Is the Foot a Prosodic Domain in European Portuguese?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(1) |
2. Evidence for the Foot in European Portuguese
2.1. Word-Level Stress Assignment
- (i)
- Inflected verbs instantiate the following strictly ordered morphological components: (a) a derivational root, containing a root, and possible derivational affixes (Rt)—obligatory; (b) a theme vowel or thematic constituent (TC)—obligatory; the TC -a is the most frequent and the only productive TC; -e and -i are also quite frequent TC; the TC is deleted when followed by an affix starting with a vowel, except when the TC and the following vowel are both non-open, non-back, in which case they undergo fusion (see also note 2); (c) a tense–mood–aspect morpheme (TMA)—a single morpheme instantiating the categories of Tense (past, present, and future/conditional), Aspect (perfect and imperfect), and Mood (indicative and subjunctive); in present indicative, TMA is not visible (this may be analysed as lack of a TMA morpheme or presence of a Ø morpheme); TMA is always present elsewhere; and a person–number morpheme (PN)—usually with no phonetic content in the 3Sg, and also in the 1Sg (again, this may be analysed as lack of PN or presence of a Ø morpheme), but obligatory elsewhere.2 Examples are given in (2a), below.
- (ii)
- Non-verbs may be (a) thematic, in which case they are composed of the derivational root (Rt), plus the thematic indexes (or TC) -a, -u (spelt as <o>), or -e, or (b) athematic, in which case the Rt also corresponds to the stem; both thematic and athematic non-verbs may inflect for plural (Pl), by adding a partially underspecified coronal fricative (/S/).3,4,5 Examples are given in (2b), below. Hereafter, boldface signals the syllable bearing word stress in orthographic forms.
(2) | a. | Inflected verb forms | ||
fala | falRt aTC | ‘speakPres.Ind.3Sg’ | ||
falavas | falRt aTC vaTMA sPN | ‘speakPast.Imp.Ind.2Sg’ | ||
falemos | falRt aTC eTMA mosPN | ‘speakPres. Subj.1Pl’ |
b. | Thematic non-verbs (Sg, Pl) | |||
menino/meninos | meninRt oTC sPl | ‘boy(s)’ | ||
batata/batatas | batatRt aTC sPl | ‘potato(s)’ | ||
envelope/envelopes | envelopRt eTC sPl | ‘envelop(s)’ |
Athematic non-verbs | ||||
papel/papéis | papelRt,St/papejRt,St sPl | ‘paper(s)’ | ||
andar/andares | andarRt sPl | ‘flat(s)’ | ||
rapaz/rapazes | rapazRt sPl | ‘boy(s)’ | ||
café/cafés | caféRt sPl | ‘coffee(s)’ | ||
menu/menus | menuRt sPl | ‘menu(s)’ |
- (iii)
- The root (Rt) and the thematic constituent (TC) form a morphological constituent, the stem (St), both in verbs and non-verbs (Villalva and Gonçalves 2016). In athematic non-verbs, which lack a TC, the root and the stem coincide, as in café and cafés ‘coffee(s)’. Inflectional affixes are morphosyntactic specifiers of the stem (MSS) (Villalva and Gonçalves 2016). According to Villalva (2008), inflectional morphemes form a morphological domain: in verbs the inflection domain contains TMA and PN suffixes; in non-verbs, it contains the Pl suffix. If the word lacks MSS, the St and morphological word (MW) coincide, as in café ‘coffee’ (but not in cafés ‘coffees’). Non-compound content words in EP display the structure in (3), where MSS may be null in verbs and MSS and TC may be null in non-verbs (4).
(3) | [[[X]Rt [Y]TC]St [Z]MSS ]MW (adapted from Villalva and Gonçalves 2016: 167) |
(4) | [[[fal]Rt [a]TC]St [va s]MSS ]MW | ‘speakPast.Imp.Ind.2Sg’ |
[[[fal]Rt [a]TC]St [ ]MSS ]MW | ‘speakPres.Ind.3Sg’ | |
[[[menin]Rt [o]TC]St [s]MSS ]MW | ‘boys’ | |
[[[café]Rt [ ]TC]St [ ]MSS ]MW | ‘coffee’ |
(5) | a. | Stress distribution in inflected forms (falar ‘to speak’) | |||
Present | 1Sg | falo | |||
2Sg | falas | ||||
3Pl | falam | ||||
Past | 1/3Sg | falava | |||
1Pl | falávamos | ||||
3Pl | falavam | ||||
Future/Conditional | 1/3Sg | falaria | |||
1Pl | falaríamos | ||||
3Pl | falariam | ||||
b. | Stress distribution in non-verbs | ||||
sapato | ‘shoe’ | ||||
batata | ‘potato’ | ||||
animal | ‘animal’ | ||||
chapéu | ‘hat’ |
- (i)
- The role of syllable weight—see in particular the discussion in Pereira (1999), who defends that in EP what matters is number of syllables at the right edge in present tense verbs, and the morphological structure and lexical marking of elements that cannot bear stress elsewhere, and Wetzels (2007), who extensively argues for the role of syllable quantity in (B)P non-verbs only (see also the review of Magalhães 2016).
- (ii)
- The relevance of metrical foot construction in computing stress location—e.g., for Pereira (1999) present tense forms are assigned stress via the formation of trochaic feet, whereas for Wetzels (2007), in the present tense, what accounts for stress location is morphological position—stress falls on the last vowel of the root, and 1/2Pl is subject to a specific constraint that ensures that stress falls on the theme vowel; by contrast, in non-verbs, the general rule in Pereira assigns stress to the final syllable of the (derivational) root, whereas for Wetzels, stress is assigned via the formation of quantity-sensitive trochaic feet (like Bisol 1992, and others).
- (iii)
- The extent to which there is a general pattern for stress assignment in non-verbs, with some exceptions marked in the lexicon (as assumed in most current literature on the topic), or stress is simply lexically marked for every non-verb (Garcia 2017).
- (iv)
- (v)
- The role of foot structure in stress assignment—most frequently, under the more widespread approaches, where stress is morphologically conditioned but there is also metrical stress, the foot is relevant for determining stress location in some areas of the lexicon, such as non-verbs, but not in verbs, where stress location is obtained as a function of morphological information, and foot construction takes stress location into account, via alignment constraints, as in Wetzels’ (2007) approach.
- (a)
- Phonological metrics: metrical stress springs from the formation of syllabic trochees—the same foot type for designated subcategories of verbs and non-verbs.
- (b)
- Reference to a morphological position: the location of stress is obtained with reference to a morphological position—also the same position for designated subcategories of verbs and non-verbs.
(6) | a. | (fa.la) | ‘speakPres.Ind.3Sg’ | (fa.le) | ‘speakPres.Subj.1/3Sg’ |
(fa.las) | ‘speakPres.Ind.2Sg’ | (fa.les) | ‘speakPres.Subj.2Sg’ | ||
fa(la.mos) | ‘speakPres.Ind.1Pl’ | fa(le.mos) | ‘speakPres.Subj.1Pl’ | ||
(fa.lam) | ‘speakPres.Ind.3Pl’ | (fa.lem) | ‘speakPres.Subj.3Pl’ | ||
b. | sa(pa.to) | ‘shoe’ | |||
ba(ta.ta) | ‘potato’ | ||||
car(pe.te) | ‘carpet’ |
(7) | a. Verbs—Past (falar ‘speak’) | ||||||
Indicative Imperfect | Subjunctive Imperfect | ||||||
1/3Sg | falava | fal a)St va | falasse | fal a)St sse | |||
2Sg | falavas | fal a)St va s | falasses | fal a)St sse s | |||
1Pl | falávamos | fal a)St va mos | falássemos | fal a)St sse mos | |||
3Pl | falavam | fal a)St va m | falassem | fal a)St sse m | |||
b. Non-verbs—Athematic | |||||||
papel | papel)St | ‘paper’ | |||||
andar | andar)St | ‘flat’ | |||||
Inglês | Inglês)St | ‘English’ | |||||
chapéu | chapéu)St | ‘hat’ | |||||
herói | herói)St | ‘heroe’ | |||||
bisturi | bisturi)St | ‘scalpel’ | |||||
menu | menu)St | ‘menu’ | |||||
você | você)St | ‘you’ | |||||
avô | avô)St | ‘grandpa’ | |||||
café | café)St | ‘coffee’ | |||||
avó | avó)St | ‘grandma’ | |||||
sofá | sofá)St | ‘sofa’ |
(8) | Future—Indicative | Conditional | |
1Sg | falarei | falaria | |
2Sg | falarás | falarias | |
3Sg | falará | falaria | |
1Pl | falaremos | falaríamos | |
3Pl | falarão | falariam |
(9) | Non-standard present subjunctive inflection | |
1/3Sg | suponha | |
2Sg | suponhas | |
1Pl | supônhamos (suponhamos, in Standard EP) | |
3Pl | suponham | |
1Sg | perceba | |
2Sg | percebas | |
1Pl | percêbamos (percebamos, in Standard EP) | |
3Pl | percebam |
(10) | a. Exceptional antepenult stress in thematic non-verbs | |||
genético | (instead of *genetico) | ‘genetic’ | ||
calêndula | (instead of *calendula) | ‘calendula’ | ||
ênfase | (instead of *enfase) | ‘emphasis’ | ||
b. Exceptional penult and antepenult stress in athematic non-verbs | ||||
líder | (instead of *lider) | ‘leader’ | ||
fácil | (instead of *facil) | ‘easy’ | ||
Lúcifer | (instead of *Lucifer) | ‘id.’ | ||
Júpiter | (instead of *Jupiter) | ‘id.‘ |
- (i)
- In inflected verbs, in the present tense, where stress is prosodically-based, syllables closed by nasals or consonants at the right edge of the word do not attract stress (batemPres.Ind.3Pl, batemosPres.Ind.1Pl, and batesPres.Ind.2Sg).
