Feature Borrowing in Language Contact
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(1) | [In balt] | [haüt] | [dar pua] | hatt | gesek | in has |
in-the woods | today | the boy | has | seen | the hare | |
‘In the woods today, the boy saw the hare’. |
- (i)
- Subject–finite verb inversion, although this is limited to the pronominal subject:
(2) a. Gestarn in balt hatt=ar nèt gesek in has yesterday in-the wood has=he.cl not seen the.acc hare ‘Yesterday, he didn’t see the hare in the wood’. b. *Gestarn in balt hatt dar pua nèt gesek in has yesterday in-the wood has the.nom boy not seen the.acc hare - (ii)
- Residual asymmetry between the root and the embedded word order pattern, essentially based on the relative order of the negative particle (nèt) with respect to the finite verb (Vfin NEG versus NEG Vfin):
(3) | Haüt | geat=ar | nèt | ka | Trento | ||
today | go=he.cl | not | to | Tria | |||
‘Today, he doesn’t go to Trento’. | |||||||
(4) | (I speràr), | az=ar | nèt | gea | ka Tria | haüt | |
I hope | that=he.cl | not | go.sbjv | to Trento | today | ||
‘I hope that he doesn’t go to Trento today’. |
(5) | Dar | geat | nèt | ka | Tria | |||
he | go | not | to | Trento | ||||
‘He doesn’t go to Trento’. | ||||||||
(6) | a. | (I speràr), | az=ar | nèt | gea | ka Tria | ||
I hope | that=he.cl | not | go.sbjv | to Trento | ||||
‘I hope that he doesn’t go to Trento’. | ||||||||
b. | (I boaz), | ke | dar | geat | nèt | ka Tria | ||
I know | that | he | go | not | to Trento | |||
‘I know that he doesn’t go to Trento’. |
- -
- az always requires the subjunctive mood, and typically introduces a declarative clause selected by non-factive (volitional) verbs such as bölln ‘to want’ and non-assertive (affective) verbs such as speràrn ‘to hope’ (see 6a above).
- -
- ke introduces embedded declarative clauses in the indicative mood that are selected by strongly assertive verbs such as khön ‘to say’ or semi-factive (knowledge) verbs such as bizzan ‘to know’ (see 6b above), perceptive verbs such as seng ‘to see’ and weakly assertive (epistemic) ones such as pensàrn ‘to think’.
2. The Specialization of az versus ke
(7) |
3. A First Exception to the System: ke with the Subjunctive Mood
(8) | Italian stimulus sentence: | Loro | credono | che | (lui) | sia | arrivato | tardi | |||
they | believe | that | (he) | be.sbjv | arrived | late | |||||
‘They believe that he arrived late’. | |||||||||||
(9) | a. | Sa | gloam | ke | dar | iz | gerift spet | (ke + Ind) | |||
they | believe | that | he | is.ind | arrived late | ||||||
b. | Sa | gloam | azz=ar | sai(be) | gerift | spet | (az + Subjv) | ||||
they | believe | that=he.cl | is.subjv | arrived | late | ||||||
c. | Sa | gloam | ke | dar | sai(be) | gerift | spet | (ke + Subjv) | |||
they | believe | that | he | is.subjv | arrived | late | |||||
d. | *Sa | gloam | azz=ar | iz | gerift spet | *(az + Ind) | |||||
they | believe | that=he.cl | is.ind | arrived late |
(10) | Italian stimulus sentence: | Non | è | detto | che | Gianni | venga | con | noi | |||
not | is | said | that | Gianni | come.subjv | with | us | |||||
‘It is not certain that Gianni will come with us’. | ||||||||||||
(11) | ’Z | iz | nèt | khött | ke | dar Gianni | khemm | pit | üs | |||
it | is | not | said | that | the Gianni | come.subjv | with | us |
(12) |
4. A Gap in the Paradigm of the Subjunctive Mood: The First-Person Plural
