.; Callara, A.L.; Sebastiani, L.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Interoception as a Function of Hypnotizability during Rest and a Heartbeat Counting Task. Psychophysiology 2024, 61, e14535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Diolaiuti, F.; Huber, A.; Ciaramella, A.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Sebastiani, L. Hypnotisability-Related Interoceptive Awareness and Inhibitory/Activating Emotional Traits. Arch. Ital. Biol. 2020, 157, 111–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Weitzenhoffer, A.M.; Hilgard, E.R. The Stanford Scales of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Forms A and B; Consulting Psychologists Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
  39. De Pascalis, V.D.; Bellusci, A.; Russo, P.M. Italian Norms for the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C 1. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2000, 48, 315–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Tellegen, A.; Atkinson, G. Openness to Absorbing and Self-Altering Experiences (“absorption”), a Trait Related to Hypnotic Susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1974, 83, 268–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An Open Source Toolbox for Analysis of Single-Trial EEG Dynamics Including Independent Component Analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Perrin, F.; Pernier, J.; Bertrand, O.; Echallier, J.F. Spherical Splines for Scalp Potential and Current Density Mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1989, 72, 184–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Makeig, S.; Bell, A.J.; Jung, T.-P.; Sejnowski, T.J. Independent Component Analysis of Electroencephalographic Data. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 8; Touretzky, D., Mozer, M.C., Hasselmo, M., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 145–151. [Google Scholar]
  44. Thanapaisal, S.; Mosher, S.; Trejo, B.; Robbins, K. EEG-Beats: Automated Analysis of Heart Rate Variability (HVR) from EEG-EKG. BioRxiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Glover, S.; Bibby, E.; Tuomi, E. Executive Functions in Motor Imagery: Support for the Motor-Cognitive Model over the Functional Equivalence Model. Exp. Brain Res. 2020, 238, 931–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Glover, S.; Baran, M. The Motor-Cognitive Model of Motor Imagery: Evidence from Timing Errors in Simulated Reaching and Grasping. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2017, 43, 1359–1375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Scattina, E.; Carli, G.; Ghelarducci, B.; Orsini, P.; Manzoni, D. Can Imagery Become Reality? Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 206, 329–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Williams, S.E.; Guillot, A.; Di Rienzo, F.; Cumming, J. Comparing Self-report and Mental Chronometry Measures of Motor Imagery Ability. Eur. J. Sport. Sci. 2015, 15, 703–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Carli, G.; Cavallaro, F.I.; Rendo, C.A.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Imagery of Different Sensory Modalities: Hypnotizability and Body Sway. Exp. Brain Res. 2007, 179, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cavallaro, F.I.; Cacace, I.; Del Testa, M.; Andre, P.; Carli, G.; De Pascalis, V.; Rocchi, R.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Hypnotizability-Related EEG Alpha and Theta Activities during Visual and Somesthetic Imageries. Neurosci. Lett. 2010, 470, 13–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Shapiro, L. Embodied Cognition, 2nd ed.; New Problems of Philosophy Series; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ruggirello, S.; Campioni, L.; Piermanni, S.; Sebastiani, L.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Does Hypnotic Assessment Predict the Functional Equivalence between Motor Imagery and Action? Brain Cogn. 2019, 136, 103598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Van Der Lubbe, R.H.J.; Sobierajewicz, J.; Jongsma, M.L.A.; Verwey, W.B.; Przekoracka-Krawczyk, A. Frontal Brain Areas Are More Involved during Motor Imagery than during Motor Execution/Preparation of a Response Sequence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2021, 164, 71–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kilavik, B.E.; Zaepffel, M.; Brovelli, A.; MacKay, W.A.; Riehle, A. The Ups and Downs of Beta Oscillations in Sensorimotor Cortex. Exp. Neurol. 2013, 245, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Filgueiras, A.; Quintas Conde, E.F.; Hall, C.R. The Neural Basis of Kinesthetic and Visual Imagery in Sports: An ALE Meta-Analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018, 12, 1513–1523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Yang, Y.J.; Jeon, E.J.; Kim, J.S.; Chung, C.K. Characterization of Kinesthetic Motor Imagery Compared with Visual Motor Imageries. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 3751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gruzelier, J.H. Frontal Functions, Connectivity and Neural Efficiency Underpinning Hypnosis and Hypnotic Susceptibility. Contemp. Hypn. 2006, 23, 15–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Naish, P.L.N. Hypnosis and Hemispheric Asymmetry. Conscious. Cogn. 2010, 19, 230–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Amemiya, K.; Ishizu, T.; Ayabe, T.; Kojima, S. Effects of Motor Imagery on Intermanual Transfer: A near-Infrared Spectroscopy and Behavioural Study. Brain Res. 2010, 1343, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Jensen, M.P.; Jamieson, G.A.; Lutz, A.; Mazzoni, G.; McGeown, W.J.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Demertzi, A.; De Pascalis, V.; Bányai, É.I.; Rominger, C.; et al. New Directions in Hypnosis Research: Strategies for Advancing the Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience of Hypnosis. Neurosci. Conscious. 