- (ii)
- In non-verbs, syllables closed by /S/ at the right edge of the word pattern variably with respect to stress: an inflectional /S/ in word final position does not trigger final stress (janelas ‘windows’), whereas a non-inflectional final /S/ triggers final stress (rapaz ‘boy’). Under an approach whereby stress is quantity-sensitive, the reason for this asymmetry is not clear; in our account, the difference is expected, since metrical stress is not sensitive to quantity (and hence the presence or absence of /S/ yields the same result, that is, a syllabic trochee) and stress assigned to the syllable containing the final vowel of the stem simply ignores the syllable (number and/or composition).
- (iii)
- Under our account, the tendency for final stress in words ending in a diphthong or non-inflectional consonant, which is an important argument suggesting the relevance of syllable weight (chapéu ‘hat’), springs from the fact that in non-verbs such endings appear in the stem final position of athematic classes; crucially, the fact that the same behavior is found in athematic words with stems ending in vowel (café ‘coffee’) strongly suggests that what is relevant is morphological position, instead of syllable quantity.
- (iv)
- Along the lines of Pereira (1999), we may account for the fact that in the Portuguese lexicon stress does not appear to the left of closed syllables in penult position (*σHσ) by admitting that a different phonological grammar shaped the lexicon of the language, which however is no longer active in the present day; loanwords seem to support this view, since stress is preserved in the antepenult syllable in words like Washington, and Parkinson.
(11) | a. | música (N) | ‘music’ | / | musica (V) | ‘musicPres.Ind.3Sg’ |
crítica (N) | ‘criticism’ | / | critica (V) | ‘criticizePres.Ind.3Sg’ | ||
furão (N) | ‘ferret’ | / | furam (V) | ‘drillPres.Ind.3Pl’ | ||
zangão (N) | ‘bumblebee’ | / | zangam (V) | ‘get madPres.Ind.3Pl’ | ||
lícito (A) | ‘legal’ | / | licito (V) | ‘betPres.Ind.1Sg’ | ||
vívido (A) | ‘vivid’ | / | vivido (V) | ‘livePast.Part’ | ||
b. | bambo (A) | ‘wobbly’ | / | bambu (N) | ‘baboo’ | |
têmpora (N) | ‘temple’ | / | tempura (N) | ‘tempura’ | ||
partiram (V) | ‘leavePast.Perf.3Pl’ | / | partirão (V) | ‘leaveFut.Ind.3Pl’ | ||
incidiram (V) | ‘fallPast.Perf.3Pl’ | / | incidirão (V) | ‘fallFut.Ind.3Pl’ | ||
contem (V) | ‘tellPres.Subj.3Sg’ | / | contém (V) | ‘includePres.Ind.3Sg’ | ||
c. | canto (N/V) | ‘corner; chant’, ‘singPres.Ind.1Sg’ | ||||
manifesto (N/V) | ‘manifest’, ‘manifestPres.Ind.1Sg’ | |||||
conquistas (N/V) | ‘conquests’, ‘conquer Pres.Ind.2Sg’ | |||||
verão (N/V) | ‘Summer’, ‘seeFut.Ind.3Pl’ | |||||
arranque (N/V) | ‘start’, ‘startSubj.Pres.1/3PSg’ | |||||
acórdão (N) ‘court decision’ / acordam (V) ‘wake upPres.Ind.3Pl’ |
2.2. Diminutive/Evaluative Formation with -inh-
(12) | patos - p[á]tos ‘duckMasc.Pl’ / patinhos - p[ɐ]tinhos, *p[a]tinhos | ‘duckDIM.Masc.Pl’ |
peras - p[é]ras ‘pearDIM.Fem’ / perinhas - p[ɨ]rinhas, *p[e]rinhas | ‘pearDIM.Fem.Pl’ | |
gordos - g[ó]rdos ‘fatMasc.Pl’ / gordinhos - g[u]rdinhos, *g[o]rdinhos | ‘fatDIM.Masc.Pl’ |
(13) | (i) | amável - am[á]vel ‘gentle’ / amavelzinho - am[á]velz[í]nho, *am[ɐ]velz[í]nho | |||
‘gentle(DIM)’ | |||||
avô - av[ó] / avozinho - av[ó]zinho, *av[u]z[í]nho | ‘grandpa(DIM)’ | ||||
(ii) | pêndulo - pendulozinho | ‘pendulum(DIM)’ | |||
carapaça - carapaçazinha | ‘shell(DIM)’ | ||||
(iii) | amável / amavelzinho ‘gentleSg(DIM)’ vs. amáveis / amaveizinhos ‘gentlePl(DIM)’ | ||||
leão / leãozinho ‘lionSg(DIM)’ vs. leões / leõezinhos ‘lionPl(DIM)’ |
(14) | a. | livrinho/livrozinho | ‘bookDIM’ |
almofadinha/almofadazinha | ‘pillowDIM’ | ||
b. | livrinhozinho | ‘bookDIM’ | |
almofadinhazinha | ‘pillowDIM’ | ||
c. | sofazinho/*sofainho | ‘sofaDIM’ | |
europeuzinho/*europeuinho | ‘EuropeanDIM’ | ||
seculozinho/*seculinho | ‘centuryDIM’ | ||
d. | *livrozinhinho | ‘bookDIM’ | |
*almofadazinhinha | ‘pillowDIM’ |
(15) | irmã | irmãzinha/*irmãinha | ‘sister(DIM)’ |
irmão | irmãozinho/*irmãinho, *irmãoinho | ‘brother(DIM)’ | |
jardim | jardinzinho/*jardinho, *jard[ı̃]inho | ‘garden(DIM) ’ | |
(16) | café | cafezinho/*cafeinho | ‘coffee(DIM)’ |
europeu | europeuzinho/*europeuinho | ‘european(DIM)’ | |
(17) | pomar | pomarzinho/*pomarinho | ‘orchard(DIM)’ |
tropical | tropicalzinho/*tropicalinho | ‘tropical(DIM)’ | |
(18) | século | seculozinho/*seculinho | ‘century(DIM)’ |
frívolo | frivolozinho/*frivolinho | ‘frivolous(DIM)’ | |
câmara | camarazinha/*camarinha | ‘camera(DIM)’ | |
frigorífico | frigorificozinho/*frigorifiquinho | ‘fridge(DIM)’ | |
pálida | palidazinha/*palidinha | ‘pale(DIM)’ | |
(19) | Lúcifer | Luciferzinho/*Luciferinho | ‘Lucifer(DIM)’ |
júnior | juniorzinho/*juniorinho | ‘junior(DIM)’ | |
álcool | alcoozinho/*alcolinho | ‘alcohol(DIM)’ | |
líder | liderzinho/*liderinho | ‘leader(DIM)’ | |
átlas | atlazinho/*atlasinho | ‘atlas(DIM)’ | |
abdómen | abdomenzinho/*abdomeninho | ‘abdomen(DIM)’ |
2.3. Vowel Reduction
(20) | fala [ˈfalɐ] | ‘(he) speaks’ |
falava [fɐˈlavɐ] | ‘(he) spoke’ | |
falaria [fɐlɐˈɾiɐ] | ‘(he) would speak’ |
- (1)
- Syllables with post-vocalic glides, nasals, and laterals (Mateus 1975/1982; Mateus and d’Andrade 2000), that is, syllables with a branching nucleus, assuming partial nuclearization of /l/ (Vigário 2022, and references therein)—vaidade [vajˈdad] ‘vanity’ and beldade [bɛlˈdad] ‘beauty’.
- (2)
- The ω-initial position (non-central V) (Mateus 1975/1982; Mateus and d’Andrade 2000); here, there is a partial reduction (Vigário 2003)—erguer [eɾˈɡeɾ]~[iɾˈɡeɾ] ‘rise’ and ocupar[okuˈpaɾ]~[ɔkuˈpaɾ] ‘occupy’.
