Language Processing Units Are Not Equivalent to Sentences: Evidence from Writing Tasks in Typical and Dyslexic Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. In Search for the Unit of Language Processing
2.1. The Role of the Sentence in Language Processing
2.2. Defining the Sentence and Its Scope
2.3. Information Processing and Processing in Writing
3. Writing and Sentence Processing in Children
3.1. Sentence Conceptualization and Processing in Children
3.2. Writing in Children with Dyslexia
4. Materials and Methods
4.1. Data
4.2. Product Analysis: Sentence Description in Finished Texts
4.3. Process Analysis: Bursts of Writing and Revision Processes
- -
- Bursts of writing (P-burst), aiming at producing text: [pause] but now she [pause];
- -
- Revision bursts (R-burst), aiming at modifying previously produced text: [pause] and the last one [pause]—followed by more than one burst—[pause] then [pause], where ‘then’ modifies the first burst as ‘and then the last one’;
- -
- Immediate revision bursts (RB-burst), which modify the last word(s) of the immediately produced burst: [pause] the [pause] the other [pause] (‘the other’ replaces ‘the’).
4.4. Statistics for Pause Analysis
5. Results
5.1. Product Analysis: (Un)marked Sentences in the Original and Corrected Corpora
5.1.1. Typical Children
(1) | il c’etait fait punir, | il disait que c’etait de la faute de trois fille, | une des trois fille s’appeler léa mais maintenant elle n’ais plus dans cette école | la deuxième s’appeler enola| elle aussi elle n’ai plus dans l’ecole et puis la dernière s’appeler amendine [P4CM2N2, 10 yo, M] |
he got punished, | he said it was the fault of the three girls, | one of the girls [’ name] was léa but now she isn’t at this school any more | the second was enola | she is not at this school anymore either and then the last was amendine |
(2) | Mais j’ai insisté il mon frapé et j’ai eu mal j’ai appelé la police quand elle est arrivé ils ont voulu s’échapé il n’on pas pu il m’on dit qu’il allait s’occupé d’eux et que je devait rentrait chez moi [P14CM2N2, 9 yo, M] |
But I insisted, and I was hurt I called the police when the police arrived they tried to escape, but they couldn’t they told me they would take care of them and that I should go home |
5.1.2. Dyslexic Children
(3) | Il y a un garçon qui joue pour tirer sur la d[b]alle. Il tire en l’air et ça casse la vitre. Il cecivon se fait gronder. Le père les gronde et le père ramène une cèsse aouti [caisse à outils]. [10 yo; reading: 5th percentile] |
There is a boy who plays at shooting the d[b]all. He shoots in the air and it breaks the window. He cecivon gets scolded. The father scolds them and the father brings back a cèsse aouti [toolbox]. |
(4) | Tom joue a la bale. Emlie prene le balon et il case la viter. Emile et Tom ont prer. Le papa de Tom et Emile les gonde et il le répare. [10 yo; reading: 5th percentile] |
Tom plays ball. Emlie takes the ball and he breaks the window. Emile and Tom are afraid. Tom and Emile’s dad scolds them and he fixes it. |
(5) | harli jou o balon. il tir | il mark | il cas la.vitre jen di a harli | tu a fai une gros betis. son papa a di | vou save fe une betis. ge ve voir oooo vousave fe coi. la se grav les sanfen | coman on vaver pour acheter une nouvelle vitro [9,7 yo; reading: 40th percentile] |
harly plays ball. he shoots he scores he breaks the .window jean said to harly you’ve done something very stupid. his dad said you’ve done something stupid. i’m going to see what you’ve done. this is serious, kids, how are we going to buy a new window |
5.2. Process Analysis
5.2.1. Process Analysis
- Overview
- Grammatical boundaries and factors impacting pausal segmentation
5.2.2. Burst Analysis
- Typical children
(6) | *il c’*etai*t fait punir*, | il* disait que c’etait de la faute de trois fille* ,* | une des trois fille s’appeler* léa* mais maintenant elle* n’ais plus da*ns *[dans] cette* école* | la deuxième s’appeler* eno*la*| elle aussi elle n’[‘’a]*ai plus dans l’ecole* et [puis] la derni*ère s’appeler* [puis*] amendine [P4CM2N2, 10 yo, M] |
he got punished, | he said it was the fault of the three girls, | one of the girls[’ name] was léa but now she isn’t at this school any more | the second was enola | she is not at this school anymore either and then the last was amendine |
- Dyslexic children
(7) | *Tom* joue* a la* b*ale*.*[eilme*Eilme*] [E]mli*e *p*r*ene* le* balon* et* il* ca*se* la* viter*. *Em*ile* et* [t]om*[T*] ont* [p*r*e*?*?*] p*rer* .* Le papa de* Tom* et Emile* les* gon*de* et il l*e* r*é*pare*. [10 yo; reading: 5th percentile] |
Tom plays ball. Emlie takes the ball and he breaks the window. Emile and Tom are afraid. Tom and Emile’s dad scolds them and he fixes it. |
5.2.3. Revision Analysis
- Typical children
(8) | 0:<P |
1:<ierre et Paul étaient deux a>:30 | |
29:<mis>:33 | |
32:<. Ces deux amis | |
48:<connaiss>:57 | |
56:<aient un garçon qui >:77 | |
76:< | |
76:< s’appellait Louis >:96 [P28CM2N1, 10 yo, F] |
(9) | 95:<, |
97:< | |
97:<ce dernier était méchant. I | |
124:<l taper les autres et n’avait pas d’amis. [P28CM2N1, 10 yo, F] |
- Dyslexic children
6. Discussion
6.1. Sentences in the Finished Product
6.2. Language Processing When Writing: Infra- and Supra-Sentential Units vs. Sentences
6.3. The Case of the Dyslexic Children: When Spelling Interferes with Sentence Processing
6.4. Challenges and Limitations
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Material for Writing (Children with Dyslexia)
Appendix B. Examples of Texts Produced by Typical Children
(1) | c’est etait une bagarre avec moi Baséhou contre Noah et Thibaut ca commencer par Noah il ma taper j’ai rendu aprés Baséhou à dit je vien t’aider Thibaut à dit moi aussi Baséhou à mit un coup Thibaut à dit moi aussi j’ai donné une claque Noah a conier Baséhou au mur j’ai poussé Noah vers l’herbe il est tomber sur l’herbe Noah c’est lever je les repouser il c’est relever il ma mit un coup de pied Thibaut a taper Baséhou la pousser contre le mur lui a taper il sont partie [P16CM2N1, 10 yo, M] |
(2) | Il etais une fois un garcon qui se battais toot le temps il cherchais toujours a se batre il frappais il insulter les mere et il ettais toujour punis il avais un bon fon mais il ne pouvez pas en nanpecher sa mamam le punisais de tout mais sa ne changer rien unjour il rencontra son pere et le pere lui dit si tu fait encore ca je vais t envoyer au maroc dans un ecole ou il auron le droit de te fraper ou sinon tu finis ton anne trankil et tu auras tous ce que tu voudras le petit a dis ca ne ce fais pas d acheter le gent le petit dit je te jure papa c eu qui me cherche apres il vont dire au prof que sait moi uje prefere aller au maroc et etre avec ma famille et me fer fraper au lieu de rester seul ma famille me manque papa tu peut m emener au maroc papa sil te plait si tu finis l anne bien oui et ensuite le garcon ne se battais plus il avais des bonne note et quand il termina l anne il alla au maroc et il devient ingenieur chez apple et il devin richce [P03CM2N1, 10 yo, M] |
Appendix C. Examples of Corrected Versions of the Texts—Corrected Versions of Examples (1) and (2)
(1b) | C’était une bagarre avec moi, Baséhou contre Noah et Thibaut. Ça commençait par Noah. Il m’a tapé, j’ai rendu. Après Baséhou a dit: « je viens t’aider », Thibaut a dit: « moi aussi ». Baséhou a mis un coup. Thibaut a dit: « moi aussi ». J’ai donné une claque. Noah a cogné Baséhou au mur. J’ai poussé Noah vers l’herbe, il est tombé sur l’herbe. Noah s’est levé, je l’ai repoussé. Il s’est relevé, il m’a mis un coup de pied. Thibault a tapé Baséhou, l’a poussé contre le mur, lui, a tapé. Ils sont partis. [P16CM2N1] |
(2b) | Il était une fois un garçon qui se battait tout le temps. Il cherchait toujours à se battre. Il frappait, il insultait les mères, et il était toujours puni. Il avait un bon fond, mais il ne pouvait pas en n’empêcher. Sa maman le punissait de tout, mais ça ne changeait rien. Un jour, il rencontra son père, et le père lui dit: “si tu fais encore ça, je vais t’envoyer au Maroc dans une école où ils auront le droit de te frapper”. “Ou sinon tu finis ton année tranquille, et tu auras tout ce que tu voudras”. Le petit a dit: “ça ne se fait pas d’acheter les gens”. “Le petit dit: “je te jure papa, c’est eux qui me cherchent, après ils vont dire au prof que c’est moi. Je préfère aller au Maroc et être avec ma famille, et me faire frapper au lieu de rester seul. Ma famille me manque. Papa, tu peux m’emmener au Maroc, Papa s’il te plaît”. “Si tu finis l’année bien, oui”. Et ensuite, le garçon ne se battait plus. Il avait de bonnes notes. Et quand il termina l’année, il alla au Maroc et il devint ingénieur chez Apple et il devint riche. [P03CM2N1] |
References
- Almond, Russell, Paul Deane, Thomas Quinlan, Michael Wagner, and Tetyana Sydorenko. 2012. A preliminary analysis of keystroke log data from a timed writing task. ETS Research Report Series 2: i-61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arfé, Barbara, and Eleonora Pizzocaro. 2016. Sentence Generation in Children with and Without Problems of Written Expression. In Written and Spoken Language Development across the Lifespan. Literacy Studies. Edited by Joan Perera, Melina Aparici, Elisa Rosado and Naymé Salas. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, vol. 11, pp. 327–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baaijen, Veerle M., David Galbraith, and Kees de Glopper. 2012. Keystroke analysis: Reflections on procedures and measures. Written Communication 29: 246–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Béguelin, Marie-José, Mathieu Avanzi, and Gilles Corminboeuf. 2011. La Parataxe. Lausanne: Peter Lang Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Benzitoun, Christophe, Anne Dister, Kim Gerdes, Sylvain Kahane, Paola Pietrandrea, Frédéric Sabio, and Jeanne-Marie Debaisieux. 2010. Tu veux couper là faut dire pourquoi—Propositions pour une segmentation syntaxique du français parlé. Paper presented at the Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française, New Orleans, LA, USA, July 12–15; pp. 2075–90. [Google Scholar]
- Berninger, Virginia W., Kathleen H. Nielsen, Robeftr D. Abbott, Ellen Wijsman, and Wendy Raskind. 2008. Writing problems in developmental dyslexia: Under-recognized and under-treated. Journal of School Psychology 46: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Berninger, Virginia W., William Nagy, and Scott Beers. 2011. Child writers’ construction and reconstruction of single sentences and construction of multi-sentence texts: Contributions of syntax and transcription to translation. Reading & Writing 24: 151–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berrendonner, Alain. 2004. Grammaire de l’écrit vs grammaire de l’oral: Le jeu des composantes micro- et macro-syntaxiques. In Interactions Orales en Contexte Didactique. Edited by Alain Rabatel. Lyon: PUL, pp. 249–62. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, Dorothy V. M., and Margaret J. Snowling. 2004. Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychol Bull. 130: 858–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blanche-Benveniste, Claire, Mireille Bilger, Christine Rouget, and Karel van den Eynde. 1990. (with the participation of Piet Mertens). Le français parlé. Etudes Grammaticales. Paris: CNRS Éditions. [Google Scholar]
- Bock, Kathryn, and Willem Levelt. 1994. Language production. Grammatical encoding. In Handbook of Psychology. Edited by Morton Ann Gernsbacher. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 945–84. [Google Scholar]
- Bowey, Judith A. 1986. Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing reading comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41: 282–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brazil, David. 1995. A Grammar of Speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bressoux, Pascal, Bernard Slusarczyk, Ludovic Ferrand, and Michel Fayol. 2023. Is spelling related to written composition? A longitudinal study in French. Reading & Writing 37: 615–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruck, Maggie. 1992. Persistence of dyslexics phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology 28: 874–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cain, Kate. 2007. Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 679–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campione, Eselle, and Jean Véronis. 2002. A large-scale multilingual study of silent pause duration. Paper presented at the Actes de Speech Prosody 2002, Aix-en-Provence, France, April 11–13; pp. 199–202. [Google Scholar]
- Chafe, Wallace. 1992. Information flow in speaking and writing. In The Linguistics of Literacy. Edited by Pamela Downing, Susan D. Lima and Michael Noonan. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 17–29. [Google Scholar]
- Chanquoy, Lucile, Jean-Noël Foulin, and Michel Fayol. 1990. Temporal management of short text writing by children and adults. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 10: 513–40. [Google Scholar]
- Chenoweth, Norton Ann, and John R. Hayes. 2001. Fluency in Writing: Generating text in L1 and L2. Written Communication 18: 80–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chenu, Florence, Francois Pellegrino, Harriet Jisa, and Michel Fayol. 2014. Interword and intraword pause threshold in writing. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cislaru, Georgeta, ed. 2015. Writing(s) at the Crossroads: The Process/Product Interface. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Cislaru, Georgeta, and Thierry Olive. 2017. Segments répétés, jets textuels et autres routines. Quel niveau de pré-construction? Corpus 17: 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Cislaru, Georgeta, and Thierry Olive. 2018. Le Processus de Textualisation. Bruxelles: De Boeck. [Google Scholar]
- Cislaru, Georgeta, and Thierry Olive. 2021. Que peut nous apprendre l’écriture enregistrée en temps réel au sujet des figures de construction? L’Information Grammaticale 169: 21–29. [Google Scholar]
- Cislaru, Georgeta, Iris Eshkol-Taravella, and Sarah Almeida Barreto. 2023. Automatic processing of real-time recorded writing: Pausal segmentation versus chunking. In RADH 2023—Recent Advances in Digital Humanities. Timisoara: Universitatea de Vest din Timișoara. [Google Scholar]
- Combettes, Bernard. 2011. Phrase et proposition: Histoire et évolution de deux notions grammaticales. Le français aujourd’hui 173: 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connelly, Vince, Julie E. Dockrell, Kirsty Walter, and Sarah Critten. 2012. Predicting the Quality of Composition and Written Language Bursts From Oral Language, Spelling, and Handwriting Skills in Children With and Without Specific Language Impairment. Written Communication 29: 278–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dockrell, Julie E., and Vince Connelly. 2016. The Relationships Between Oral and Written Sentence Generation in English Speaking Children: The Role of Language and Literacy Skills. In Written and Spoken Language Development across the Lifespan. Literacy Studies. Edited by Joan Perera, Melina Aparici, Elisa Rosado and Naymé Salas. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, vol. 11, pp. 161–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faigley, Lester, and Stephen Witte. 1981. Analyzing revision. College Composition and Communication 32: 400–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fayol, Michel. 1991. From Sentence Production to Text Production: Investigating Fundamental Processes. European Journal of Psychology of Education 6: 101–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fayol, Michel. 2013. L’enfant confronté à l’écrit. In L’acquisition de l’écrit. Edited by Michel Fayol. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 33–51. [Google Scholar]
- Feltgen, Quentin, Florence Lefeuvre, and Dominique Legallois. 2023. Sujet clitique et dynamique de l’écrit: Un éclairage par les jets textuels. Discours, 32. Available online: http://journals.openedition.org/discours/12509 (accessed on 10 December 2023).
- Ferreiro, Emilia. 1978. What is Written in a Written Sentence? A Developmental Answer. The Journal of Education 160: 25–39. Available online: http://www.jstor.org/stable/42750832 (accessed on 31 October 2023).
- Foulin, Jean-Noël. 1998. To what extent does pause location predict pause duration in adults’ and children’s writing? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 17: 601–20. [Google Scholar]
- Frank, Stefan L., Rens Bod, and Morten H. Christiansen. 2012. How hierarchical is language use? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279: 4522–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardiner, Alan H. 1922. The Definition of the Word and the Sentence. British Journal of Psychology 12: 352–61. [Google Scholar]
- Garrett, Merrill F. 1975. The analysis of sentence production. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Gordon H. Bower. New York: Academic Press, vol. 9, pp. 133–77. [Google Scholar]
- Garrett, Merrill F. 1980. Levels of processing in sentence production. In Language Production, Volume I: Speech and Talk. Edited by Brian L. Butterworth. London: Academic Press, pp. 177–220. [Google Scholar]
- Gerlaud, Béatrice. 2016. La phrase: Entité insaisissable au lycée? Lidil 54: 151–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilquin, Gaëtanelle. 2024. The Processing of Multiword Units by Learners of English: Evidence from Pause Placement in Writing Process Data. Languages 9: 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Givón, Talmy. 2002. Biolinguistics: The Santa Barbara Lectures. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Goody, Jack. 1987. The Interface between the Written and the Oral. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, Sophie, Veerle M. Baaijen, and David Galbraith. 2022. Constructing theoretically informed measures of pause duration in experimentally manipulated writing. Reading and Writing 37: 329–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, Michèle. 2000. Perspectives sur l’acquisition du langage. In L’acquisition du langage. Edited by Michèle Kail and Michel Fayol. Paris: PUF, pp. 13–30. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufer, David S., John R. Hayes, and Linda Flower. 1986. Composing Written Sentences. Research in the Teaching of English 20: 121–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krasowicz-Kupis, Grażyna, Aneta R. Borkowska, and Pietras Izabela. 2009. Rapid automatized naming, phonology and dyslexia in Polish children. Medical Science Monitor: International Medical Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research 15: CR460-9. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Lalain, Muriel, Luciana Mendoça-Alves, Robert Espesser, Alain Ghi, Céline De Looze, and César Reis. 2012. Lecture et prosodie chez l’enfant dyslexique, le cas des pauses. Paper presented at the TALN-RECITAL, Journées d’Études sur la Parole, Grenoble, France, June 4–8; pp. 41–48. [Google Scholar]
- Le Goffic, Pierre. 2011. Phrase et intégration textuelle. Langue Française 170: 11–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leijten, Marielle, and Luuk Van Waes. 2013. Keystroke Logging in Writing Research: Using Inputlog to Analyze and Visualize Writing Processes. Written Communication 30: 358–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Masseron, Caroline. 2019. De la textualité narrative aux faits syntaxiques dans un écrit scolaire. Peut-on articuler micro- et macro-syntaxe dans une perspective didactique? In Types d’unités et procédures de segmentation. Edited by Marie-José Béguelin, Gilles Corminboef and Florence Lefeuvre. Limoges: Lambert Lucas, pp. 45–63. [Google Scholar]
- Matsuhashi, Ann. 1981. Pausing and planning: The tempo of written discourse production. Research in the Teaching of English 15: 113–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McConnell, Kyla. 2023. Individual Differences in Holistic and Compositional Language Processing. Journal of Cognition 6: 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Medimorec, Srdan, and Evan F. Evan Risko. 2017. Pauses in written composition: On the importance of where writers pause. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 30: 1267–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell, Don C. 1994. Sentence parsing. In Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Edited by Morton Ann Gernsvacher. New York: Elsevier, pp. 375–409. [Google Scholar]
- Mokhtari, Kouider, and Brian H. Thompson. 2006. How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Reading Research and Instruction 46: 73–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morken, Frøydis, and Turid Helland. 2013. Writing in dyslexia: Product and process. Dyslexia 19: 131–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nicolson, Roderick I., and Angela J. Fawcett. 1990. Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition 35: 159–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Grady, Gerard. 2010. A Grammar of Spoken English Discourse: The Intonation of Increments. London: Bloomsbury Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Olive, Thierry. 2014. Toward an Incremental and Cascading Model of Writing: A review of research on writing processes coordination. Journal of Writing Research 6: 173–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, Robin L., and Bruce F. Pennington. 2012. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 379: 1997–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Plisson, Anne, Daniel Daigle, and Isabelle Montesinos-Gelet. 2013. The spelling skills of French-speaking dyslexic children. Dyslexia 19: 76–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rasteiro, Isabel, and Teresa Limpo. 2023. Examining Longitudinal and Concurrent Links Between Writing Motivation and Writing Quality in Middle School. Written Communication 40: 30–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roeser, Jens, Sven De Maeyer, Mariëlle Leijten, and Luuk Van Waes. 2021. Modelling typing disfluencies as finite mixture process. Reading and Writing 37: 359–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rondelli, Fabienne. 2013. La phrase, segment textuel «de base»: Choix d’écriture d’élèves de cycle 3 et jugements des enseignants. Le Français Aujourd’hui 181: 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rønneberg, Vibeke, Mark Torrance, Per H. Uppstad, and Christer Johansson. 2022. The process-disruption hypothesis: How spelling and typing skill affects written composition process and product. Psychological Research 86: 2239–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rutter, Michael, Avshalom Caspi, David Fergusson, John L. Horwood, Robert Goodman, Barbara Maughan, Terrie E. Moffitt, Howard Meltzer, and Julia Carroll. 2004. Sex differences in developmental reading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. JAMA 291: 2007–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruwet, Nicolas. 1967. Introduction à la grammaire générative. Paris: Plon. [Google Scholar]
- Schilperoord, Joost. 2002. On the Cognitive Status of Pauses in Discourse Production. In Contemporary Tools and Techniques for Studying Writing. Edited by Thierry Olive and C. Michael Levy. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Press, pp. 61–87. [Google Scholar]
- Schneuwly, Bernard. 1988. Le langage écrit chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel and Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé. [Google Scholar]
- Seguin, Jean-Pierre. 1993. L’Invention de la phrase au XVIIIe siècle, contribution à l’histoire du sentiment linguistique français. Leuven: Peeters. [Google Scholar]
- Sinclair, John, and Anna Mauranen. 2006. Linear Unit Grammar: Integrating Speech and Writing. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Sinclair, John M. 1991. Corpus, Concordance, Collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Snell, Joshua, and Jonathan Grainger. 2017. The sentence superiority effect revisited. Cognition 168: 217–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Snowling, Margaret J. 1995. Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading 18: 132–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snowling, Margaret J. 2001. From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia 7: 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Snowling, Margaret J., Charles Hulme, and Kate Nation. 2020. Defining and understanding dyslexia: Past, present and future. Oxford Review of Education 46: 501–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spelman Miller, Krystian. 2006. The pausological study of written language production. In Computer Keystroke Logging: Methods and Applications. Edited by Kirk P. H. Sullivan and Eva Lindgren. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 11–31. [Google Scholar]
- Sprenger-Charolles, Liliane, and Willy Serniclaes. 2003. Acquisition de la lecture et de l’écriture et dyslexie: Revue de la littérature. Revue française de linguistique appliquée VIII: 63–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, John F., Patricia M. Riddell, and Sue Fowler. 1988. Disordered vergence control in dyslexic children. British Journal of Ophthalmology 72: 162–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sumner, Emma, Vincent Connely, and Anna L. Barnett. 2013. Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution. Reading & Writing 26: 991–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sumner, Emma, Vincent Connelly, and Anna L. Barnett. 2016. The influence of spelling ability on vocabular choices when writing for children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities 49: 293–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tallal, Paula. 1980. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain Lang 9: 182–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tolchinsky, Liliana, and Anna Teberosky. 1998. The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development 13: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Hell, Janet, Ludo Verhoeven, and Liesbeth van Beijsterveldt. 2008. Pause time patterns in writing narrative and expository texts by children and adults. Discourse Processes 45: 406–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Raemdonck, Dan. 2013. Aussitôt la détermination effectuée, et toute prédication dehors, on dit le syntagme clôturé. Etude de structures entre syntagme et sous-phrase. In Les Fonctions Grammaticales. Histoire, Théorie, Pratiques. Edited by Aboubakar Ouattara. Bruxelles: P.I.E. Peter Lang, pp. 209–21. [Google Scholar]
- Van Raemdonck, Dan, Marie Détaille, and Lionel Meinertzhagen. 2011. Le sens grammatical. Référentiel à destination des enseignants. Bruxelles: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- Van Reybroeck, Marie. 2020. Grammatical spelling and written syntactic awareness in children with and without dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Waes, Luuk, Marielle Leijten, Jens Roeser, Thierry Olive, and Joachim Grabowski. 2021. Measuring and assessing typing skills in writing research. Journal of Writing Research 13: 107–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wen, Yun, Joshua Snell, and Jonathan Jonathan Grainger. 2019. Parallel, cascaded, interactive processing of words during sentence reading. Cognition 189: 221–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zawadowski, Leon. 1971. Sentence, its grammatical definition. Linguistics 9: 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Original Corpus | Corrected Corpus | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Statistics | Sentences | Words | Paragraphs | Sentences | Words | Paragraphs |
Total | 177 | 4277 | 70 | 336 | 4357 | 57 |
Minimum | 0 | 39 | 1 | 2 | 41 | 1 |
1st Quartile | 4 | 104 | 1 | 8 | 107 | 1 |
Median | 6 | 136 | 2 | 12 | 139 | 1 |
3rd Quartile | 8 | 182 | 3 | 15 | 185 | 2 |
Maximum | 23 | 321 | 12 | 21 | 314 | 10 |
Average | 6.1 | 147.5 | 2.4 | 11.6 | 150.2 | 2 |
Variance (n − 1) | 24.5 | 4008.1 | 5.3 | 24.6 | 3916.8 | 3.3 |
Standard deviation (n − 1) | 5 | 63.3 | 2 | 5 | 62.6 | 1.8 |
Original Corpus | Corrected Corpus | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Statistics | Words | Sentences | Words Corrected | Sentences Corrected |
Total | 393 | 23 | 430 | 44 |
Minimum | 18 | 0 | 19 | 3 |
1st Quartile | 30 | 0 | 32 | 3.5 |
Median | 34 | 1 | 35 | 4 |
3rd Quartile | 38 | 4 | 44 | 4 |
Maximum | 59 | 5 | 67 | 6 |
Average | 36 | 2.1 | 39 | 4 |
Variance (n − 1) | 130 | 4.5 | 170 | 0.8 |
Standard deviation (n − 1) | 11 | 2.1 | 13 | 0.89 |
Number of Events | Number of Words | Number of Sentences | Number of Revisions | Number of Bursts | Mean Burst Length | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | 455/305 | 50/37 | 2.2/2.5 | 24/18 | 47/78 | 11.4/4.1 |
Standard deviation | 181/183 | 24/20 | 2.9/2.2 | 11.7/8.6 | 21/46 | 8.4/1.2 |
Minimum | 121/188 | 15/14 | 0/0 | 3/6 | 10/26 | 6/3 |
Q1 | 314/208 | 30/26 | 0/0 | 17/13 | 31/54 | 7/3 |
Median | 448/242 | 46/30 | 1/4 | 24/17 | 43/62 | 8/3 |
Q3 | 558/297 | 69/37 | 3/4 | 30/20 | 55/82 | 10/4 |
Maximum | 872/854 | 98/89 | 11/6 | 58/40 | 102/208 | 51/7 |
Parameter | Mean (Typical) | Standard Deviation (Typical) | Mean (Dyslexia) | Standard Deviation (Dyslexia) | Mann-Whitley U (p-Value) | Rank-Biserial r |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mode 1 | 0.37 | 0.13 | 0.48 | 0.27 | 0.65 | −0.10 |
Standard deviation 1 | 0.46 | 0.11 | 0.58 | 0.14 | 0.02 | −0.49 |
Mode 2 | 1.30 | 0.51 | 1.88 | 0.57 | 0.0077 | −0.54 |
Standard deviation 2 | 0.68 | 0.09 | 0.72 | 0.13 | 0.25 | −0.70 |
Mode 1 weight | 0.60 | 0.10 | 0.59 | 0.10 | 0.65 | 0.09 |
% of revisions | 0.12 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.09 | 0.35 |
Baseline | Between Words | Before Period | After Period | Before Revision | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (typical) | 0.05 | 0.28 | 0.58 | 0.64 | 0.45 |
Mean (dyslexia) | 0.15 | 0.64 | 0.5 | 0.63 | 0.59 |
p-value | 8.6 × 10−10 | 3.5 × 10−9 | 0.86 | 0.63 | 0.26 |
r | −1 | −0.99 | 0.04 | −0.11 | −0.33 |
Baseline | Between Words | Before Period | After Period | Before Revision | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (typical) | 0.34 | 0.38 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.19 |
Mean (dyslexia) | 0.46 | 0.33 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.15 |
p-value | 7 × 10−5 | 0.06 | 0.37 | 0.44 | 0.23 |
r | −0.77 | 0.39 | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.26 |
