Next Article in Journal
Moving From the Old to the New: Insecticide Research on Bed Bugs since the Resurgence
Next Article in Special Issue
Phylogeography of the Cactophilic Drosophila and Other Arthropods Associated with Cactus Necroses in the Sonoran Desert
Previous Article in Journal
Host Searching and Aggregation Activity of Recently Fed and Unfed Bed Bugs (Cimex lectularius L.)
Previous Article in Special Issue
Phylogeography of Phytophagous Weevils and Plant Species in Broadleaved Evergreen Forests: A Congruent Genetic Gap between Western and Eastern Parts of Japan
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Speciation, Divergence, and the Origin of Gryllus rubens: Behavior, Morphology, and Molecules

Department of Biology, California State University, Northridge, 18111 Nordhoff Street, Northridge, CA 91330, USA
Insects 2011, 2(2), 195-209; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects2020195
Submission received: 30 March 2011 / Revised: 22 April 2011 / Accepted: 26 April 2011 / Published: 4 May 2011
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Phylogeographic Syntheses)

Abstract

: The last 25 years or so has seen a huge resurgence of interest in speciation research. This has coincided with the development and widespread use of new tools in molecular genetics, especially DNA sequencing, to inform ecological and evolutionary questions. Here I review about a decade of work on the sister species of field crickets Gryllus texensis and G. rubens. This work has included analysis of morphology, behavior, and the mitochondrial DNA molecule. The molecular work in particular has dramatically re-shaped my interpretation of the speciational history of these taxa, suggesting that rather than ‘sister’ species we should consider these taxa as ‘mother-daughter’ species with G. rubens derived from within a subset of ancestral G. texensis.

1. Introduction

The process of speciation remains one the central issues in evolutionary biology. For most of the ca. 150 years since Darwin's Origin, speciation research was lower profile, and lower priority, than other major topics in evolution, namely (1) the integration of Mendelian genetics and the flourishing of quantitative population genetics, and (2) the measurement of allozyme genetic variation in wild populations. Evolution by natural selection (inclusive of sexual selection) was generally considered to be such a slow process that intrinsically historical research agendas, such as speciation, were considered nearly intractable. Subsequent direct observation of natural selection in the wild [1] as well as renewed interest in sympatric speciation, starting most prominently with Guy Bush's work with Rhagoletis [2], have together revitalized interest in speciation research and made it one of the most active areas within evolutionary biology today [310]. At about the same time, there have been enormous advances in molecular genetics, particularly DNA sequencing, as well as bioinformatics approaches to phylogenetics and phylogeography [11]. Today it would be almost inconceivable to study speciation without molecular data bolstering interpretations of host shifts [12,13], sympatry [14,15], and/or adaptive genetic divergence [16,17]. In this review, I will re-cap about a decade of work on speciation and reproductive isolation in the southern USA trilling field crickets, Gryllus rubens and Gryllus texensis. Rather than follow this research strictly chronologically, I will move from behavior, to eco-morphology, to molecular genetics, while hopefully illustrating the singularly enormous contribution that the molecular genetic data has made to my current thinking about speciation of these taxa. I will also indulge in far more speculation than is typically allowed in publications of primary scientific data sets. Hopefully many of those speculations will one day be tested, whether shown to be correct or not. More specifically, I hope that other researchers, including graduate students and postdocs (especially those savvy with advanced molecular techniques) find this system worthy of their time and study.

2. Reproductive Behavior

Acoustically communicating insects have been among the many ‘non-model’ models of speciation research over the past 40 or so years. Because of the prominence Dobzhansky and Mayr gave allopatric divergence followed by the reinforcement of pre-zygotic barriers as a model of speciation [18,19]; reproductive character displacement, that is, greater separation of species-specific reproductive characters in sympatry than in allopatry, became a key element in the search for indirect evidence of the process of speciation [20]. One of the most influential of the early reviews of reproductive character displacement compiled data from acoustically communicating insects, primarily crickets and katydids, and concluded that the evidence for reproductive character displacement was scarce at best [21]. Although there are now several strong examples of reproductive character displacement in acoustically communicating insects [22,23], it was against the background of Walker's influential review that Bill Cade and I undertook to test for behavioral divergence and reproductive character displacement between the southeastern USA trilling field crickets Gryllus rubens and G. texensis (for photos and songs, see the Singing Insects of North America website [24]). What we hoped would make our study significant was our ability to test female responses to male song in addition to male song parameters, in particular pulse rate. We decided to do this using the first generation offspring of wild caught females, isolated from males after capture, which would therefore be sets of full and/or half siblings with paternity that reflected female mating decisions in the wild.

Detailed methodology is provided in [25], but basically what we did was collect adult females from throughout the range of G. texensis, bring those females into the lab, and rear each female's offspring separately. After adult eclosion we recorded the calling songs of males, and we tested females' responses to variation in song. Thus for each wild-caught female we obtained pulse rate data from her sons, and female preference for pulse rate data from her daughters. From these data, for G. texensis, we were able to show (1) that male pulse rate has heritable genetic variation, with h2 roughly 40%, (2) that female preference for pulse rate also has heritable genetic variation, ca. 38%, (3) that there was a significant genetic correlation between male pulse rate and female preference for pulse rate, rG = 0.49, and (4) that neither male song nor female preference for song differed across the geographic range in a way consistent with reproductive character displacement—there was in fact strikingly little geographic variation in G. texensis song and preference. At the time we did not collect enough G. rubens to conduct a parallel test in that species, but subsequent work has shown that G. rubens also lacks reproductive character displacement in male calling song [26] and that extensive field collections show strongly bimodal nearly non-overlapping pulse rates (Figure 1).

We interpreted those results in the context of the predictions of the reinforcement model, and in light of what we already knew about hybrid viability in crosses of G. texensis and G. rubens. The essence of the reinforcement model of speciation is that selection acts against hybrid mating because of the lower fitness of hybrids, which in turn is due to divergence that has already taken place in allopatry. That is, the reinforcement model specifically predicts that hybrids are unfit—i.e., some degree of post-zygotic isolation exists, which therefore selects for increased pre-zygotic isolation. From previous work with these species, we already knew that viable hybrids could be formed in the laboratory [27,28], and that they had pulse rates intermediate between G. rubens and G. texensis [29,30], see Figure 1. So it appeared to us that neither the preconditions of the reinforcement model, post-zygotic isolation, nor its predictions, reproductive character displacement, were met. Instead we interpreted our results as suggesting the primary importance of sexual selection causing song divergence—and thereby effecting reproductive isolation and speciation. The sexual selection model was indirectly supported by the female preference data, and by the quantitative genetic data including a genetic correlation sufficiently strong to suggest the potential for rapid Fisherian runaway coevolution of male trait and female preference [31,32].

Our studies of cricket reproductive isolation had thus far mostly emphasized the long distance and comparatively loud calling song. It is the pulse rate of that song that we had determined was the principal character that we could use to delimit the species, and it also seems to be of primary importance for how the crickets themselves recognize species. However cricket mating behavior is considerably more complex than just female phonotaxis to male calling song; we thought it was important to characterize the close-range courtship interactions between males and females to gain a more complete picture of reproductive isolation. In field crickets generally, the mating sequence could be characterized as (1) male calling; (2) female phonotaxis to calling song; (3) mutual antennation and assessment of cuticular hydrocarbons; (4) male courtship song; (5) spermatophore transfer, and (6) sperm storage and utilization [3335]. Steps 1 and 2, male calling song and female phonotaxis, do not always precede mating however. Many males call infrequently or not at all [36-38], and may adopt a satellite strategy [39,40], and/or actively search for females especially at high population density [4143]. Reduced male calling activity may reflect a history of attack by the tachinid fly, Ormia ochracea, which locates hosts via the male's calling song [4449].

Given that males and females may encounter one another without calling song, we conducted several studies to examine divergence in male courtship song, and in male and female close-range mating behaviors. Courtship song in G. rubens and G. texensis is composed of a series of quieter mostly pure tone lower-frequency ticks (ca. 5 kHz) with louder broader spectrum higher-frequency ticks (most sound energy 11–14 kHz) at regular intervals (Figure 2). Just by recording and analyzing male courtship songs, we were able to show that G. rubens and G. texensis differ in their courtship song rates in a manner that mirrors their calling song pulse rates: G. texensis has faster courtship song than G. rubens [50], and experimental manipulation of the diet showed that courtship song features did not appear to reflect male nutritional condition [51]. Based on this, my lab conducted two separate studies of close-range courtship and mating behavior with these species. The first [52], used muted males with virgin females of both species. Males that courted females, although themselves mute, were accompanied by a synthetic species average courtship song played via a tweeter directly under the pair of crickets. Thus males had the opportunity to court females, or not, and females had two experimentally separated sources of information about male species identity (1) from their cuticular hydrocarbons (unmanipulated) and (2) from the accompanying courtship song (manipulated). I replicated the entire experiment with crickets from allopatry and sympatry, using G. texensis from Austin, Texas and Tuscalloosa, Alabama, and G. rubens from Gainesville, Florida, and from Tuscallosa, Alabama. In short, the results showed separate significant effects of male species identity and courtship song played on the likelihood of female mounting; there was no difference in either species between the behaviors of crickets from sympatry and allopatry (Figure 3). Although these results show divergence in close-range courtship mating behaviors, they also notably show that despite significant species effects, the potential for hybridization is not trivial—on the assumption of no prior female phonotaxis to male calling song.

The second courtship study we conducted was motivated by the idea that females may control paternity via selectively re-mating. Female crickets generally are polyandrous [5363] and G. texensis is no exception [64]. Because sperm competition in these crickets favors the last male to mate (average paternity of the second male to mate in a study of G. texensis was 72% [65]), females first mated to a conspecific should be very reluctant to engage in a second mating with a heterospecific. Our results somewhat supported these predictions [66], but differed in some interesting and suggestive ways. Males of both species courted females of both species at equal rates [66], as I had found for G. texensis males in my previous study [52]; G. rubens males had preferentially courted conspecifics in that study. G. texensis females basically met our predictions: upon first exposure to males, female G. texensis preferred conspecific males, and they preferred conspecific males following exposure to conspecific males, i.e., they were unwilling to ‘trade-down’ to heterospecific males. Choosiness was relaxed following first exposure to heterospecific males. Female G. rubens, however, appeared to prefer heterospecific males upon either first or subsequent exposure. Results for both species are summarized in Figure 4. The lack of choosiness in G. rubens we interpreted in light of a Kaneshiro effect and what we then knew about their evolutionary history (see below).

To summarize the results discussed so far: behavioral reproductive isolation between these taxa appears to be very strong. First, isolation by male calling song and female phonotaxis approaches 100%—male pulse rate distributions are nearly non-overlapping, and females show strong preferences for pulse rates of conspecifics. Second, in those few instances of phonotaxis to heterospecific song that might occur [or encounters in the absence of calling song (i.e., via random encounter)] courtship interactions reduce the likelihood of mating, mostly mediated by female selectivity not male selectivity. These studies, while informative, obviously beg the question of eco-morphological divergence, and the degree of genetic isolation and divergence. That is, the observed levels of species behavioral divergence, the absence of readily identifiable hybrids in the field, and the results showing strong behavioral isolation all suggest that some degree of ecological divergence might facilitate species persistence and also suggests that genetic reproductive isolation should be quite strong.

3. Eco-Morphological Divergence

At the time that Bill Cade and I started our character displacement study, there were no known morphological means of distinguishing between these two species. That situation is unchanged for males, however during our work we observed that female G. rubens appeared to have longer ovipositors relative to body size than did female G. texensis. To confirm this, we measured ovipositor length and pronotal width, and we found that females were in fact quite strongly divergent in relative ovipositor length [67]. In particular, if we make a histogram of ovipositor length divided by pronotal width, we see imperfect but bimodal separation (Figure 5). Ovipositor length in crickets is clearly an ecologically linked trait; effects of phylogenetic history, latitude, egg size, oviposition behavior, diapause strategy, and soil type have all been described [6873]. G. rubens and G. texensis share their deeper phylogenetic history, are found at similar latitudes, and both have facultative nymphal diapause with no egg diapause observed. Egg size, oviposition strategy, and soil preferences have not been examined, but G. rubens, in Florida and the southeastern coastal areas especially, experiences sandier soils than elsewhere throughout the species' ranges. Sandy soils drain and dry quickly compared to more loamy or clay-laden soils, and crickets are known to oviposit deeper in dryer sandy soil [73]. The congeneric species pair, G. firmus (known as the ‘sand field cricket’) and G. pennsylvanicus show a similar pattern: both have recent shared evolutionary history, both are egg diapausing species, but G. firmus has a considerably longer ovipositor than the more inland G. pennsylvanicus [71,74]. Ovipositor length thus appears to be an indicator of adaptive ecological divergence between G. rubens and G. texensis. This would be especially true for G. rubens populations in the southeastern US coastal areas, including peninsular Florida.

4. Genetic Divergence and Species History

Unfortunately, at the present time, the only data available to test genetic divergence and illuminate the species' deeper evolutionary history are mitochondrial sequence data; however those mitochondrial data show clear and interesting patterns that greatly inform our understanding of speciation in these taxa.

Our first study was with the mitochondrial Cytochrome c Oxidase I (COI) and cytochrome b (CYT-B) genes. It showed that G. texensis had much higher levels of genetic variation than did G. rubens, that the gene trees showed species level separation, but were not reciprocally monophyletic, and that G. rubens appeared closely related to only one subset of the G. texensis variation [75]. That study consisted of only 10 individuals for each species, but the essential results were all later confirmed in a much larger analysis (see below) and our genetic variation results were concordant with previous work showing low levels of genetic variation in G. rubens [76]. Although informative, this first data set was nonetheless insufficient for large scale phylogeographic analysis.

To really get insight into these species' evolutionary past, I wanted a much larger sample of crickets, both in terms of numbers of individuals and geographic coverage. Fortunately, I had kept, preserved in 100% ethanol, nearly all of the many hundreds of wild caught crickets described in the previous behavioral section of this review, and had even collected or acquired a number of additional samples from parts of the species' ranges not before sampled. Because our first study [75] showed higher variation in COI than in CYT-B, I extracted DNA and amplified a portion of COI resulting in 724 aligned bases from 365 individuals from 48 collection localities spread throughout the species' ranges (Figure 6). At this point I really needed expert help with the analysis, and I was very fortunate that Lacey Knowles and Huateng Huang agreed to a collaboration. Their analysis and simulations, although in agreement with our prior results, added novel insights and completely changed how I view the evolutionary history of these two cricket species. The major results of that study [77] were that (1) G. rubens had far lower genetic variation than G. texensis, (2) G. texensis sequences could be divided into two mostly discrete groups, (3) G. rubens sequences were mostly nested within one subset of the G. texensis sequences, (4) that G. rubens, but not G. texensis, showed indications of both recent population expansion and isolation by distance, and (5) that recent hybridization/gene flow is an unlikely explanation for the observed lack of reciprocal gene tree monophyly.

Taken together these results suggest that G. rubens arose from within a subgroup of G. texensis, i.e., that G. rubens is the daughter species of G. texensis, and that G. rubens arose in isolation, probably in sandy soil habitat within and/or near peninsular Florida, and has subsequently expanded demographically and geographically throughout its current range. Additionally, it seems likely that the significant song evolution that is the primary reproductive isolating barrier between these species took place in proto-rubens with perhaps little or no song change in G. texensis. Such a scenario also implies that G. rubens may have persisted at low population density prior to expansion. If true, this may have favored the reduced mating selectivity in G. rubens (compared to G. texensis) that we have observed in our courtship studies [66], analogous to a Kaneshiro effect [78].

In short, the molecular phylogeographic work [77] altered my interpretation of speciation in these taxa from a symmetric gradualist model to an asymmetric punctuational model, with the important changes being within the proto-rubens lineage (Figure 7). Does that imply that sexual selection and the observed genetic correlation between male song and female preference in G. texensis was not important in speciation? Genetic correlations can arise via pleiotropy or linkage, and in the case of sexually selected traits/preferences assortative mating can create and maintain a genetic correlation. For example, our previous work with a different G. texensis song character, numbers of pulses per trill (PPT), showed that there is a strong genetic correlation between male PPT and female preference for PPT in the wild, but that that genetic correlation disappears after random mating in the laboratory [79]. This demonstrates that it is assortative mating in the wild that creates and maintains the observed genetic correlation between these traits in G. texensis. In contrast, an important recent study of pulse rate and pulse rate preference quantitative trait loci in the Hawaiian crickets Laupala kohalensis and L. paranigra demonstrated that the observed strong genetic correlation was due mostly to either close physical linkage or pleiotropy [80]. The genetic basis of the G. texensis pulse rate/preference genetic correlation is unknown, and the corresponding genetic correlation has not been investigated in G. rubens. However, the phylogeographic data polarize the ancestral state of proto-rubens and suggest that that ancestor was, in effect, the G. texensis lineage that has led to the current G. texensis. As G. texensis of today show a strong pulse rate/preference genetic correlation, that shared ancestor likely did also, and so—in the absence of any supporting data whatsoever—it is reasonable to think that a similar genetic correlation may have been important in proto-rubens song/preference evolution.

5. Conclusion and Prospects for Future Work

The molecular genetic data to date have dramatically changed my interpretation of speciation between these taxa. Yet the genetic data presently available are miniscule compared to the numbers of unresolved questions that could be addressed with molecular genetics. First, and most obviously, nuclear DNA sequences, microsatellites, and/or AFLPs, etc. should be used to confirm or refute the major conclusions of the mitochondrial data. Second, determination of a pulse rate song/preference genetic correlation and its mechanistic basis in G. rubens is essential. Third, were candidate genes available, comparative work on the genetics of species divergence would be super interesting given that: (1) three features of G. rubens are currently known to be strongly divergent from G. texensis: male calling song pulse rate, female preference for calling song pulse rate, and female ovipositor length; and (2) other features are less divergent, but clearly have diverged: male courtship song rate, female preference for courtship song rate, and cuticular hydrocarbons (by implication from the courtship data in Gray 2005). For example, a comparative study of selection at the molecular genetic level for ovipositor length and cuticular hydrocarbon genes would be very interesting; I would guess that G. rubens ovipositor length evolved by comparatively recent strong selection and hydrocarbons by weak selection or drift; I would also guess that the courtship song and preference divergence evolved as a correlated response to the calling song divergence, probably because of pleiotropy of calling song/courtship song genes and also pleiotropy of calling song preference/courtship song preference genes. All of this would make excellent further study, and clarify the mechanistic basis of speciation. I hope someone takes it up.

Figure 1. Pulse rates in the calling songs of males (corrected to a common temperature) are strongly bimodal and show almost no overlap; laboratory produced hybrids are intermediate.
Figure 1. Pulse rates in the calling songs of males (corrected to a common temperature) are strongly bimodal and show almost no overlap; laboratory produced hybrids are intermediate.
Insects 02 00195f1 1024
Figure 2. Waveform (amplitude versus time) representative courtship songs of G. rubens (Figure 2a) and G. texensis (Figure 2b).
Figure 2. Waveform (amplitude versus time) representative courtship songs of G. rubens (Figure 2a) and G. texensis (Figure 2b).
Insects 02 00195f2 1024
Figure 3. Rates of female acceptance of male courtship when courtship song is experimentally decoupled from male species identity; muted males were accompanied by the species-average courtship song indicated. There were separate statistically significant effects of both male species identity and courtship song played, but no significant effect of female allopatry or sympatry for both G. rubens females (Figure 3a) and G. texensis females (Figure 3b). Figure modified from Gray (2005).
Figure 3. Rates of female acceptance of male courtship when courtship song is experimentally decoupled from male species identity; muted males were accompanied by the species-average courtship song indicated. There were separate statistically significant effects of both male species identity and courtship song played, but no significant effect of female allopatry or sympatry for both G. rubens females (Figure 3a) and G. texensis females (Figure 3b). Figure modified from Gray (2005).
Insects 02 00195f3 1024
Figure 4. Female responsiveness to males in sequential mating trials in which the first male was a conspecific, and the second male was either a conspecific or heterospecific.
Figure 4. Female responsiveness to males in sequential mating trials in which the first male was a conspecific, and the second male was either a conspecific or heterospecific.
Insects 02 00195f4 1024
Figure 5. Ovipositor length relative to body size (pronotal width) in females.
Figure 5. Ovipositor length relative to body size (pronotal width) in females.
Insects 02 00195f5 1024
Figure 6. Map of the south-eastern United States of America showing collection localities for 177 G. rubens and 188 G. texensis sampled for mitochondrial DNA genetic variation. Figure modified from Gray et al. (2008).
Figure 6. Map of the south-eastern United States of America showing collection localities for 177 G. rubens and 188 G. texensis sampled for mitochondrial DNA genetic variation. Figure modified from Gray et al. (2008).
Insects 02 00195f6 1024
Figure 7. Contrasting models of speciation. Figure 7a shows the classical gradualist model with divergence attributable to approximately equal changes in both sister lineages; Figure 7b shows a punctuational view of species formation, with the majority of divergence attributable to rapid change within one ‘daughter’ lineage and relatively little change within the ‘parental’ lineage. Currently available molecular data favor a peripatric ‘puctuational’ origin of G. rubens.
Figure 7. Contrasting models of speciation. Figure 7a shows the classical gradualist model with divergence attributable to approximately equal changes in both sister lineages; Figure 7b shows a punctuational view of species formation, with the majority of divergence attributable to rapid change within one ‘daughter’ lineage and relatively little change within the ‘parental’ lineage. Currently available molecular data favor a peripatric ‘puctuational’ origin of G. rubens.
Insects 02 00195f7 1024

References

  1. Endler, J.A. Natural Selection in the Wild; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bush, G.L. Sympatric host race formation and speciation in frugivorous flies of the genus Rhagoletis (Diptera, Tephritidae). Evolution 1969, 23, 237–251. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bolnick, D.I.; Fitzpatrick, B.M. Sympatric speciation: Models and empirical evidence. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Systemat. 2007, 38, 459–487. [Google Scholar]
  4. Coyne, J.A.; Orr, H.A. Speciation; Sinauer Associates, Inc.: Sunderland, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  5. Nosil, P.; Harmon, L.J.; Seehausen, O. Ecological explanations for (incomplete) speciation. Trend. Ecol. Evolut. 2009, 24, 145–156. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ritchie, M. Sexual selection and speciation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Systemat. 2007, 38, 79–102. [Google Scholar]
  7. Soltis, P.S.; Soltis, D.E. The role of hybridization in plant speciation. Annu. Rev. Plant Biol. 2009, 60, 561–588. [Google Scholar]
  8. Howard, D.J.; Berlocher, S.H. Endless Forms: Species and Speciation; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  9. Price, T. Speciation in Birds; Roberts & Co.: Greenwood Village, CO, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  10. Schluter, D. Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative. Science 2009, 323, 737–742. [Google Scholar]
  11. Avise, J.C. Phylogeography: The History and Formation of Species; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  12. Faucci, A.; Toonen, R.J.; Hadfield, M.G. Host shift and speciation in a coral-feeding nudibranch. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2007, 274, 111–119. [Google Scholar]
  13. Sorenson, M.D.; Sefc, K.M.; Payne, R.B. Speciation by host switch in brood parasitic indigobirds. Nature 2003, 424, 928–931. [Google Scholar]
  14. Barluenga, M.; Stölting, K.N.; Salzburger, W.; Muschick, M.; Meyer, A. Sympatric speciation in Nicaraguan crater lake cichlid fish. Nature 2006, 439, 719–723. [Google Scholar]
  15. Savolainen, V.; Anstett, M.C.; Lexer, C.; Hutton, I.; Clarkson, J.J.; Norup, M.V.; Powell, M.P.; Springate, D.; Salamin, N.; Baker, W.J. Sympatric speciation in palms on an oceanic island. Nature 2006, 441, 210–213. [Google Scholar]
  16. Seehausen, O.; Terai, Y.; Magalhaes, I.S.; Carleton, K.L.; Mrosso, H.D.J.; Miyagi, R.; van der Sluijs, I.; Schneider, M.V.; Maan, M.E.; Tachida, H.; et al. Speciation throught sensory drive in cichlid fish. Nature 2008, 455, 620–627. [Google Scholar]
  17. Via, S. Natural selection in action during speciation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 9939–9946. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dobzhansky, T. Genetics and the Origin of Species; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1937. [Google Scholar]
  19. Mayr, E. Animal Species and Evolution; Belknap Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
  20. Howard, D.J. Reinforcement: Origin, Dynamics, and Fate of an Evolutionary Hypothesis. In Hybrid Zones and the Evolutionary Process; Harrison, R.G., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1993; pp. 46–69. [Google Scholar]
  21. Walker, T.J. Character displacement and acoustic insects. Amer. Zool. 1974, 14, 1137–1150. [Google Scholar]
  22. Marshall, D.C.; Cooley, J.R. Reproductive character displacement and speciation in periodical cicadas, with description of a new species, 13-year Magicicada neotredecim. Evolution 2000, 54, 1313–1325. [Google Scholar]
  23. Jang, Y.; Gerhardt, H.C. Divergence in the calling songs between sympatric and allopatric populations of the southern wood cricket Gryllus fultoni (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J. Evol. Biol. 2006, 19, 459–472. [Google Scholar]
  24. Singing Insects of North America. Available online: http://entnemdept.ufl.edu/walker/buzz/ (accessed on 30 March 2011).
  25. Gray, D.A.; Cade, W.H. Sexual selection and speciation in field crickets. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 14449–14454. [Google Scholar]