- (3)
- The final position of a ω containing non-thematic material: athematic non-verbs, non-final roots within a morphological compound, clipped words resulting from morphological compounds, and stressed prefixes—líder [ˈlidɛɾ] ‘leader’ and monogâmico [ˈmɔnɔˈɡɐmiku] ‘monogamous’ (Vigário 2003, 2016, 2022).21
(21) | protocular | [pɾɔtɔkuˈlaɾ]/*[pɾutukuˈlaɾ] | ‘protocol’ |
preleção | [pɾɨlɛˈsɐ̃w̃]/*[pɾɨlɨˈsɐ̃w̃] | ‘lecture’ | |
afetividade | [ɐfɛtiviˈdad]/ *[ɐfɨtiviˈdad] | ‘affectivity’ | |
retórica | [ʀɛˈtɔɾikɐ]/*[ʀɨˈtɔɾikɐ] | ‘rhetoric’ | |
Aveiro | [aˈvɐjɾu]/*[ɐˈvɐjɾu] | ‘Id.’ |
(22) | perceberás (/peɾsebeɾaS/) [pɨɾsɨbɨˈɾaʃ] (>[pɾsbˈɾaʃ]) ‘understandFut.2sg’ |
2.4. Glide Formation—VV > GV
(23) | criatura | [i]/[j] | ‘creature’ |
canoagem | [u]/[w] | ‘canoeing’ |
(24) | mobília | mobíl[j]a / *mobíl[i]a | ‘furniture’ |
nódoa | nód[w]a / *nód[u]a | ‘stain’ |
(25) | bebe-o | beb[j]o / *beb[i]o | ‘drink-it’ | |
pede-a | ped[j]a / *ped[i]a | ‘ask-itF’ |
2.5. Three Syllables Stress Window
(26) Possible locations of primary stress | |
a. Final syllable: | amará ‘loveFut3Sg’, colosal ‘colossal’, jacaré ‘alligator’ |
b. Penultimate syllable: | comemos ‘eatPres.3Sg’, amigo ‘friend’, fácil ‘easy’ |
c. Antepenultimate syllable: | amávamos ‘lovePast.Imp.1Pl’, câmara ‘camera’, Júpiter ‘Jupiter’25 |
2.6. Poetic Rhyme
(27) | Outros haverão de ter1 |
O que houvermos de perder1. | |
Outros poderão achar2 | |
O que, no nosso encontrar2, | |
Foi achado, ou não achado3, | |
Segundo o destino dado3. | |
Mas o que a eles não toca4 | |
É a Magia que evoca4 | |
O Longe e faz dele história5. | |
E por isso a sua glória5 | |
É justa auréola dada6 | |
Por uma luz emprestada6. |
2.7. Vowel Lowering Processes
(28) | Lowering of stressed mid vowels | ||
a. So called Dactylic Lowering | |||
esquel[ɛ́]tico ‘skeletal’ | (cf. esquel[é]to ‘skeleton’) | ||
b. So called Spondaic Lowering | |||
d[ɔ́]cil ‘gentle’ | (cf. d[ó]ce ‘sweet’) |
(29) | f[ó]lego ‘breath’ | est[ó]mago ‘stomach’ | ser[ó]dio ‘old’ |
s[ó]frego ‘lickerish’ | b[é]bedo ‘drunk’ | l[é]vedo ‘fermented’ | |
pul[ó]ver ‘pullover’ | Almod[ó]var ‘Id.’ | Est[é]vão ‘Id.’ |
(30) | No lowering of mid stressed vowels in root final position (regular) | ||
Thematic | car[é]t)Rt a)St)MW | ‘grimace’ | |
r[é]d)Rt e)St)MW | ‘net’ | ||
m[ó]rr)Rt o)St)MW | ‘hill’ | ||
Athematic | voc[é])Rt)St)MW | ‘you’ | |
av[ó])Rt)St)MW | ‘grandpa’ | ||
corred[ó]r)Rt)St)MW | ‘runner’ |
(31) | Lowering of mid stressed vowels not root final | ||
Thematic | esquel[ɛ́]tic)Root o)St)MW | ‘skeletal’ (cf. esquel[é]to ‘skeleton’) | |
Ahematic | d[ɔ́]cil)Root)St)MW | ‘gentle’ (cf. d[ó]ce ‘sweet’) |
(32) Lowering of unstressed non-high vowel in closed final syllables at the edge of a stress domain not coinciding with the MW | |||||
Athematic stems | |||||
amáv[ɛ]l | ‘kind’ | líd[ɛ]r | ‘leader’ | ||
Vít[ɔ]r | ‘id.’ | híf[ɛ]n | ‘hyphen’ | ||
âmb[a]r | ‘amber’ | ímp[a]r | ‘odd’ | ||
fórc[ɛ]ps | ‘forceps | síl[ɛ]x | ‘flint’ | ||
Prefixes that form independent stress domains | |||||
sup[ɛ]r-interessante | ‘super interesting’ | ||||
híp[ɛ]r-giro | ‘mega cute’ | ||||
int[ɛ]r-geracional | ‘intergenerational’ |
(33) Lowering of unstressed mid vowel at the edge of a stress domain not coinciding with the MW | ||||
Roots that form independent ω within morphological compounds | ||||
aut[ɔ]-correção | ‘self correction’ | |||
bi[ɔ]-degradável | ‘biodegradable’ | |||
tel[ɛ]-chamada | ‘phone call’ | |||
Roots+linking vowel forming an independent w within morphological compounds | ||||
israel[ɔ]-palestiniano | ‘Israeli-Palestinian’ | |||
sóci[ɔ]-cultural | ‘sociocultural’ | |||
polític[ɔ]-económico | ‘politic-economical’ | |||
Clipped words | ||||
eur[ɔ] | ‘euro’ | |||
fot[ɔ] | ‘photo’ | |||
exp[ɔ] | ‘exhibit’ |
2.8. Other Areas in Phonology Potentially Relating to the Foot
2.8.1. Hypocoristic Formation and Word Clipping
- Thematic non-verbs (regular stress pattern): output may correspond to the input trochee, minus coda consonants, if any (ˈCVCV).
(34) | Helena > Lena | Filomena > Mena |
Palmira > Mira | Alcino > Cino | |
Albertina > Tina | (Manuela>) Manela > Nela | |
Francisco > Chico | Alexandra > Xana | |
Fernando > Nando | Andreia > Neia | |
Alberta > Berta |
- B.
- Athematic non-verbs (regular stress pattern): output may correspond to what may be analysed as the input (degenerate) foot, minus the coda consonant, if any.33
(35) | José > Zé |
Isabel > Bé | |
Joaquim > Quim | |
Conceição > São |
- C.
- Various stress patterns in the input and an output template:
- (a)
- Output consists of the first two syllables of the input noun, minus the second C of a branching onset and/or a coda consonant, if any (regular and irregular stress patterns in the input)—output template: CVˈCV (stress final).34
(36) | Gabriela > Gabi |
Patrícia > Pati | |
Fátima > Fati | |
Fernando > Fanan | |
Filomena > Filó |
- (b)
- Output consists of the input stressed syllable (minus coda consonant, if any), reduplicated—output template: a branching, rightheaded PWG with the format ((CV)ω ˈ(CV)ω).35
(37) | Pedro > ((Pê)ω (Pê)ω)PWG |
Ricardo > ((Cá)ω (Cá)ω)PWG | |
Jorge > ((Jó)ω (Jó)ω)PWG | |
Leonor > ((Nô)ω (Nô)ω)PWG | |
José > ((Zé)ω (Zé)ω)PWG |
- D.
- Proper names composed of two w in the input: output corresponds to the prominent syllable (minus coda consonant, if any), or the (prominent) foot of each ω in the input, filling in a right-headed, branching PWG template ((CV)ω ˈ(CV)ω)PWG:36
(38) | Carlos Jorge > Cá Jó |
António José > Tó Zé | |
José António > Zé Tó |
(39) | a. | supermercado (super)ω (mercado)ω > super | ‘supermarket’ | |
hipermercado (hiper)ω (mercado)ω > híper | ‘megastore’ | |||
heterossexual (hétero)ω (sexual)ω > hétero | ‘heterosexual’ | |||
telemóvel (tele)ω (móvel)ω > móvel | mobile phone’ | |||
BMW (bê)ω (eme)ω (dâblio)ω > BM (bê)ω (eme)ω | ‘id.’ | |||
JSD (jota)ω (esse)ω (dê)ω > (jota)ω ‘Social Democratic Youth’(abbreviation) | ||||
computador portátil (computador)ω (portátil)ω > (portátil)ω ‘portable computer’ | ||||
paragem cardíaca (paragem)ω (cardíaca)ω >(paragem)ω | ‘heart arrest’ | |||
pré-primária (pré)ω -(primária)ω > (pré)ω | ‘kindergarden’ | |||
b. | sindicato (sindicato)ω > *(sindi)ω, *(cato)ω | ‘union’ | ||
frigorífico (frigorífico)ω > *(frigo)ω, *(rífico)ω | ‘fridge’ | |||
bicicleta (bicicleta)ω > *(bici)ω, *(cleta)ω | ‘bicycle’ | |||
televisão (televisão)ω >*(tele)ω, *(visão)ω | ‘automobile’ | |||
polícia (polícia)ω > *(poli)ω, *(cia)ω | ‘police’ | |||
meteorologia (meteorologia)ω > *(meteo)ω (rologia)ω | ‘weather forecast’ |
2.8.2. Minimality Constraints
(40) | vi | ‘seePast.Per.1PSg’ | li ‘readPast.Per.1PSg’ | si ‘him/herdat’ | ri ‘laughPast.Per.1PSg’ |
dê | ‘givePres.Subj.1/3PSg’ | lê ‘readPres.Ind.3PSg’ | sê ‘beImper’ | vê ‘see Pres.Ind.3PSg’ | |
pé | ‘foot’ | ré ‘defendentFem’ | sé ‘cathedral’ | fé ‘faith’ | |
nu | ‘nakedMasc’ | cu ‘ass’ | tu ‘you’ | Ju ‘id.’ (hypocoristic) | |
pó | ‘dust’ | dó ‘pain’ | nó ‘knot’ | mó ‘millstone’ | |
cá | ‘here’ | dá ‘givePres.Ind.3PSg’ | já ‘already’ | má ‘meanF’ |
2.8.3. Secondary Stress
(41) | a inteligência or a inteligência vs. *a inteligência | ‘the intelligence’ |
(Frota and Vigário 2000; Castelo 2006; proclitics: adjuncts to the ω, Vigário 2003) |
2.8.4. Schwa Deletion
(42) | desprevenidos | [dʃpɾvˈnidwʃ] | ‘unawarepl’ |
restremecerás | [ʀʃtɾmsˈɾaʃ] | ‘tremble againFut.2Sg’ | |
descreverá | [dʃkɾvˈɾa] | ‘describe Fut.3Sg’ |
3. Discussion and Conclusions
- (i)
- Stress assignment—we have proposed that stress is best analysed as regularly assigned via two mechanisms, one that refers to morphological position (final in the stem in athematic non-verbs and in past tense verbs), and another that is prosodically-based, involving the construction of syllabic trochaic feet at the right edge of the word, accounting for stress placement in the most frequent subclasses of non-verbs and verbs (thematic non-verbs and present tense verbs, respectively). Independent evidence for the two types of regular stress assignment was also found, coming from -inh-/-zinh- DIM formation—while -zinh- attaches to morphological bases that bear (any kind of) stress (full words with regular stress, morphologically-based or metrically based, or words with lexically marked stress), -inh- attaches to stems not-marked for stress (with no lexical stress and no stem final stress).
- (ii)
- The domain of regular vowel reduction—non-high unstressed vowels usually undergo vowel reduction in EP, but there are unpredictable lexical exceptions; however, in a number of prosodic contexts, both VR and the absence of VR are fully predictable. We have shown that one of these contexts is the foot domain, since there are no unpredictable exceptions to VR within the prominent foot of a word (i.e., in posttonic position). When VR does not apply within the foot, that is entirely predictable, as VR systematically fails to apply to the last vowel of a stress domain containing non-thematic material. This means that, in EP, VR is sensitive to the position of the vowel within the syllable (no VR in a branching syllabic nucleus), the foot (regular VR within the prominent foot; unstressed vowels ending a stress domain (ω) coinciding with a morphological domain below the morphological word-level systematically escape VR), and the prosodic word (no complete VR of non-high, non-central vowels in ω initial position).
- (iii)
- Glide formation, creating rising diphthongs, at the right edge of ω—this pure prosodic process, which is optional elsewhere, is mandatory in posttonic position; the fact that it is obligatory only in this position finds a straightforward explanation if we admit that its motivation is to create a trochee, the most frequent foot in EP, and avoid a dactyl, which is a rare and marked ternary foot. Note that a purely non-structural stress-based account of gliding in EP would correctly spot the location of gliding (i.e., post-stressed position), but would be unable to account for its double underlying motivation (i.e., the construction of the unmarked foot in the language, the avoidance of a marked foot in the language).
- (iv)
- The maximal size of the stress window—in the language, stress always falls in one of the last three syllables; as defended by Kager (2012) and others, such a limited window for stress clearly suggests that the foot, maximally consisting of a binary foot with an adjoined syllable, plays a role in restricting the possible locations of ω stress;
- (v)
- Poetic rhyme—in EP, like in other languages, perfect poetic rhymes involve the repetition of a subdomain of the word; the definition of this domain, which is intuitive for EP speakers, is straightforward if it is considered the final foot of the word, minus the onset of its first syllable.
(43) |
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Foot heads tend to surface with stress, but there are also languages where foot heads are not overtly realized with stress. In these languages, foot heads are often strong in other respects: they can display segmental strengthening in the form, for instance, of vowel lengthening (Kashasya, Buckley 2014), they can attract a high tone (de Lacy 2002), or they can display other types of strengthening effects (the epenthesis of a coda, emergence of a fortis allophone, etc.). | ||||||||||||
2 | Irregular patterns may show fusions involving the Rt, TC, TMA, and/or PN. | ||||||||||||
3 | For lexical categories, Villalva (2008) and Rio-Torto (2013) (§1.2.3) provide sound arguments for gender not being an inflectional category in EP, despite the tendency for non-verbs ending in -o to be masculine and non-verbs ending in -a to be feminine. Thus, we assume that gender is inflectional in closed word categories only. | ||||||||||||
4 | We will not discuss here allomorphy involving plurals in non-verbs, a matter much debated in literature on Portuguese (e.g., Morales-Front and Holt 1997; Mateus and d’Andrade 2000; Vigário 2003). We may note, in any case, that some facts may require assuming allomorphy, involving for instance the presence vs. absence of a V-slot, but in general the relevant alternations involve the root rather than the plural morpheme. In addition, there is evidence that in EP ω final position licenses more consonantal material than word internal codas, which in general contain at most one consonant (Veloso 2007; Vigário 2022). This means that several cases considered in the literature to involve allomorphy, are in fact inexistent in EP, such as in forms like mar/mares, where plural is marked with a surfacing palatal fricative (cf. [ˈmaɾ]/[ˈmaɾʃ] ‘sea(s)’), despite what the spelling suggests (<es>). That plurals of words like líder ‘leader’, abdómen ‘abdomen’, and sílex ‘flint’ do not involve a new syllable with a V-slot nucleus is shown by the fact that final unstressed non-high vowels undergo lowering, like in the singular (Vigário 2003, 2022), and similarly to other word final syllables closed by consonants—cf. líderes and abómenes (see also Section 2.7); in addition, non-verbs with stress on the antepenult syllable do not trigger a change in stress location in the plural form—cf. Lúcifer/Lúciferes ‘id.’ and Júpiter/Júpiteres ‘id.’. Importantly, despite the spelling <es>, no vowel is ever realized in that position in current speech. So, these plurals do not violate the three syllables stress window. | ||||||||||||
5 | We adopt Rio-Torto’s view of thematic classes in non-verbs (Rio-Torto’s 2013, §1.2.2.2). For other scholars, thematic classes in non-verbs also include a Ø TC (e.g., Villalva and Gonçalves 2016). In this paper, we assume that speakers treat these cases, where no vowel emerges at the right edge of EP non-verbs, either as athematic, or as thematic non-verbs specified with an -e TC; note that, like other prosodic words, thematic non-verbs with a final -e TC undergo the general process of a deletion of stressless non-back, non-high vowels in ω final position, and therefore, at the surface, the vowel is absent also in thematic non-verbs ending in -e (see Vigário 2003, 2022). | ||||||||||||
6 | These values contrast sharply with those of closely related languages, such as Spanish, where penult stress is clearly dominant (64%), and antepenult stress is also more frequent (9%) than in EP (Morales-Front 2014, p. 244). | ||||||||||||
7 | In non-verbs, specific fusions involving the root final vowel and VT render the thematic subclass opaque. In these cases, non-verbs are treated as athematic for the purpose of stress assignment. | ||||||||||||
8 | In the interest of text readability, in our examples we avoid forms involving the fusion of VT with adjacent material, and portmanteau allomorphs (which appear in several forms of the past perfect). Such cases pattern as expected, in that in the past tense, the syllable that incorporates the last vowel of the stem bears stress—cf. partia (part i ia) ‘leavePast.Imp.1/3Sg’ and falou (fal a w) ‘speak Past.Perf.3Sg’. | ||||||||||||
9 | Most often in the literature on EP, it has been assumed that future and conditional inflections are still formed periphrastically, with an infinitive verb plus an auxiliary, possibly reanalysed as inflection. There are strong arguments against this view, however (Vigário 2003, chp. 4). Only in syntactic constructions with weak enclitic pronouns, the weak pronoun that is enclitic to the verbal host blocks the construction of synthetic inflected forms. This originates a peculiar positioning of the clitic with respect to TMA and PN suffixes, known as mesoclisis (as in percerber-te-íamos ‘understandCond.1Pl-2SgDat’, to be contrasted with não te perceberíamos ‘not youDat understandCond.1Pl’). We should note that mesoclisis, which is specific to EP, is currently only found in educated speakers (and not all). For a fully-fledged account of future and conditional and mesoclisis, considering syntactic, morphologic, and phonologic facts, see Vigário (2003, chp. 4). See also Duarte and Matos (2000) and Luís and Spencer (2005). | ||||||||||||
10 | |||||||||||||
11 | The same paradigm uniformity has taken place in other Romance varieties, for instance, in non-standard Uruguayan Spanish (Martínez-Paricio 2013b), and in Chicano Spanish and in Andalusian, but in the latter, only in the second and third conjugations (Wetzels 2007). | ||||||||||||
12 | Due to space limitations, the full formal analysis of EP stress will be left for future work. | ||||||||||||
13 | According to Rio-Torto (2013, p. 359), DIM suffixes in general are special in that they attach to several types of categories: nouns, adjectives, verbs, and also in some cases, adverbials and pronominals. However, this is not the case of -inh-/-zinh-, which cannot attach to verbs, at least not productively (escrever/escrevinhar ‘write/scrawl and jogar/*joguinhar ‘play’). In addition, attachment to adverbs and pronominals shows different properties, suggesting we are talking about a different process; for instance, the attachment of -inh- or -zinh- is not always possible (nunca/nunquinha ‘never (ever)’ vs. sempre/*semprinho ‘always(indeed)’), and the contribution to the meaning of the resulting word is different (e.g., intensity degree, cf. devagar ‘slow’/devagarinho, ‘very slow’, cf. Rio-Torto 2013, p. 362). In addition, whereas when -inh- may form DIM, -zinh- formation is usually also an option (e.g., envelope/envelopinho~envelopezinho); the same is not true if adverbs or pronouns are involved (e.g., agora/agorinha, *agorazinha ‘(exactly) now’ and nunca/nunquinha, *nuncazinha ‘never (indeed)). | ||||||||||||
14 | Nevertheless, it is worth observing that, for reasons that need to be better understood, unstressed mid open vowels (/ɛ/ and /ɔ/) in the last syllable of the root/stem -inh- attaches to usually do not undergo VR (cf. m[ɛ́]ta-m[ɛ]tinha ‘goal(DIM)’ and r[ɔ́]da-r[ɔ]dinha ‘wheel(DIM)’. This quasi-systematic pattern, which to our knowledge has not been noticed in the literature before, requires further investigation. | ||||||||||||
15 | In addition to this, as noticed in the literature (e.g., Menuzzi 1993; Lee 1995, 2013; Villalva 2008; Bisol 2010), when the thematic index of the root is -a- and -o- but, contrary to what most often happens, the gender is masculine and feminine, respectively, DIM words with -inh- preserve the thematic index of the base, whereas in words with -zinh- the thematic index is only found in the internal base; in the resulting word, what is found, instead, is what we believe can be analysed as gender inflection, because here -a- always marks feminine and -o- systematically signals masculine, agreeing with the gender of the base word, as illustrated below. In the examples below, illustrative DIM words are preceded by the definite article, which displays morphosyntactic gender agreement with the DIM word.