Pause Length (In Seconds) | Burst Length (N° of Characters) | Burst |
---|---|---|
13.775 | 6 | il c’s |
1.264 | 4 | étai |
1.638 | 13 | t fait punir |
3.135 | 6 | |
2.465 | 2 | |
2.106 | 1 | |
4.446 | 0 | |
1.497 | 2 | |
2.277 | 3 | il |
1.435 | 64 | disait que c’etait de la faute de trois fille |
21.56 | 0 | |
2.403 | 0 | |
7.69 | 1 | , |
2.293 | 30 | une des trois fille s’aooekler |
1.124 | 0 | |
5.694 | 7 | ppelerv |
1.451 | 1 | |
1.077 | 4 | léa |
50.373 | 21 | mais maintenant elle |
1.341 | 14 | n’ais plyus da |
9.454 | 0 | |
1.996 | 14 | us dans cette |
1.092 | 6 | ecokle |
1.17 | 3 | |
4.929 | 8 | |
1.95 | 1 | |
1.17 | 4 | a de |
1.154 | 14 | uxième s’appel |
2.917 | 1 | e |
2.527 | 1 | |
2.137 | 1 | |
8.58 | 4 | eno: |
1.436 | 3 | la |
1.888 | 11 | elle aussi |
1.934 | 9 | elle n”‘a |
1.233 | 0 | |
1.498 | 21 | ‘ai plus dans l’ecole |
14.804 | 1 | |
59.499 | 1 | . |
69 bursts later | ||
17.987 | 12 | et la derni |
2.138 | 14 | ère s’appeler |
19.687 | 5 | puis |
4.992 | 1 | |
2.901 | 8 | amendine |
Pause Length (In Seconds) | Burst Length (N° of Characters) | Burst |
---|---|---|
4 | Tom | |
5.550 | 4 | joue |
3.736 | 1 | |
3.252 | 5 | a la |
2.161 | 1 | b |
1.705 | 4 | ale |
12.914 | −1 | |
1.991 | 2 | . |
8.414 | 6 | eilme |
2.061 | −1 | Eilme |
1.923 | −2 | Mli |
1.626 | 2 | e |
39.371 | 1 | p |
12.996 | 1 | r |
6.958 | 4 | ene |
3.272 | 3 | le |
4.293 | 6 | balon |
4.738 | 3 | et |
6.295 | 3 | il |
3.023 | 2 | ca |
2.895 | 3 | se |
3.024 | 3 | la |
11.579 | 6 | viter |
5.443 | −1 | |
1.537 | 2 | . |
5.274 | 2 | Em |
3.748 | 3 | ile |
2.430 | 1 | |
1.834 | 3 | et |
2.984 | 4 | tom |
5.592 | 0 | T |
28.092 | 4 | ont |
6.456 | 1 | p |
8.736 | 1 | r |
2.806 | 1 | e |
3.510 | 1 | ? |
4.780 | 1 | ? |
4.172 | −5 | |
15.371 | 1 | p |
1.576 | 3 | rer |
2.410 | 1 | |
2.975 | 1 | |
10.938 | 2 | . |
5.871 | 11 | Le papa de |
3.719 | 4 | Tom |
1.924 | 9 | et Emile |
4.244 | 4 | les |
3.917 | 3 | gon |
3.075 | 3 | de |
6.853 | 7 | et il l |
2.480 | 2 | e |
1.696 | 1 | r |
7.666 | 1 | e |
2.479 | 4 | pare |
2.509 | 1 | . |
Initial Version | Modification | Impact |
---|---|---|
Ce j[our] [this day] | Elle [she] | Temporality → subject |
ses parents [her/his parents] | / | Deletion |
Une fois [once, on day] | Je suis Lilou [I am Lilou] | Temporality → subject (incipit) |
Il a [he has] | Un [a] | Sentence base →article (Noun Phrase) |
Je ma [I my/myself] | Il était une fois [once upon a time] | Subject → temporality (narrative form) |
mais les [but the] | le petit dit [the little one says] | Coordination → parataxis |
Mais [but] | et après [and then] | Coordination → coord. & temporality |
ne revient plus jamais [never ever returned] | il devient un [he became a] | Verbal clause → sentence |
les trois filles s [the three girls …] | il disait les trois filles [he said the three girls] | Main sentence → Reported speech |
que c’était [that it was] | que les filles se moquer [that the girls were mocking] | Cleft sentence → complement clause |
qyuand le maitreudertiam [when the teacher] | qui tape [who hit] | Temporal → relative clause |
Il [he] | et y en a un [and there is one] | Subject → coordinated sentence |
EN [in] | et il [and he] | Location → coordinated sentence |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Cislaru, G.; Feltgen, Q.; Khoury, E.; Delorme, R.; Bucci, M.P. Language Processing Units Are Not Equivalent to Sentences: Evidence from Writing Tasks in Typical and Dyslexic Children. Languages 2024, 9, 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050155
Cislaru G, Feltgen Q, Khoury E, Delorme R, Bucci MP. Language Processing Units Are Not Equivalent to Sentences: Evidence from Writing Tasks in Typical and Dyslexic Children. Languages. 2024; 9(5):155. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050155
Chicago/Turabian StyleCislaru, Georgeta, Quentin Feltgen, Elie Khoury, Richard Delorme, and Maria Pia Bucci. 2024. "Language Processing Units Are Not Equivalent to Sentences: Evidence from Writing Tasks in Typical and Dyslexic Children" Languages 9, no. 5: 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050155
APA StyleCislaru, G., Feltgen, Q., Khoury, E., Delorme, R., & Bucci, M. P. (2024). Language Processing Units Are Not Equivalent to Sentences: Evidence from Writing Tasks in Typical and Dyslexic Children. Languages, 9(5), 155. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050155