  26. Izzo, A.S.; Gray, D.A. Cricket song in sympatry: Species specificity of song without reproductive character displacement in Gryllus rubens. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 2004, 97, 831–837. [Google Scholar]
  27. Smith, C.J.; Cade, W.H. Relative fertility in hybridization experiments using three song types of the field crickets Gryllus integer and Gryllus rubens. Can. J. Zool. 1987, 65, 2390–2394. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cade, W.H.; Tyshenko, M.G. Geographic variation in hybrid fertility in the field crickets Gryllus integer, G. rubens, and Gryllus sp. Can. J. Zool. 1990, 68, 2697–2700. [Google Scholar]
  29. Walker, T.J. Pulse rates in the songs of trilling field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae: Gryllus). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 2000, 93, 565–572. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gray, D.A. California State University, Northridge, CA, USA. Unpublished work, 1999.
  31. Lande, R. Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 1981, 78, 3721–3725. [Google Scholar]
  32. Higashi, M.; Takimoto, G.; Yamamura, N. Sympatric speciation by sexual selection. Nature 1999, 402, 523–526. [Google Scholar]
  33. Alexander, R.D. Aggressiveness, territoriality, and sexual behavior in field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Behaviour 1961, 17, 130–223. [Google Scholar]
  34. Walker, T.J.; Masaki, S. Natural History. In Cricket Behavior and Neurobiology; Huber, F., Moore, T.E., Loher, W., Eds.; Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 1989; pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar]
  35. Zuk, M.; Simmons, L.W. Reproductive strategies of the crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). In The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids; Choe, J.C., Crespi, B.J., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997; pp. 89–109. [Google Scholar]
  36. Bertram, S. The influence of rearing and monitoring environment on temporal mate signaling patterns in the field cricket, Gryllus texensis. J. Insect. Behav. 2002, 15, 127–137. [Google Scholar]
  37. Bertram, S.M. The influence of age and size on temporal mate signalling behaviour. Anim. Behav. 2000, 60, 333–339. [Google Scholar]
  38. Cade, W.H. Inter- and intraspecific variation in nightly calling duration in field crickets, Gryllus integer and G. rubens (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J. Insect. Behav. 1991, 4, 185–194. [Google Scholar]
  39. Cade, W.H. The Evolution of Alternative Male Reproductive Strategies in Field Crickets. In Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects; Blum, M.S., Blum, N.A., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1979; pp. 343–380. [Google Scholar]
  40. Cade, W.H. Alternative male strategies: Genetic differences in crickets. Science 1981, 212, 563–564. [Google Scholar]
  41. French, B.W.; Cade, W.H. Sexual selection at varying population densities in male field crickets, Gryllus veletis and Gryllus pennsylvanicus. J. Insect. Behav. 1989, 2, 105–122. [Google Scholar]
  42. Cade, W.H.; Cade, E.S. Male mating success, calling and searching behaviour at high and low densities in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Anim. Behav. 1992, 43, 49–56. [Google Scholar]
  43. Walker, S.E.; Cade, W.H. A simulation model of the effects of frequency dependence, density dependence and parasitoid flies on the fitness of male field crickets. Ecol. Model. 2003, 169, 119–130. [Google Scholar]
  44. Bertram, S.M. Temporally fluctuating selection of sex-limited signaling traits in the Texas field cricket, Gryllus texensis. Evolution 2002, 56, 1831–1839. [Google Scholar]
  45. Cade, W.H. Acoustically orienting parasitoids: Fly phonotaxis to cricket song. Science 1975, 190, 1312–1313. [Google Scholar]
  46. Cade, W.H. Effects of fly parasitoids on nightly calling duration in field crickets. Can. J. Zool. 1984, 62, 226–228. [Google Scholar]