2017, 2017, nix004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Young, M.J.; Fecchio, M.; Bodien, Y.G.; Edlow, B.L. Covert Cortical Processing: A Diagnosis in Search of a Definition. Neurosci. Conscious. 2024, 2024, niad026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. De Lange, F.P.; Helmich, R.C.; Toni, I. Posture Influences Motor Imagery: An fMRI Study. NeuroImage 2006, 33, 609–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Saimpont, A.; Malouin, F.; Tousignant, B.; Jackson, P.L. The Influence of Body Configuration on Motor Imagery of Walking in Younger and Older Adults. Neuroscience 2012, 222, 49–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ridderinkhof, K.R.; Brass, M. How Kinesthetic Motor Imagery Works: A Predictive-Processing Theory of Visualization in Sports and Motor Expertise. J. Physiol. Paris 2015, 109, 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Fu, J.; Chen, S.; Jia, J. Sensorimotor Rhythm-Based Brain–Computer Interfaces for Motor Tasks Used in Hand Upper Extremity Rehabilitation after Stroke: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2022, 13, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Brunner, I.; Lundquist, C.B.; Pedersen, A.R.; Spaich, E.G.; Dosen, S.; Savic, A. Brain Computer Interface Training with Motor Imagery and Functional Electrical Stimulation for Patients with Severe Upper Limb Paresis after Stroke: A Randomized Controlled Pilot Trial. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 2024, 21, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental design of the study. Blocks represent the experimental conditions, which were separated by a 30 s rest. Each imagery condition was repeated 3 times.
Figure 1. Experimental design of the study. Blocks represent the experimental conditions, which were separated by a 30 s rest. Each imagery condition was repeated 3 times.
Brainsci 14 00832 g001
Figure 2. Visual and kinesthetic imagery. Subjectively reported efficacy (a) and chronometric values (b). Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences.
Figure 2. Visual and kinesthetic imagery. Subjectively reported efficacy (a) and chronometric values (b). Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences.
Brainsci 14 00832 g002
Figure 4. Alpha PSD. Region × condition × group interaction. FC region (a) and PO region (b). Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences (differences between M and V/K are presented only if V/K are different from B).
Figure 4. Alpha PSD. Region × condition × group interaction. FC region (a) and PO region (b). Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences (differences between M and V/K are presented only if V/K are different from B).
Brainsci 14 00832 g004
Figure 7. High-beta PSD. Region × condition interaction. Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences (differences between M and V/K are presented only if V/K are different from B).
Figure 7. High-beta PSD. Region × condition interaction. Mean values and standard errors. Lines indicate significant differences (differences between M and V/K are presented only if V/K are different from B).
Brainsci 14 00832 g007
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the studied variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the studied variables.
LowsMediumsHighs
MSDMSDMSD
TAS * 19.555.2624.274.0524.313.75
MAIANoticing *3.180.663.670.783.720.63
not distracting1.940.601.760.532.021.01
not worrying2.760.852.441.332.460.92
attention regulation3.080.833.200.673.350.54
emotional awareness3.440.803.660.944.010.63
self-regulation2.860.863.220.792.810.87
body listening2.560.782.990.932.960.80
trusting3.481.113.791.023.371.17
ΔHRv 6.413.518.534.947.056.11
ΔHRk 3.372.613.442.064.063.68
ΔV 0.280.220.290.190.350.22
ΔK 0.330.250.280.240.260.15
Ve 6.681.166.701.226.771.72
Ke * 6.781.177.821.197.581.09
Note: * significant differences among hypnotizability groups. For details, see the text.
Table 2. ANOVA results for alpha PSD.
Table 2. ANOVA results for alpha PSD.
Effect Fdfpη2αMAIA
side 4.091, 500.0480.7540.510ns
region 273.891, 500.00010.8460.999
condition 18.743, 1500.00010.2730.999ns
region x condition x group 2.356, 1500.0480.0860.618
with:highs
region 103.681, 150.0001FC < PO
condition 4.043, 450.038
mediums
region 48.221, 100.0001FC < PO
condition 6.373, 300.005
lows
region 177.961, 250.0001FC < PO
condition 12.903, 750.0001
region x condition 3.163, 750.042
Table 3. ANOVA results for low and high-beta PSD.
Table 3. ANOVA results for low and high-beta PSD.
EffectLow-BetaFdfpη2α MAIA
side 7.281, 500.0090.1270.754left < rightns
region 360.731, 500.00010.8770.999FC < POns
condition 37.813, 1500.00010.4310.999 ns
high-beta
side 12.791, 500.00010.2040.939left < rightns
region 196.401, 500.00010.7970.999FC < POns
side x group 3.792, 500.0290.1310.664 ns
condition 6.463, 1500.0010.1140.951 ns
region x condition 3.723, 1500.0220.0690.715 ns
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Malloggi, E.; Zelič, Ž.; Santarcangelo, E.L. The Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity and Hypnotizability in Motor Imagery. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 832. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080832

AMA Style

Malloggi E, Zelič Ž, Santarcangelo EL. The Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity and Hypnotizability in Motor Imagery. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(8):832. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080832

Chicago/Turabian Style

Malloggi, Eleonora, Žan Zelič, and Enrica Laura Santarcangelo. 2024. "The Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity and Hypnotizability in Motor Imagery" Brain Sciences 14, no. 8: 832. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14080832

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop