The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction
Abstract
:1. The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction
2. Traditional Linguistic Analysis of English Articles
3. The English Article Construction: Usage-Based Accounts
4. A Usage-Based Account of Language Development
5. The Present Study
- What categories emerge from second language usage of article cues?
- Does L2 learners’ emergent categorisation of article cues change as a function of increased proficiency? If yes, how?
6. Methodology
6.1. Participants
6.2. Measurement
7. Results
7.1. Emergent Categories in the Low-to-Intermediate Proficiency Group
7.2. Emergent Categories in the Advanced Proficiency Group
7.3. Learner Performance on Categories in Proficiency Groups
8. Discussion
9. Conclusions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Cue No. | Cue Name | Metalinguistic Explanation and Selected Example |
---|---|---|
1 | non–countable with post–modifiers → the | Use the when a non-countable noun is post–modified by a relative clause or a prepositional phrase. (They try to protect the land they own at all cost.) |
2 | non–countable → Ø | Use Ø with unmodified non-countable nouns. (They try to protect Ø land at all cost.) |
3 | plural with post–modifiers → the | Use the when the plural noun is post–modified by a relative clause or a prepositional phrase and is uniquely identifiable. (I have found you the people who used to work as Chinese–English translators.) |
4 | plural → Ø | Use Ø with plural nouns unless they are uniquely identifiable. (Do you need Ø people who can translate English to Chinese?) |
5 | singular countable with post–modifiers → the | Use the when the singular countable noun is post-modified by a relative clause or a prepositional phrase and is uniquely identifiable. (I thanked the travel agency who helped me find a cheap air ticket.) |
6 | singular countable → a/an | Use a/an when the singular countable noun is not made concrete or instantiated by any modifier. (I am working with a travel agency to find a cheap air ticket.) |
7 | geographical feature names → the | Use the with major geographic features such as rivers, oceans, seas, and deserts. (the Ohio River, the Pacific Ocean, the Red Sea, the Sahara Desert) |
8 | exceptions of geographical feature names → Ø | Use Ø with the names of individual lakes, bays, and mountains. (Ø Lake Michigan, Ø Hudson Bay, Ø Mount Whitney) |
9 | construction names → the | Use the with the names of hotels, theatres, bridges, and buildings. (the Hilton Hotel, the Majestic Theatre, the Brooklyn Bridge, the Empire State Building) |
10 | exceptions of construction names → Ø | Use Ø with the names of halls, stadiums, and hospitals. (Ø Carnegie Hall, Ø Yankee Stadium, Ø Hillsdale Hospital) |
11 | XX University/College → Ø | Use Ø when the name of a university has this structure: XX University. (Ø Harvard University) |
12 | the University/College of XX → the | Use the when the name of a university has this structure: the University of XX. (the University of Edinburgh) |
13 | XX Street/Road/Avenue → Ø | Use Ø when the names of streets, roads, avenues, lanes, or boulevards have this structure: XX Street/Road/Avenue/Boulevard. (Ø Fifth Avenue) |
14 | the Street/Road/Avenue of XX → the | Use the when the names of streets, roads, avenues, lanes, or boulevards have this structure: the Street/Road/Avenue/Boulevard of XX. (the Avenue of Stars) |
15 | the architecture of XX → the | Use the when describing constructions that are made specific through the addition of post–modifiers. (the Statue of Liberty) |
16 | XX’s architecture → Ø | Use Ø when the names of constructions are modified by possessive nouns (such as London’s). (Ø New York’s Statue of Liberty) |
17 | political/military institution → the | Use the when established institutions (e.g., political and military institutions) are used alone. (the World Bank) |
18 | political/military institution as adjective → Ø | Use Ø when established institutions (e.g., political and military institutions) are used as adjectives to modify other nouns. (Ø World Bank loans) |
19 | ‘go to’ habitual location → Ø | Use Ø when the word ‘go’ is used with a habitual location or a habitual transport method. (go to Ø school) |
20 | ‘go to’ recreation activity → a/an | Use a/an when the word ‘go’ is used with recreational activities. (go for a dance) |
21 | second mention with variation → the | Use the when the noun has already been mentioned before, and the second way in which it is mentioned is slightly different from the first. (I saw a peacock at the zoo. The bird had beautiful feathers.) |
22 | part of → the | Use the when describing an object that is a unique part of some overall scene, event, or object being discussed. (I’m returning this coat for a refund. The zipper broke after one day.) |
23 | disease names → Ø | Use Ø with the names of diseases (except the flu, the measles, and the mumps). (His uncle has Ø cancer.) |
References
- Ahn, Hyunah. 2019. From interlanguage grammar to target grammar in L2 processing of definiteness as uniqueness. Second Language Research. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akakura, Motoko. 2012. Evaluating the effectiveness of explicit instruction on implicit and explicit knowledge. Language Teaching Research 16: 9–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ambridge, Ben, and Elena Lieven. 2015. A constructivist account of child language acquisition. In The Handbook of Language Emergence. Edited by Brian MacWhinney and William OGrady. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 478–510. [Google Scholar]
- Azazil, Lina. 2020. Frequency effects in the L1 acquisition of the catenative verb construction–evidence from experimental and corpus data. Cognitive Linguistics 31: 417–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bickerton, Derek. 1981. Roots of Language. Ann Arbor: Karoma. [Google Scholar]
- Butler, Yuko Goto. 2002. Second language learners’ theories on the use of English articles: An analysis of the metalinguistic knowledge used by Japanese students in acquiring the English article system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 24: 451–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bybee, Joan. 2008. Usage-based grammar and second language acquisition. In Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Peter Robinson and Nick C. Ellis. New York and London: Routledge, pp. 216–36. [Google Scholar]
- Bybee, Joan. 2010. Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cole, Tom. 2000. The Article Book: Practice Toward Mastering a, an, and the, 2nd ed. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press. [Google Scholar]
- Croft, William, and Alan D. Cruse. 2004. Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Crossley, Scott, and Thomas Lee Salsbury. 2011. The development of lexical bundle accuracy and production in English second language speakers. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 49: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, Mark. 2008. The Corpus of Contemporary American English: 520 Million Words, 1990–Present. Provo: Brigham Young University, Available online: http://corpus.byu.edu/coca (accessed on 1 June 2017).
- Ellis, Nick C. 2003. Constructions, chunking, and connectionism: The emergence of second language structure. In Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Catherine Doughty and Michael H. Long. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 33–68. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, Nick C. 2006. Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics 27: 164–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, Nick C. 2008. Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned attention, and the limited L2 end state. In Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Peter Robinson and Nick C. Ellis. New York: Routledge, pp. 372–405. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, Nick C. 2016. Online processing of verb-argument constructions: Lexical decision and meaningfulness. Language and Cognition 8: 391–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellis, N. C., and Laura Collins. 2009. Input and second language acquisition: The roles of frequency, form, and function. Modern Language Journal 93: 329–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellis, Nick C., and Fernando Ferreira-Junior. 2009. Construction learning as a function of frequency, frequency distribution, and function. Modern Language Journal 93: 370–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellis, Nick C., Rita Simpson-Vlach, and Carson Maynard. 2008. Formulaic language in native and second language speakers: Psycholinguistic, corpus linguistics, and TESOL. TESOL Quarterly 42: 375–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eskildsen, Søren W. 2008. Constructing another language: Usage-based linguistics in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics 30: 335–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eskildsen, Søren W. 2012. L2 negation constructions at work. Language Learning 62: 335–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eskildsen, Søren W. 2015. What counts as a developmental sequence? Exemplar-based L2 learning of English questions. Language Learning 65: 33–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, Vyvyan, and Melanie Green. 2006. Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Fodor, Janet Dean, and Ivan A. Sag. 1982. Referential and quantificational indefinites. Linguistics and Philosophy 5: 355–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García Mayo, Maria del Pilar, and Roger Hawkins. 2009. Second Language Acquisition of Articles: Empirical Findings and Theoretical Implications. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, Adele E. 1999. The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In The Emergence of Language. Edited by Brian MacWhinney. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 197–212. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, Adele E. 2019. Explain me this: Creativity, Competition, and the Partial Productivity of Constructions. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins, Roger, Saleh Al-Eid, Ibrahim Almahboob, Panos Athanasopoulos, Rangsiya Chaengchenkit, James Hu, Mohammad Rezai, Carol Jaensch, Yunju Jeon, Amy Jiang, and et al. 2006. Accounting for English article interpretation by L2 speakers. EUROSLA Yearbook 6: 7–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huebner, Thom. 1983. A Longitudinal Analysis of the Acquisition of English. Ann Arbor: Karoma. [Google Scholar]
- Ionin, Tania, Heejeong Ko, and Kenneth Wexler. 2004. Article semantics in L2 acquisition: The role of specificity. Language Acquisition 12: 3–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jach, Daniel. 2018. A usage-based approach to preposition placement in English as a second language. Language Learning 68: 271–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jespersen, Otto. 1933. Essentials of English Grammar. Boston: George Allen & Unwin. [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, Nan. 2013. Conducting Reaction Time Research in Second Language Studies. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press, vol. I. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, Ronald W. 1991. Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Langacker, Ronald W. 2008. Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Li, Peiwen, Søren W. Eskildsen, and Teresa Cadierno. 2014. Tracing an L2 learner’s motion constructions over time: A usage-based classroom investigation. Modern Language Journal 98: 612–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lieven, Elena, Julian M. Pine, and Gillian Baldwin. 1997. Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language 24: 187–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacWhinney, Brian. 1987. The Competition Model. In Mechanisms of Language Acquisition. Edited by Brian MacWhinney. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 249–308. [Google Scholar]
- Master, Peter. 1990. Teaching the English articles as a binary system. TESOL Quarterly 24: 461–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Master, Peter. 2002. Information structure and English article pedagogy. System 30: 331–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2013. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Radden, Günter, and René Dirven. 2007. Cognitive English Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Roehr-Brackin, Karen. 2014. Explicit knowledge and processes from a usage-based perspective: The developmental trajectory of an instructed L2 learner. Language Learning 64: 771–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Römer, Ute, and Cynthia M. Berger. 2019. Observing the emergence of constructional knowledge: Verb patterns in German and Spanish learners of English at different proficiency levels. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 41: 1089–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sung, Min-Chang, and Hyunwoo Kim. 2020. Effects of verb-construction association on second language constructional generalizations in production and comprehension. Second Language Research. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tarone, Elaine, and Betsy Parrish. 1988. Task-related variation in interlanguage: The case of articles. Language Learning 38: 21–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Trenkic, Danijela. 2008. The representation of English articles in second language grammars: Determiners or adjectives? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verspoor, Marjolijn H., and Nguyen Thi Phuong Huong. 2008. Cognitive Grammar and teaching English articles to Asian students. In Du Fait Grammatical au Fait Cognitive. Edited by Jean-Remi Lapaire, Guillaume Desagulier and Jean-Baptise Guignard. Bordeaux: Presses Universitaires de Bordeaux, vol. 1, pp. 249–68. [Google Scholar]
- Wulff, Stephanie, Nick C. Ellis, Ute Römer, Kathleen Bardovi-Harlig, and Chelsea J. Leblanc. 2009. The acquisition of tense-aspect: Converging evidence from corpora and telicity rating. Modern Language Journal 93: 354–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhao, Helen, and Brian MacWhinney. 2018. The instructed learning of form-function mappings in the English article system. Modern Language Journal 102: 99–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | The distinction between the specific and generic reference of the indefinite is played down in cognitive grammar (Verspoor and Huong 2008). |