| ||||||||||||
16 | An important difference between EP and BP is that, in BP, phonological evidence indicates that the base to which both -inh- and -zinh- attach forms an independent prosodic word (Menuzzi 1993; Lee 1995, 2013; Bisol 2010; Ulrich and Schwindt 2018). | ||||||||||||
17 | -zinh- and -inh- formations are also differentiated in early child productions. For instance, the first words produced by the child analysed in Vigário and Garcia (2012) that do not coincide morphologically with the adult target, showing evidence that the child is already applying morphological processes, are inflected forms—e.g., foba (target: sobe ‘climbImper’) and diziu (target: disse ‘said PastPerf3Sg)—and -zinh- formations—e.g., sozuzinha (target: sozinha ‘alone’) and baixozinho (target: baixinho ‘low’), produced at 2:04-2;06. | ||||||||||||
18 | EP provides evidence supporting the analysis of nasality in these cases, relying on the underlying underspecification of the root-final nasal segment and the nasalization of the preceding vowel or diphthong, via an association of the nasal feature to the syllable nucleus (see Vigário 2003, pp. 74–78 and work cited there). The examples in (15) are thematic words that show fusion of root-final material and thematic index vowels: the root is irma[+nas] in both irmã and irmão; the root final vowel, the nasal feature, and the class markers a or u, respectively, merge in a single syllable; and the result is a nasal vowel or a nasal diphthong occupying the nucleus of a single syllable. An indication that thematic words with roots ending in [+nas] are still analysed as thematic comes from the form of plurals (see Vigário’s work cited above for details). Note, to conclude, the clear difference between the underspecified root-final nasals in the examples above and fully specified nasals in root-final position, as in cama ‘bed’, cana ‘cane’, and pano ‘cloth’: in the latter, the fully specified nasal surfaces as a nasal consonant, with a contrastive place of articulation, and the preceding vowel remains oral. Words like cama, cana, and pano pattern like other consonant-final thematic roots not marked for stress, regularly forming DIM with -inh- (cf. caminha, caninha, and paninho). | ||||||||||||
19 | Under this analysis, the stem final vowel in thematic non-verbs is deleted. We adopt here Bisol’s (2010) account of this type of deletion. Note that this is perfectly defensible for Portuguese, since other stem-based morphological processes also trigger vowel deletion in similar contexts: for instance, stem final vowels (or theme vowels) are also systematically deleted in verbs when preceding an inflectional affix starting in a vowel. | ||||||||||||
20 | |||||||||||||
21 | See also Section 2.6 and footnote 14. | ||||||||||||
22 | Loanwords not fully integrated in the language may also exhibit unreduced non-high vowels in an unstressed position. This feature signals the non-native status of these words (Vigário 2016). | ||||||||||||
23 | There is a single word that could cast doubt on this observation, which is inclusive, where the last vowel is low for some speakers (i.e., [ı̃kluˈzivɛ] ‘including’) (and for some speakers also exclusive [ɐjʃkluˈzivɛ] ‘excluding’, this latter form being rare and clearly more marked). However, we do not think these are counter-examples to our generalization. In our view, speakers analyse these words like clipped forms (Vigário 2003), possibly relating them with inclusivamente and exclusivamente, respectively. Note that these pairs of forms are obviously morphologically related and they can have the exact same meaning and syntactic distribution. Like other clipped words that frequently show a low stressless final vowel, such as euro ([ˈewɾɔ]) ‘euro’, there is a tendency for the complete integration of the clipped word as a thematic morphological word, in which case the final vowel undergoes regular vowel reduction and deletion. Thus, in the same way as many speakers already produce euro as [ˈewɾu], many also realize inclusive as [ı̃kluˈziv]. | ||||||||||||
24 | Only in very few cases, the glide surfacing before a vowel is not the result of a gliding process: it is part of an underlying representation and there is no option to realize it as a full vowel, as in qualidade ‘quality (e.g., Bisol 1989 and others), and racional ‘rational’ (Vigário 2016). | ||||||||||||
25 | Recall that plural suffixation does not add a new syllable in EP (see note 4). | ||||||||||||
26 | For an alternative non foot-based account of stress windows, see van der Hulst’s (2012) parametric approach to stress. Instead of feet, van der Hulst refers to a “binary (non-headed) domain” and a “satellite”, which is somewhat equivalent to an adjunct in an ILT foot. Given that there is ample phonotactic, tonotactic, and morphophonological evidence for the asymmetrical treatment of the two constituents within a binary foot and for the contrast between foot heads vs. foot dependents (see, among many others, de Lacy 2002; Bennett 2012 and references therein), the foot-based account is the approach adopted here. Likewise, the ILT foot model allows to representationally capture interesting properties.shared by the two syllables located in the weak branches of feet: the adjunct of the ILT foot (in a weak branch of a non-minimal foot) and the dependent of the binary foot (also in a weak branch of a foot) (see Martínez-Paricio 2013a for details). | ||||||||||||
27 | Alternative proposals entertained to account for similar data in several Romance languages and varieties face problems, if adopted for EP. It may be hypothesized that the domain of stress is the morphological word, in this case, the verb, therefore excluding enclitics (damoVb+vo2Pl.DAT+la3Sg.ACC), as recently proposed for Spanish (Martínez-Paricio 2021; Hualde, forthcoming). However, only prosodic words, not morphological words, exhaust the stress domains in EP, since there are morphological categories that do not correspond to a morphological word which can have independent stress and form an independent prosodic word (such as stressed prefixes, stressed suffixes, and non-final roots in morphological compounds). It is also empirically incorrect to assume that clitics attach prosodically at a level higher than the ω in EP, for example, the phonological phrase (ϕ), as in ((dávamo)ω nola)ϕ (see Peperkamp 1997; for Italian, Veloso 2012; for EP, Elordieta 2014, for Spanish), since that would fail to account for the array of post-lexical phonological processes that apply with reference to the ω, which treat enclitics as incorporated into the host ω—e.g., obligatory posttonic, non-final gliding (Section 2.4), and ω final deletion of underlying non-high non-back stressless vowels, which is blocked before a vowel bearing PWG-level prominence and mandatory elsewhere, both in ω containing no enclitic and in ω with a final enclitic (cf. Vigário 2003, chps. 5–6; 2010; see also Loporcaro 2000; Vigário 2016, for other Romance languages showing the same behavior as EP with respect to host-enclitic lexical and post-lexical phonology). | ||||||||||||
28 | An interesting piece of data that is specific to EP is the fact that this type of lowering seems to be responsible for the two unique instances of phonetic [ɔ̃] (cf. [ɔ̃́]tem ‘yesterday’ and ante[ɔ̃́]tem ‘the day before yesterday’ (Vigário 2016). Note, nevertheless, that these realizations are atypical because lowering only applies to oral vowels. In addition, for many speakers, these words show a surfacing [õ], which is the regular realization of nasal non-high round vowels (Vigário 2016, 2022). | ||||||||||||
29 | As also noted in Wetzel’s work, much of the data systematically exhibiting a low vowel consist of derived words with learned preaccenting suffixes (e.g., -ic-, cf. esquelético ‘skiny, skeletal’). | ||||||||||||
30 | Accordingly, thematic words with lexically marked stress do not display lowering, since in this case ω and MW coincide (e.g., lápid[ɨ] or lápid, but *lápid[ɛ], ‘gravestone’). For completeness’ sake, we should add that onomastics ending in /S/, like Carlos, Lopes, Rodrigues, usually pattern in this respect like regular thematic words (Andrade 2020). That these words are treated more in general as thematic non-verbs is shown by the distribution of stress, and the way they pattern with respect to evaluative suffixation: -inh- is selected instead of -zinh-, and /S/ follows -inh- and the TC (cf. Carlos, Carlinhos). | ||||||||||||
31 | In some cases, the output does not coincide precisely with the input trochee (with simplified syllables), but the relation is close, e.g., Margarida [mɐɾgɐˈɾidɐ] > [ˈgidɐ] instead of *[ɾ]ida; this can be explained by the phonotactic constraint banning ω initial tap. Margarida > Guida, as well as the forms Francisco > Chico, Alexandra > Xana, Ferando > Fanan, Andreia > Neia, Isabel > Bé, Gabriela > Gabi, Patrícia > Pati, Ricardo > Cá Cá, and Leonor > Nô Nô illustrate the strong tendency for the simplification of codas and onsets, whereas Alberta > Berta shows coda consonant exceptionally preserved. | ||||||||||||
32 | The noun António typically reduces to Tó, illustrating hypocoristic formation in thematic non-verbs with exceptional stress. Here, the output corresponds to the head of the input (prominent) foot. More data need to be collected in order to determine how general this pattern is. In non-standard varieties, António is also reduced to Toino, with metathesis of the glide (an underlying /i/), turning a spondaic foot (ˈtɔ.ni.u) into a trochee (ˈtɔj.nu). | ||||||||||||
33 | An alternative for these cases would be to consider that the input contains an iamb and the output preserves the head of the foot. | ||||||||||||
34 | Here, the first vowel is often open (i.e., does not undergo vowel reduction and may even undergo strengthening, as in the case of Fanan < Fernando). | ||||||||||||
35 | Note that there is an inviolable constraint in EP preventing close mid vowels ([e] and [o]) from appearing in an unstressed non-initial position in open syllables. Therefore, the presence of close mid syllables in Pê Pê [ˈpe ˈpe] and Nô Nô [ˈno ˈno]) indicates that both syllables of the reduplicated word form an independent ω. As usually happens with branching PWG, the hypocoristic noun is perceived as stress final, since the head of the PWG corresponds to its rightmost ω. In some case, the reduplicated syllable may also correspond to the first (simplified) syllable of the input form—e.g., Teresa > Té Té; Cristina > Qui Qui. | ||||||||||||
36 | Hypocoristics like António Man(u)el > Tó Mané are maximally binary both at the foot level, since each ω is formed of a degenerate or a disyllabic foot, and at the level of PWG, since they are formed of two ω—((Tó)ω (Mané)ω)PWG. | ||||||||||||
37 | This observation holds true for all morphosyntactic categories except for interjections. | ||||||||||||