(13) | Italian stimulus sentence: | Credono | che | (noi) | siamo | arrivati | tardi | [+/− realis] | |||
believe.3pp | that | we | are | arrived | late | ||||||
‘They believe that we arrived late’. | |||||||||||
(14) | a. | Sa | gloam | ke | bar | soin | gerift | spet | |||
they | believe | that | we | are | arrived | late | |||||
b. | *Sa | gloam | az=par | soin | gerift | spet | |||||
they | believe | that=we.cl | are | arrived | late |
(15) | Credo | di | essere | in ritardo | I | goabe | zo soina | spet | ||||
believe.1ps | to | be | late | I | believe | to be.inf.fl | late | |||||
(16) | a. | Credo | che | tu | sia | in ritardo | I | gloabe | az=to | sai(be)st | spet | |
believe.1ps | that | you | be.subjv | late | I | believe | that=you.cl | be.2s.subjv | late | |||
b. | I | gloabe | ke | du | pist | spet | ||||||
I | believe | that | you | are.ind | late | |||||||
(17) | a. | Credo | che | lui | sia | in ritardo | I | gloabe | azz=ar | sai(be) | spet | |
believe.1ps | that | he | be.subjv | late | I | believe | that=he.cl | be.3s.subjv | late | |||
b. | I | gloabe | ke | dar | iz | spet | ||||||
I | believe | that | he | is.ind | late | |||||||
(18) | a. | Credo | che | noi | siamo | in ritardo | *I | gloabe | az=par | soin | spet | |
believe.1ps | that | we | be.ind/subjv | late | I | believe | that=we.cl | be.1p.ind/subjv | late | |||
b. | I | gloabe | ke | bar | soin | spet | ||||||
I | believe | that | we | are.ind | late | |||||||
(19) | a. | Credo | che | voi | siate | in ritardo | I | gloabe | azz=ar | sait | spet | |
believe.1ps | that | you | be.subjv | late | I | believe | that=you.cl | be.2p.ind/subjv | late | |||
b. | I | gloabe | ke | dar | sait | spet | ||||||
I | believe | that | you | are.ind | late | |||||||
(20) | a. | Credo | che | loro | siano | in ritardo | I | gloabe | az=ze | soin | spet | |
believe.1ps | that | you | be.subjv | late | I | believe | that=they.cl | be.3p.ind/subjv | late | |||
b. | I | gloabe | ke | se | soin | spet | ||||||
I | believe | that | they | are.ind | late |
(21) |
5. Conclusions
- (i)
- Syntactic knowledge, particularly the knowledge of phrase structure and word order, appears to be more resilient to incomplete acquisition under reduced input conditions than is inflectional morphology. In other words, the structure of the C domain is not only maintained, but is even enriched due to the pressure of the model language. In fact, the borrowing of the lexical complementizer ke resulted in an innovation within the Cimbrian system that is not found in either Italian or German, namely a specialized introduction for embedded clauses selected by factive versus non-factive verbs; furthermore, an embedded sentence introduced by ke does not manifest root-embedded asymmetry, but maintains the root word order pattern characterized by structural V2.
- (ii)
- The relative stability of syntactic features compared to morpho-phonological exponents. Our research revealed that the feature characterization of C as [+Fin], [+Mood] was definitely more stable than were the corresponding morpho-phonological exponents (subjunctive versus indicative)
- (iii)
- A prevalence for ‘defaults’; that is, the prevalence of the indicative mood with respect to the subjunctive mood and the dismantling of the root versus embedded word order asymmetry in favor of the root word order pattern.
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | |
2 | See, amongst others, Grewendorf and Poletto (2011), Bidese and Tomaselli (2018), and Bidese et al. (2020). |
3 | Again, we refer to a large amount of research on this topic. In particular, see Grewendorf and Poletto (2009, 2011), Padovan (2011), Bidese et al. (2012, 2014), Padovan et al. (2016), Bidese and Tomaselli (2016), and Bidese (2017). |
4 | We follow the proposal by Grewendorf and Poletto (2011, based on Bhatt and Yoon 1991), who differentiated between complementizers that act as indicators of mood, such as the Cimbrian az ‘that’, and complementizers that act as pure subordinators (see Grewendorf and Poletto 2011, p. 318, Note 10 and Bidese 2017). |
5 | More precisely, we should describe the borrowing of functional words in terms of lexicalization or spelling out of the highest structural position of the Cimbrian C-domain by a lexical item taken from Italian functional lexicon. What matters to us is that the transfer of functional words does not imply the borrowing of a chunk of structure, but it works just like a filler of a new position created by the internal development of the language. |
6 | In this sense, we assume that the functional loanword ke enter the system ‘from above’ into the Cimbrian clause spine, affecting the highest portion of the C-domain. This assumption is supported by several facts, already discussed. |
7 | For example, this is the case in the Salentino dialect, which uses the complementizer ca + Ind in declarative or epistemic contexts and cu + Subjv in modal contexts (see Calabrese 1993; Damonte 2010). The same phenomenon can be found in southern Calabrian dialects (ca versus mu/ma/mi; see Rohlfs 1969, § 786a and Trumper and Rizzi 1985), as well as in some other southern Italian varieties (see Ledgeway 2004, 2005, 2007). A differentiation between declarative and volitional contexts and complementizer selection has also been identified in Romanian (see Farkas 1992), Albanian, Bulgarian (see Krapova 2010; Metzeltin 2016, p. 157), and Greek (see Giannakidou 1998, 2009, 2013). For a general overview of different complementizers depending on factive versus non-factive (modal) predicates in Slavonic, see Hansen et al. (2016). |
8 | The acquisition of embedded clauses in Italian–German bilingual children displays a similar phenomenon. Traute Taeschner’s (1983) study of her four- and five-year-old daughters showed that bilingual children produced the first embedded clauses in German earlier than did corresponding monolingual children, approximately at the same time as in Italian, but in the ‘wrong’ order; that is, with the root word order pattern: the lexical complementizer dass ‘that’ was used to introduce a V2 clause allowing subject–finite verb inversion. The correct asymmetrical word order, that is the final position of the finite verb, appeared later than it did in monolingual children, who apparently produce embedded clauses with the correct word order from the first attestations. In other words, the contact with Italian in the bilingual brain fills in a ‘gap’ in the process of acquiring German (characterized by a late maturation of the embedded structure) with a temporary innovation that will be dismissed at the appropriate acquisition stage (for a discussion of the data, see Alessandra; Carpene 1999). |
9 | The empirical data have been already published and discussed in detail in Bidese et al. (2013). We refer to this study for further explanations; also see Bidese (2017, pp. 143–45). |
10 | An anonymous reviewer notes that it is rather stipulative to assume that the complementizer che enters the Cimbrian system without a mood feature resulting in default indicative marking as in (10b), whereas only later on the mood feature of the Italian system is imported (cf. 10d), and asks for evidence for such a stepwise process instead of a single transfer of che plus subjunctive from the Italian system into the Cimbrian one. Let us put it more precisely in the following way: the evidence for this stepwise assumption is based on the fact that the verbs selected by ke in Cimbrian are only factive or semi-factive and that they are only compatible with the default (not selected/ not specified) value. On the contrary, che in Italian selects either indicative mood with factive verbs or subjunctive with non-factive verbs; when che enters the Cimbrian C-domain it specializes for the introduction of embedded clauses selected by factive verbs. Later on [+subjunctive] is imported as mood feature (cf. 12d) under the pressure of Italian (cf. 12c) in translation tasks which involve non-factive verb plus ke plus subjunctive, violating the Cimbrian ‘specialized’ system (cf. 12a,b). |
11 | An anonymous reviewer surmised that the Cimbrian speaker translated the sentence using the complementizer ke since only this complementizer is compatible with both indicative and subjunctive forms; hence, s/he believes that these data do not indicate any deep property or change in the Cimbrian system, but simply constitute a conscious decision in the translation task. We totally refuse this interpretation. The complementizer ke is not compatible with both mood forms, but only with the indicative one. The syncretic interpretation is present only in Italian. The deletion of the sentence with az in the first-person plural (cf. 18) is rather induced by the attrition with Italian during the translation task in the bilingual mind/brain of the speakers. It shows, nevertheless, in which direction the change of the Cimbrian system may evolve, namely, expanding the domain of ke with respect to az and spreading the symmetric word order. In fact, we never find az plus indicative. Remember that ke introduces a clause that does not display word order asymmetry with the root clause; hence, its expansion to subjunctive and perhaps non-factive verbs reduces the specialization space of az, weakening the whole V2 system, as root-embedded asymmetry is one of the fundamental correlates of the V2 phenomenon. |
References
- Aboh, Enoch Oladé. 2015. The Emergence of Hybrid Grammars. Language Contact and Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bhatt, Rakesh, and James Yoon. 1991. On the Composition of COMP and Parameters of V2. In The Proceedings of the 10th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Edited by Dawn Bates. Stanford: Stanford Linguistic Association, pp. 41–52. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo. 2004. Die Zimbern und ihre Sprache: Geographische, historische und sprachwissenschaftlich relevante Aspekte. In “Alte” Sprachen. Beiträge zum Bremer Kolloquium über “Alte Sprachen und Sprachstufen”. Edited by Thomas Stolz. Bochum: Brockmeyer, pp. 3–42. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo. 2017. Reassessing contact linguistics: Signposts towards an explanatory approach to language contact. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik 84: 126–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo. 2021. Introducing Cimbrian. The main linguistic features of a German(ic) language in Italy. Energeia 46: 19–62. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2016. The decline of asymmetric word order in Cimbrian subordination and the special case of umbrómm. In Co- and Subordination in German and Other Languages. Edited by Ingo Reich and Augustin Speyer. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag, pp. 55–75. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2018. Developing pro-drop: The case of Cimbrian. In Null Subjects in Generative Grammar. A Synchronic and Diachronic Perspective. Edited by Federica Cognola and Jan Casalicchio. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 52–69. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, Andrea Padovan, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2012. A binary system of complementizers in Cimbrian relative clauses. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax 90: 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, Andrea Padovan, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2013. Bilingual competence, complementizer selection and mood in Cimbrian. In Dialektologie in Neuem Gewand. Zu Mikro-/Varietätenlinguistik, Sprachenvergleich und Universalgrammatik. Edited by Werner Abraham and Elisabeth Leiss. Hamburg: Buske, pp. 47–58. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, Andrea Padovan, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2014. The syntax of subordination in Cimbrian and the rationale behind language contact. Language Typology and Universals—STUF Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung 67: 489–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, Andrea Padovan, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2020. Rethinking V2 and Nominative case assignment: New insights from a Germanic variety in Northern Italy. In Rethinking Verb Second. Edited by Rebecca Woods and Sam Wolfe. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 575–93. [Google Scholar]
- Calabrese, Andrea. 1993. The sentential complementation of Salentino: A study of a language without infinitival clauses. In Syntactic Theory and the Dialects of ITALY. Edited by Adriana Belletti. Torino: Rosenberg & Sellier, pp. 29–98. [Google Scholar]
- Carpene, Alessandra. 1999. Bilinguismo precoce: Alcuni aspetti sintattici. In Studi su Fenomeni, Situazioni e Forme del Bilinguismo. Edited by Augusto Carli. Bolzano and Bozen: Franco Angeli. [Google Scholar]
- Damonte, Federico. 2010. Matching moods: Mood concord between CP and IP in Salentino and southern Calabrian subjunctive complements. In Mapping the Left Periphery. Edited by Paola Benincà and Nicola Munaro. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 228–56. [Google Scholar]
- Farkas, Donka. 1992. On the Semantics of Subjunctive Complements. In Romance Languages and Modern Linguistic Theory. Edited by Paul Hirschbühler and Ernst Frederyk Konrad Koerner. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 67–104. [Google Scholar]