2 | Generic use of the definite (e.g., The cat is a mammal) and of the indefinite (e.g., A cat is a mammal) are not included in the current measurement. |
No. | Feature | Form | Semantic Function |
---|---|---|---|
1 | [−SR] [+HK] | the | Generics (e.g., The lion is a beautiful animal.) |
a/an | Generics (e.g., A lion is a beautiful animal.) | ||
Ø | Generics (e.g., Ø Lions are beautiful animals.) | ||
2 | [+SR] [+HK] | the | Unique referent or conventionally assumed unique referent (e.g., The pope) |
Referent physically present (e.g., Ask the guy over there) | |||
Referent previously mentioned in the discourse (e.g., the man you have just said) | |||
Specific referents assumed known to the hearer (e.g., He went over to the book store) | |||
3 | [+SR] [−HK] | a/an Ø | First mention in a discourse of [+SR] NP which is assumed to be not known to the hearer (e.g., Dad gave me a car.) |
First mention of [+SR] NP following existential have and assumed to be not known to the hearer (e.g., Our house has a garage.) | |||
4 | [–SR] [−HK] | a/an Ø | Equative noun phrases (e.g., He is a nice man.) |
Noun phrases in the scope of negation (e.g., I don’t see a pencil.) | |||
Noun phrases in the scope of interrogative (e.g., Do you see a pencil?) | |||
Noun phrases in irrealis scope (e.g., If I had a million dollars, I’d buy a big yacht.) |
Category | Cue No. | Cue Name |
---|---|---|
Category 1 | 1 | non–countable with post–modifiers → the |
3 | plural with post–modifiers → the | |
5 | singular countable with post–modifiers → the | |
7 | geographical feature names → the | |
9 | construction names → the | |
12 | the University/College of XX → the | |
14 | the Street/Road/Avenue of XX → the | |
15 | the architecture of XX → the | |
17 | political/military institution → the | |
21 | second mention with variation → the | |
22 | part of → the | |
20 | ‘go to’ recreation activity → a/an | |
Category 2 | 2 | non–countable → Ø |
4 | plural → Ø | |
6 | singular countable → a/an | |
19 | ‘go to’ habitual location → Ø | |
23 | disease names → Ø | |
Category 3 | 8 | exceptions of geographical feature names → Ø |
10 | exceptions of construction names → Ø | |
11 | XX University/College → Ø | |
13 | XX Street/Road/Avenue → Ø | |
16 | XX’s architecture → Ø | |
18 | political/military institution as adjective → Ø |
Category | Cue No. | Cue Name |
---|---|---|
Category 1 | 1 | non–countable with post–modifiers → the |
3 | plural with post–modifiers → the | |
5 | singular countable with post–modifiers → the | |
20 | ‘go to’ recreation activity → a/an | |
21 | second mention with variation → the | |
22 | part of → the | |
Category 2 | 2 | non–countable → Ø |
4 | plural → Ø | |
6 | singular countable → a/an | |
19 | ‘go to’ habitual location → Ø | |
23 | disease names → Ø | |
Category 3 | 8 | exceptions of geographical feature names → Ø |
10 | exceptions of construction names → Ø | |
11 | XX University/College → Ø | |
13 | XX Street/Road/Avenue → Ø | |
16 | XX’s architecture → Ø | |
18 | political/military institution as adjective → Ø | |
Category 4 | 7 | geographical feature names → the |
9 | construction names → the | |
12 | the University/College of XX → the | |
14 | the Street/Road/Avenue of XX → the | |
15 | the architecture of XX → the | |
17 | political/military institution → the |
Proficiency Group | Article Category | Mean Accuracy | SD | Mean Response Time | SD |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low-to-intermediate | 1 | 0.64 | 0.13 | 6.76 | 1.49 |
2 | 0.32 | 0.17 | 6.95 | 1.44 | |
3 | 0.29 | 0.23 | 6.15 | 1.51 | |
Advanced | 1 | 0.89 | 0.06 | 5.85 | 0.85 |
2 | 0.67 | 0.16 | 6.21 | 0.98 | |
3 | 0.51 | 0.23 | 6.54 | 1.31 | |
4 | 0.64 | 0.21 | 6.12 | 1.30 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2020 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zhao, H. The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction. Languages 2020, 5, 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040054
Zhao H. The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction. Languages. 2020; 5(4):54. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040054
Chicago/Turabian StyleZhao, Helen. 2020. "The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction" Languages 5, no. 4: 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040054
APA StyleZhao, H. (2020). The Emergence of Second Language Categorisation of the English Article Construction. Languages, 5(4), 54. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages5040054