38 | See van der Hulst’s (2012, 2014) parametric account of stress, which also distinguishes the nature and factors governing primary stress and rhythmic stress in languages. Even though this author rejects the use of feet in his model, the separation of primary and rhythmic stress is crucial in both his approach and our account of EP. | ||||||||||||
39 | In corpus frequency studies, clitics represent about 30% of token frequency (Viana et al. 1996; Vigário et al. 2006, 2010), of which about 27% are proclitic (Vigário et al. 2006). | ||||||||||||
40 | See also van der Hulst (2012, 2014) on similar ideas regarding the nature of primary and rhythmic stress. |
References
- Alber, Birgit. 2010. An Exploration of Truncation in Italian. In Rutgers Working Papers in Linguistics. Edited by Peter Staroverov, Daniel Altshuler, Aaron Braver, Carlos A. Fasola and Sarah Murray. New Brunswick: LGSA, vol. 3, pp. 1–30. Available online: https://rucore.libraries.rutgers.edu/rutgers-lib/40177/ (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Alber, Birgit, and Sabine Arndt-Lappe. 2012. Templatic and subtractive truncation. In The Phonology and Morphology of Exponence: The State of the Art. Edited by Jochen Trommer. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 289–325. [Google Scholar]
- Andrade, Amália. 2020. Vocalismo. In Gramática do Português. Edited by Eduardo Paiva Raposo, Maria Fernanda Bacelar do Nascimento, Maria Antónia Coelho da Mota, Luísa Segura, Amália Mendes and Amália Andrade. Lisboa: Fundação Calouste Gulbenkian, pp. 3241–330. [Google Scholar]
- Barros, Nádia. 2014. Fraseamento prosódico em Português: Uma análise entoacional de enunciados com parentéticas e tópicos em duas variedades do Português Europeu. Master’s dissertation, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal. [Google Scholar]
- Bat-El, Outi. 2005. The emergence of the trochaic foot in Hebrew hypocoristics. Phonology 22: 115–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, Ryan. 2012. Foot-Conditioned Phonotactis and Prosodic Constituency. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, Ryan. 2017. Output optimization in the Irish plural system. Journal of Linguistics 53: 229–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, Ryan, and Emily Elfner. 2019. The Syntax–Prosody Interface. Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 151–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, Ryan, and Robert Henderson. 2013. Accent in Uspanteko. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 31: 589–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo. 2017. Stratal Phonology. In The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory. Edited by S. J. Hannahs and Anna Bosch. Abingdon: Routledge, pp. 100–34. [Google Scholar]
- Bisol, Leda. 1989. O ditongo na perspectiva da fonologia atual. Delta 5: 185–68. [Google Scholar]
- Bisol, Leda. 1992. O acento e o pé métrico binário. Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos 22: 69–80. [Google Scholar]
- Bisol, Leda. 2000. O Clítico e o seu Status Prosódico. Revista de Estudos de Linguagem UFMG 9: 5–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisol, Leda. 2010. O diminutivo e suas demandas. DELTA: Documentação E Estudos Em Linguística Teórica E Aplicada 26: 58–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisol, Leda, and João Veloso. 2016. Phonological processes affecting vowels. Neutralization, harmony and nasalization. In The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics. Edited by Leo Wetzels, Sergio Menuzzi and João Costa. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 69–85. [Google Scholar]
- Booij, Geert. 1977. Dutch Morphology: A Study of Dutch Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- Booij, Geert. 1998. Phonological output constraints in morphology. In Phonology and Morphology of the Germanic Languages. Edited by Wolfgang Kehrein and Richard Wiese. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer, pp. 143–63. [Google Scholar]
- Breteler, Jeroen. 2017. Deriving bounded tone with layered feet in Harmonic Serialism: The case of Saghala. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2: 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckley, Eugene. 2014. Kashaya extrametricality and formal symmetry. In Proceedings of the 2013 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Edited by John Kingston, Claire Moore-Cantwell, Joe Pater and Robert Staubs. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America, vol. 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bybee, Joan, and Paul Hopper. 2001. Introduction to frequency and the emergence of linguistic Structure. In Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure. Edited by Joan Bybee and Paul Hopper. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins, pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar]
- Cabré, Teresa, and Michael Kenstowicz. 1995. Prosodic Trapping in Catalan. Linguistic Inquiry 26: 694–705. [Google Scholar]
- Cabré, Teresa, Francesc Torres-Tamarit, and Maria del Mar Vanrell. 2021. Hypocoristic truncation in Sardinian. Linguistics 59: 683–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castelo, Adelina. 2006. A proeminência secundária rítmica no português europeu. Uma proposta. In Actas do XXI Encontro nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Edited by Fátima Oliveira and Joaquim Barbosa. Lisboa: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, pp. 261–72. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Câmara, Mattoso, Jr. 1970. Estrutura da Língua Portuguesa. Rio de Janeiro: Vozes, Petrópolis. [Google Scholar]
- Chitoran, Ioana, and José Ignacio Hualde. 2007. From hiatus to diphthong: The evolution of vowel sequences in Romance. Phonology 24: 37–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colina, Sonia. 1996. Spanish truncation processes: The emergence of the unmarked. Linguistics 34: 1199–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Correia, Susana, Joseph Butler, Marina Vigário, and Sónia Frota. 2015. A stress ‘deafness’ effect in European Portuguese. Language and Speech 58: 48–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cruz, Marisa. 2013. Prosodic Variation in European Portuguese: Phrasing, Intonation and Rhythm in central-Southern Varieties. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal. [Google Scholar]
- Cutler, Anne, and Dennis Norris. 1988. The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Psychophysics 14: 113–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- d’Andrade, Ernesto, and Maria do Céu Viana. 1989. Ainda sobre o acento e o ritmo em Português. In Actas do IV Encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Lisboa: APL, pp. 3–16. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- d’Andrade, Ernesto, and Maria do Céu Viana. 1994. Sinérese, diérese e estrutura silábica. In Actas do IX encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Lisboa: APL/Colibri, pp. 31–42. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- d’Andrade, Ernesto, and Maria do Céu Viana. 1999. Constantino e os acidentes de Constantinopla: Os acentos do Português e do Castelhano. In Linguística Computacional. Investigação Fundamental e Aplicações. Edited by Palmira Marrafa and Maria Antónia Mota. Lisboa: APL/Colibri, pp. 79–95. [Google Scholar]
- de Lacy, Paul. 2002. The interaction of tone and stress in Optimality Theory. Phonology 19: 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demuth, Katherine. 2015. The acquisition ofprosodic phonology and morphology. In The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. Edited by Edith Bavin and Letitia Naigles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 230–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Domahs, Ulrike, Elise Klein, Walter Huber, and Frank Domahs. 2013. Good, bad and ugly word stress—fMRI evidence for foot structure driven processing of prosodic violations. Brain and Language 125: 272–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Downing, Laura J., and Maxwell Kadenge. 2020. Re-placing PStem in the prosodic hierarchy. The Linguistic Review 37: 433–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duarte, Inês, and Gabriela Matos. 2000. Romance Clitics and the Minimalist Program. In Portuguese Syntax. New Comparative Studies. Edited by João Costa. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 116–42. [Google Scholar]
- Dupoux, Emmanuel, Sharon Peperkamp, and Núria Sebastian-Gallés. 2001. A robust method to study stress “deafness”. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110: 1606–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elordieta, Gorka. 2014. The Word in Phonology. In To Be or Not to Be a Word: New Reflections on the Definition of Word. Edited by Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano and José-Luis Mendívil-Giró. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 6–65. [Google Scholar]
- Fikkert, Paula, Liquan Liu, and Mitsuhiko Ota. 2021. The Acquisition of Word Prosody. In The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody, Oxford Handbooks. Edited by Carlos Gussenhhoven and Aoju Chen. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fonseca, Aline. 2012. O processamento prosódico de sentenças garden-path: Um estudo comparativo entre PB e PE. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil. [Google Scholar]
- Frota, Sónia. 2000. Prosody and Focus in European Portuguese: Phonological Phrasing and Intonation. New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Frota, Sónia. 2014. The intonational phonology of European Portuguese. In Prosodic Typology II. Edited by Sun-Ah Jun. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 6–42. [Google Scholar]