- Giannakidou, Anastasia. 1998. Polarity Sensitivity as (Non)veridical Dependency. Amsterdam: Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2009. The dependency of the subjunctive revisited. Temporal semantics and polarity. Lingua 120: 1883–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2013. (Non)veridicality, evaluation, and event actualization. Evidence from the subjunctive in relative clauses. In Nonveridicality and Evaluation. Theoretical, Computational, and Corpus Approaches. Edited by Maite Taboada and Rada Trnavac. Leiden: Brill, pp. 17–49. [Google Scholar]
- Grewendorf, Günther, and Cecilia Poletto. 2009. The hybrid complementizer system of Cimbrian. In Proceedings XXXV Incontro di Grammatica Generativa. Edited by Vincenzo Moscati and Emilio Servidio. Siena: Centro Interdipartimentale di Studi Cognitivi sul Linguaggio, pp. 181–94. [Google Scholar]
- Grewendorf, Günther, and Cecilia Poletto. 2011. Hidden verb second: The case of Cimbrian. In Studies on German Language-Islands. Edited by Michael T. Putnam. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 301–46. [Google Scholar]
- Hansen, Björn, Alexander Letuchiy, and Izabela Błaszczyk. 2016. Complementizers in Slavonic (Russian, Polish, and Bulgarian). In Complementizer Semantics in European Languages. Edited by Kasper Boye and Petar Kehayov. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 175–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krapova, Iliana. 2010. Bulgarian relative and factive clauses with an invariant complementizer. Lingua 120: 1240–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ledgeway, Adam. 2004. Il sistema completivo dei dialetti meridionali: La doppia serie di complementatori. Rivista Italiana di Dialettologia 27: 89–147. [Google Scholar]
- Ledgeway, Adam. 2005. Moving through the left periphery: The dual complementiser system in the dialects of southern Italy. Transactions of the Philological Society 103: 336–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ledgeway, Adam. 2007. Diachrony and finiteness: Subordination in the dialects of southern Italy. In Finiteness: Theoretical and Empirical Foundations. Edited by Irina Nikolaeva. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 335–65. [Google Scholar]
- Lohndal, Terje, Jason Rothman, Tanja Kupisch, and Marit Westergaard. 2019. Heritage language acquisition: What it reveals and why it is important for formal linguistic theories. Language and Linguistics Compass 13: e12357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Metzeltin, Michael. 2016. Das Rumänische in romanischen Kontrast. Eine sprachtypologische Betrachtung. Berlin: Frank & Timme. [Google Scholar]
- Moseley, Christopher. 2010. Atlas of the World’s Languages in Danger. Paris: The United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). [Google Scholar]
- Padovan, Andrea. 2011. Diachronic clues to grammaticalization phenomena in the Cimbrian CP. In Studies on German Language-Islands. Edited by Michael T. Putnam. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 279–99. [Google Scholar]
- Padovan, Andrea, Alessandra Tomaselli, Myrthe Bergstra, Norbert Corver, Ricardo Etxepare, and Simon Dold. 2016. Minority languages in language contact situations: Three case studies on language change. Us Wurk 65: 146–74. [Google Scholar]
- Panieri, Luca, Monica Pedrazza, Adelia Nicolussi Baiz, Sabine Hipp, and Cristina Pruner, eds. 2006. Bar lirnen z’ schraiba un zo reda az be biar. Grammatica del cimbro di Luserna. Grammatik der zimbrischen Sprache von Lusérn. Trento/Luserna: Regione Autonoma Trentino-Alto Adige/Autonome Region Trentino-Südtirol & Istituto Cimbro/Kulturinstitut Lúsern. [Google Scholar]
- Polinsky, Maria, and Gregory Scontras. 2020. Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The Fine Structure of the Left Periphery. In Elements of Grammar. Handbook in Generative Syntax. Edited by Liliane Haegeman. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 281–337. [Google Scholar]
- Rohlfs, Gerhard. 1969. Grammatica Storica Della Lingua Italiana e dei suoi Dialetti. Sintassi e formazione delle Parole. Torino: Einaudi, vol. 3. [Google Scholar]
- Taeschner, Traute. 1983. The Sun is Feminine: A Study of Language Acquisition in Bilingual Children. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Trumper, John, and Luigi Rizzi. 1985. Il problema sintattico di CA/MU nei dialetti calabresi mediani. Quaderni del Dipartimento di Linguistica Università della Calabria 1: 63–76. [Google Scholar]
- Tyroller, Hans. 2003. Grammatische Beschreibung des Zimbrischen von Lusern. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe, Sam. 2019. Verb Second in Medieval Romance. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tomaselli, A.; Bidese, E.; Padovan, A. Feature Borrowing in Language Contact. Languages 2022, 7, 288. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040288
Tomaselli A, Bidese E, Padovan A. Feature Borrowing in Language Contact. Languages. 2022; 7(4):288. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040288
Chicago/Turabian StyleTomaselli, Alessandra, Ermenegildo Bidese, and Andrea Padovan. 2022. "Feature Borrowing in Language Contact" Languages 7, no. 4: 288. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040288
APA StyleTomaselli, A., Bidese, E., & Padovan, A. (2022). Feature Borrowing in Language Contact. Languages, 7(4), 288. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040288