- Frota, Sónia, and Marina Vigário. 2000. Aspectos de prosódia comparada. Ritmo e entoação no PE e no PB. In Actas do XV Encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Edited by Rui V. Castro and Pilar Barbosa. Coimbra: APL, vol. 1, pp. 533–55. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Frota, Sónia, and Marina Vigário. 2018. Syntax-phonology interface. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia in Linguistics. Edited by Mark Aronoff. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frota, Sónia, Cátia Severino, and Marina Vigário. 2024. Unfolding prosody guides the development of word segmentation. Languages 9: 305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frota, Sónia, Marina Vigário, Nuno Matos, and Marisa Cruz. 2016. Early Prosodic Development: Emerging intonation and phrasing in European Portuguese. In Interdisciplinary Approaches to Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance. Edited by Nicholas Henriksen, Meghan E. Armstrong and Maria del Mar Vanrell. Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 295–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frota, Sónia, Marisa Cruz, Flaviane Fernandes-Svartman, Gisela Collischonn, Alina Fonseca, Carolina Serra, Pedro Oliveira, and Marina Vigário. 2015. Intonational variation in Portuguese: European and Brazilian varieties. In Intonation in Romance. Edited by Sónia Frota and Pilar Prieto. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 235–83. ISBN 978-0-19-968533-2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia, Guillherme D. 2017. Weight gradience and stress in Portuguese. Phonology 34: 41–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia, Guilherme D., and Natália B. Guzzo. 2022. Lexical access in Portuguese stress. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 21: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gouskova, Maria. 2003. Deriving economy: Syncope in Optimality Theory. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Green, Christopher. 2015. The foot domain in Bambara. Language 91: e1–e26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, Bruce. 1980. A metrical Theory of Stress Rules. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA. (Published 1985, New York: Garland Press). [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Bruce. 1989. The prosodic hierarchy in meter. In Rhythm and Meter. Edited by Paul Kiparsky and Gilbert Youmans. Orlando: Academic Press, pp. 201–60. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Bruce. 1995. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago: Chicago University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hualde, José Igacio. Forthcoming. El acento: Descripción fonológica. In Fonética y fonología descriptivas de la lengua Española. Edited by Juana Gil and Joaquim Llisterri. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
- Hyman, Larry. 2011. Does Gokana really have no syllables? Or: What’s so great about being universal? Phonology 28: 55–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inkelas, Sharon. 1990. Prosodic Constituency in the Lexicon. New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Inkelas, Sharon. 2008. The dual theory of reduplication. Linguistics 46: 351–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ito, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2012. Recursive prosodic phrasing in Japanese. In Prosody Matters: Essays in Honor of Elisabeth Selkirk. Edited by Toni Borowsky, Sigeto Kawahara, Takahito Shinya and Mariko Sugahara. Sheffield: Equinox Publishing, pp. 280–303. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 1992/2003. Weak layering and word binarity. In A New Century of Phonology and Phonological Theory: A Festschrift for Professor Shosuke Haraguchi on the Occasion of His Sixtieth Birthday. Edited by Takeru Honma, Masao Okazaki, Toshiyuki Tabata and Shin ichi Tanaka. Tokyo: Kaitakusha, pp. 26–65. [Google Scholar]
- Kager, René. 1996. On affix allomorphy and syllable counting. In Interfaces in Phonology [Studia Grammatica 41.]. Edited by Ursula Kleinhenz. Berlin: Akademie Verlag, Den Haag: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics, pp. 155–71. [Google Scholar]
- Kager, René. 2007. Feet and metrical stress. In The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology. Edited by Paul Lacy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 195–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kager, René. 2012. Stress in windows: Language typology and factorial typology. Lingua 122: 1454–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kiparsky, Paul. 1979. Metrical structure assignment is cyclic. Linguistic Inquiry 10: 421–41. [Google Scholar]
- Krämer, Martin. 2018. The perfect word in Italian. Or fruit salad matters. In Hana-Bana (花々). A Festschrift for Junko Itô and Armin Mester. Edited by Ryan Bennett, Andrew Angeles, Adrian Brasoveanu, Dhyana Buckley, Nick Kalivoda, Shigeto Kawahara, Grant McGuire and Jaye Padgett. Santa Cruz: University of California. [Google Scholar]
- Labrune, Laurence. 2012. Questioning the universality of the syllable: Evidence from Japanese. Phonology 29: 113–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lappe, Sabine. 2007. English Prosodic Morphology. Dordrecht: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Seung Hwa. 1995. Morfologia e fonologia lexical do português do Brasil. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, Brazil. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Seung Hwa. 2007. O Acento Primário em Português: Uma análise unificada na teoria da otimidade. In O acento em português. Abordagens Fonológicas. Edited by Gabriel Araújo. São Paulo: Parábola, pp. 121–43. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Seung Hwa. 2013. Interface fonologia-morfologia: Diminutivos no PB. Diadorim Special Issue. , 113–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liberman, Mark, and Alan Prince. 1977. On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry 8: 249–336. [Google Scholar]
- Loporcaro, Michele. 2000. Stress stability under cliticization and the prosodic status of Romance clitics. In Phonological Theory and the Dialects of Italy. Edited by Lori Repetti. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 137–68. [Google Scholar]
- Lu, Shuang, Cátia Severino, Marina Vigário, and Sónia Frota. 2023. Language-specific stress discrimination by European Portuguese-learning infants: An ERP study. In Proceedings of the 20th International Congress of Speech Sciences. Edited by Radek Skarnitzl and Jan Volín. Prague: Guarant International, pp. 426–30. [Google Scholar]
- Luís, Ana, and Andrew Spencer. 2005. A paradigm function account of ‘mesoclisis’ in European Portuguese. In Yearbook of Morphology 2004. Edited by Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle. Dordrecht: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magalhães, José. 2016. Main stress and secondary stress in Brazilian and European Portuguese. In The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics. Edited by Leo Wetzels, Sérgio Menuzzi and João Costa. Malden: Willey-Blackwell, pp. 107–24. [Google Scholar]
- Marquilhas, Rita. 2003. Mudança analógica e elevação das vogais pretónicas. In Razões e emoção. Miscelânea de estudos em homenagem a Maria Helena Mira Mateus, 2nd ed. Edited by Ivo Castro and Inês Duarte. Lisboa: Imprensa Nacional-Casa da Moeda, pp. 7–18. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta. 2013a. An Exploration of Minimal and Maximal Metrical Feet. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Tromsø, Tromsø, Norway. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta. 2013b. Identity effects in Uruguayan Spanish. In Proceedings of ConSOLE XX (Leipzig 2012). Edited by Enrico Boone, Martin Kohlberger and Maartje Schulpen. Leiden: SOLE, pp. 131–52. ISSN 1574-499X. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta. 2021. Stress in morphologically simple and complex Spanish words. In The Routledge Handbook of Spanish Morphology. Edited by Antonio Fábregas, Víctor Acedo-Matellán, Grant Armstrong, María Cristina Cuervo and Isabel Pujol. New York: Routledge, pp. 375–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta, and Francesc Torres-Tamarit. 2019. Trisyllabic hypocoristics in Spanish and layered feet. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 37: 659–91. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta, and René Kager. 2015. The binary-to-ternary rhythmic continuum in stress typology: Layered feet and non-intervention constraints. Phonology 32: 459–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Massini-Cagliari, Gládis, Luiz Carlos Cagliari, and Wayne J. Redenbarger. 2016. A Comparative Study of the Sounds of European and Brazilian Portuguese: Phonemes and Allophones. In The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics. Edited by Leo Wetzels, Sérgio Menuzzi and João Costa. Malden: Willey-Blackwell, pp. 56–68. [Google Scholar]
- Mateus, Maria Helena. 1975/1982. Aspectos da Fonologia Portuguesa. Lisboa: INIC. [Google Scholar]
- Mateus, Maria Helena. 1983. O acento de palavra em português. Uma nova proposta. Boletim de Filologia 28: 211–29. [Google Scholar]
- Mateus, Maria Helena, and Ernesto d’Andrade. 2000. The Phonology of Portuguese. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mateus, Maria Helena, Isabel Faria, Inês Duarte, Ana Brito, Sónia Frota, Gabriela Matos, Fátima Oliveira, Marina Vigário, and Alina Villava. 2003. Gramática da língua portuguesa, 5th ed. revised and extended. Lisboa: Caminho. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John J. 2008. The serial interaction of stress and syncope. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 26: 499–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1986/1996. Prosodic morphology. [Annotated version 1996, issued as Technical report no. 32, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science]. Waltham: University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Brandeis University. [Google Scholar]
- Menuzzi, Sérgio. 1993. On the Prosody of the Diminutive Alternation -inho/-zinho in Brazilian Portuguese. Leiden: Ms. HIL/University of Leiden. [Google Scholar]
- Mester, Armin. 1990. Patterns of truncation. Linguistic Inquiry 21: 478–85. [Google Scholar]
- Morales-Front, Alfonso. 2014. El acento. In Fonología generativa contemporánea de la lengua Española, 2nd ed. Edited by Rafael Núñez-Cedeño, Sonia Colina and Tavis G. Bradley. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 235–65. [Google Scholar]
- Morales-Front, Alfonso, and Eric Holt. 1997. On the interplay of morphology, prosody and faithfulness in Portuguese pluralization. In Phonology and Morphology of the Major Iberian Languages. Edited by Fernando Martínez-Gil and Alfonso Morales-Front. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 393–437. [Google Scholar]
- Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 1986/2007. Prosodic Phonology: With a New Foreword. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nevins, Andrew. 2011. Phonologically conditioned allomorph selection. In The Blackwell Companion to phonology 4. Edited by Marc van Oostendorp, Colin Ewen, Elizabeth Hume and Keren Rice. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 2357–82. [Google Scholar]
- Nevins, Andrew, and Paula Pinheiro Costa. 2019. Prominence Augmentation via Nasalization in Brazilian Portuguese. Catalan Journal of Linguistics 18: 161–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Özçelik, Öner. 2017. The Foot is not an obligatory constituent of the Prosodic Hierarchy: “Stress” in Turkish, French and child English. The Linguistic Review 34: 157–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulino, Nuno, Pedro Oliveira, and Marina Vigário. 2022. Hiatus resolution across words in European Portuguese dialects. In Prosodic Variation (with)in Languages: Intonation, Phrasing and Segments. Edited by Marisa Cruz and Sónia Frota. Sheffield: Equinox Publishing, pp. 219–56. [Google Scholar]
- Pearce, Mary D. 2013. The Interaction of Tone with Voicing and Foot Structure Evidence from Kera Phonetics and Phonology. Stanford: CSLI Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Peperkamp, Sharon. 1997. Prosodic Words. HIL Dissertations 34. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics. [Google Scholar]
- Pereira, Isabel. 1999. O acento de palavra em português. Uma análise métrica. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal. [Google Scholar]
- Pereira, Isabel. 2013. Capítulo 9. Processos de construção não concatenativa. In Gramática derivacional do Português. Edited by Graça Rio-Torto, Alexandra Soares Rodrigues, Isabel Pereira, Rui Pereira and Sílvia Ribeiro. Coimbra: Imprensa da Universidade de Coimbra, pp. 521–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, Conrad, Man-Kit Kan, Stephen Matthews, and Richard Wong. 2006. Syntactic ambiguity resolution and the prosodic foot: Cross-language differences. Applied Psycholinguistics 27: 301–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierrehumbert, Janet, and Mary Beckman. 1988. Japanese Tone Structure. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Piñeros, Carlos-Eduardo. 2000. Foot-sensitive word minimization in Spanish. Probus 12: 291–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poser, William J. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66: 78–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Available as ROA-537 from the Rutgers Optimality Archive. Malden: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Rio-Torto, Graça. 1993. Formação de palavras em português. Aspectos da construção de avaliativos. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal. [Google Scholar]
- Rio-Torto, Graça. 2013. Formação de avaliativos. In Gramática derivacional do Português. Edited by Graça Rio-Torto, Alexandra Soares Rodrigues, Isabel Pereira, Rui Pereira and Sílvia Ribeiro. Coimbra: Imprensa da Universidade de Coimbra, pp. 357–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roca, Iggy. 1999. Stress in the Romance languages. In Word Prosodic Systems in the Languages of Europe. Edited by Harry van der Hulst. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 659–812. [Google Scholar]
- Santana, Messias dos Santos. 2017. O sufixo diminutivo em português: Forma, funcionamento e significação—Do século xiii ao xx. Ph.D. dissertation, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. [Google Scholar]
- Schiering, René, Balthasar Bickel, and Kristine A. Hildebrandt. 2010. The prosodic word is not universal, but emergent. Journal of Linguistics 46: 657–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 2011. The syntax-phonology interface. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed. Edited by John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle and Alan C. L. Yu. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 435–84. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1978/1981. On prosodic structure and its relation to syntactic structure. In Nordic Prosody II. Edited by T. Fretheim. Trondheim: Tapir, pp. 111–40. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1980. Prosodic Domains in Phonology. In Juncture. Edited by Mark Aronoff and Mary-Louise Kean. Saratoga: Anma Libri, pp. 107–29. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1984. Phonology and Syntax. The Relation between Sound and Structure. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1996. The prosodic structure of function words. In Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition. Edited by James L. Morgan and Katherine Demuth. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 187–213. [Google Scholar]
- Thami da Silva, Hayala. 2013. Por uma abordagem unificada da hipocorização em português: Análise otimalista. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. [Google Scholar]
- Thornton, Anna Maria. 1996. On some Phenomena of Prosodic Morphology in Italian: Accorciamenti, Hypocoristics and Prosodic Delimitation. Probus 8: 81–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tilsen, Sam. 2011. Metrical regularity facilitates speech planning and production. Laboratory Phonology 2: 185–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torres-Tamarit, Francesc, and Violeta Martínez-Paricio. 2024. The prosody of Spanish acronyms. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 42: 815–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ulrich, Camila Witt, and Luiz Carlos Schwindt. 2018. O status morfoprosódico dos sufixos -inho/-zinho, -mente e -íssimo no português brasileiro. Delta 34: 769–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Hulst, Harry. 1984. Syllable Structure and Stress in Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- van der Hulst, Harry. 2010. A note on recursion in phonology. In Recursion and Human Language. Edited by Harry van der Hulst. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 301–42. [Google Scholar]
- van der Hulst, Harry. 2012. Deconstructing stress. Lingua 122: 1494–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Hulst, Harry. 2014. Representing rhythm. In Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Edited by Harry van der Hulst. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 325–65. [Google Scholar]
- Veloso, João. 2007. Rimes/VGNS/ en position finale de mot en portugais. Une contrainte «sensible au mot». In Actes du XXVe Congrès International de Linguistique et de Philologie Romanes. Edited by Maria Iliescu, Heidi M. Siller-Runggaldier and Paul Danler. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter, pp. 231–40. [Google Scholar]
- Veloso, João. 2012. Unidades acentuais proproparoxítonas e grupos clíticos em Português. In Nada na linguagem lhe é estranho. Estudos em homenagem a Isabel Hub Faria. Edited by Armanda Costa and Inês Duarte. Porto: Edições Afrontamento, pp. 471–83. [Google Scholar]
- Veloso, João. 2017. The minimal word in European Portuguese. The oralization of abbreviated forms. Alfa 61: 131–63. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina. 1999. Pronominal cliticization in European Portuguese: A postlexical operation. Catalan Working Papers in Linguistics 7: 219–37. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina. 2003. The Prosodic Word in European Portuguese, 2nd ed. 2011. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina. 2010. Prosodic structure between the prosodic word and the phonological phrase: Recursive nodes or an independent domain? The Linguistic Review 27: 485–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vigário, Marina. 2016. Segmental phenomena and their interactions: Evidence for prosodic organization and the architecture of grammar. In Manual of Grammatical Interfaces in Romance. Edited by Susann Fischer and Christoph Gabriel. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter, pp. 41–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vigário, Marina. 2022. 26 Portuguese. In Manual of Romance Phonetics and Phonology. Part II—Phonetics and Phonology of Romance Languages. Edited by Christoph Gabriel, Randall Gess and Trudel Meisenburg. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter, pp. 83–881. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina, and Paula Garcia. 2012. Palavras complexas na aquisição da morfologia do Português: Estudo de caso. In Textos Seleccionados do XXVII Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística 2011. Edited by Armanda Costa, Cristina Flores and Nélia Alexandre. Lisboa: APL, pp. 604–24. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Viana, Maria do Céu, Isabel Trancoso, Fernando Silva, Gonçalo Marques, Ernesto d’Andrade, and Luís Caldas de Oliveira. 1996. Sobre a pronúncia de nomes próprios, siglas e acrónimos em Português Europeu. In Actas do Congresso Internacional sobre o Português. Edited by Inês Duarte and Isabel Leiria. Lisboa: Colibri/APL, vol. III, pp. 481–517. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina, Fernando Martins, and Sónia Frota. 2006. A ferramenta FreP e a frequência de tipos silábicos e classes de segmentos no Português. In XXI Encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Textos Seleccionados. Edited by Fátima Oliveira and Joaquim Barbosa. Lisboa: APL, pp. 675–87. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Vigário, Marina, Marina Cruz, and Sónia Frota. 2019. Why tune or text? The role of language phonological profile in the choice of strategies for tune-text adjustment. In Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019. Edited by Sasha Calhoun, Paola Escudero, Marija Tabain and Paul Warren. Canberra: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association Inc., pp. 567–71. Available online: https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2019/papers/ICPhS_616.pdf (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Vigário, Marina, Sónia Frota, and Fernando Martins. 2010. A frequência que conta na aquisição da fonologia: Types ou tokens. In XXV Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Textos seleccionados. Edited by Ana Maria Brito, Fátima Silva, João Veloso and Alexandra Fiéis. Porto: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, pp. 749–67. Available online: https://apl.pt/atas-2 (accessed on 25 September 2024).
- Vilela, Ana Carolina, Luisa Godoy, and Thaïs Cristófaro Silva. 2006. Truncamento no português brasileiro: Para uma melhor compreensão do fenômeno. Revista de Estudos da Linguagem 14: 149–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villalva, Alina, and Carlos Alexandre Gonçalves. 2016. The phonology and morphology of word formation. In The Handbook of Portuguese Linguistics. Edited by Leo Wetzels, Sérgio Menuzzi and João Costa. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 167–87. [Google Scholar]
- Villalva, Alina. 2008. Morfologia do Português. Lisboa: Universidade Aberta. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel, Irene. 2019. Life after the Strict Layer Hypothesis. Prosodic Stucture Geometry. In Prosodic Studies: Challenges and Prospects. Edited by Hongming Zhang and Youyong Qian. New York: Routledge, pp. 9–60. [Google Scholar]
- Wetzels, Leo. 1992. Mid vowel neutralization in Brazilian Portuguese. Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos 23: 19–55. [Google Scholar]
- Wetzels, Leo. 2007. Primary stress in Brazilian Portuguese and the quantity parameter. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics 5–6: 9–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Vigário, M.; Martínez-Paricio, V. Is the Foot a Prosodic Domain in European Portuguese? Languages 2024, 9, 332. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9110332
Vigário M, Martínez-Paricio V. Is the Foot a Prosodic Domain in European Portuguese? Languages. 2024; 9(11):332. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9110332
Chicago/Turabian StyleVigário, Marina, and Violeta Martínez-Paricio. 2024. "Is the Foot a Prosodic Domain in European Portuguese?" Languages 9, no. 11: 332. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9110332
APA StyleVigário, M., & Martínez-Paricio, V. (2024). Is the Foot a Prosodic Domain in European Portuguese? Languages, 9(11), 332. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9110332