  47. Gray, D.A.; Banuelos, C.; Walker, S.E.; Cade, W.H.; Zuk, M. Behavioural specialisation among populations of the acoustically-orienting parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea utilising different cricket species as hosts. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 99–104. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gray, D.A.; Cade, W.H. Sex, death and genetic variation: natural and sexual selection on cricket song. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 1999, 266, 707–709. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sakaguchi, K.M.; Gray, D.A. Host song selection by an acoustically-orienting parasitoid fly exploiting a multi-species assemblage of cricket hosts. Anim. Behav. 2011, 81, 851–858. [Google Scholar]
  50. Fitzpatrick, M.J.; Gray, D.A. Divergence between the courtship songs of Gryllus texensis and G. rubens (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Ethology 2001, 107, 1075–1086. [Google Scholar]
  51. Gray, D.A.; Eckhardt, G.E. Is cricket courtship song condition dependent? Anim. Behav. 2001, 62, 871–877. [Google Scholar]
  52. Gray, D.A. Does courtship behavior contribute to species-level reproductive isolation in field crickets? Behav. Ecol. 2005, 16, 201–206. [Google Scholar]
  53. Bretman, A.; Tregenza, T. Measuring polyandry in wild populations: A case study using promiscuous crickets. Mol. Ecol. 2005, 14, 2169–2179. [Google Scholar]
  54. Brown, W.D. Female remating and the intensity of female choice in black-horned tree crickets, Oecanthus nigricornis. Behav. Ecol. 1997, 8, 66–74. [Google Scholar]
  55. Burpee, D.M.; Sakaluk, S.K. Repeated matings offset costs of reproduction in female crickets. Evol. Ecol. 1993, 7, 240–250. [Google Scholar]
  56. Fleischman, R.R.; Sakaluk, S.K. Sexual conflict over remating in house crickets: No evidence of anti-aphrodisiac in males' ejaculates. Behaviour 2004, 141, 633–646. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ivy, T.M.; Sakaluk, S.K. Polyandry promotes enhanced offspring survival in decorated crickets. Evolution 2005, 59, 152–159. [Google Scholar]
  58. Jennions, M.D.; Drayton, J.M.; Brooks, R.; Hunt, J. Do female black field crickets Teleogryllus commodus benefit from polyandry? J. Evol. Biol. 2007, 20, 1469–1477. [Google Scholar]
  59. Sakaluk, S.K.; Schaus, J.M.; Eggert, A.K.; Snedden, W.A.; Brady, P.L. Polyandry and fitness of offspring reared under varying nutritional stress in decorated crickets. Evolution 2002, 56, 1999–2007. [Google Scholar]
  60. Simmons, L.W. The evolution of polyandry: Patterns of genotypic variation in female mating frequency, male fertilization success and a test of the sexy-sperm hypothesis. J. Evol. Biol. 2003, 16, 624–634. [Google Scholar]
  61. Simmons, L.W. The evolution of polyandry: Sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2005, 36, 125–146. [Google Scholar]
  62. Simmons, L.W.; Beveridge, M. The strength of postcopulatory sexual selection within natural populations of field crickets. Behav. Ecol. 2010, 21, 1179–1185. [Google Scholar]
  63. Simmons, L.W.; Beveridge, M.; Kennington, W.J. Polyandry in the wild: Temporal changes in female mating frequency and sperm competition intensity in natural populations of the tettigoniid Requena verticalis. Mol. Ecol. 2007, 16, 4613–4623. [Google Scholar]
  64. Solymar, B.; Cade, W.H. Heritable variation for female mating frequency in field crickets, Gryllus integer. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1990, 26, 73–76. [Google Scholar]
  65. Backus, V.L.; Cade, W.H. Sperm competition in the field cricket Gryllus integer (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Fla. Entomol. 1986, 69, 722–728. [Google Scholar]
  66. Izzo, A.S.; Gray, D.A. Heterospecific courtship and sequential mate choice in sister species of field crickets. Anim. Behav. 2011, 81, 259–264. [Google Scholar]
  67. Gray, D.A.; Walker, T.J.; Conley, B.E.; Cade, W.H. A morphological means of distinguishing females of the cryptic field cricket species, Gryllus rubens and G. texensis (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Fla. Entomol. 2001, 84, 314–315. [Google Scholar]
  68. Begin, M.; Roff, D.A. From micro- to macroevolution through quantitative genetic variation: positive evidence from field crickets. Evolution 2004, 58, 2287–2304. [Google Scholar]
  69. Bradford, M.J.; Guerette, P.A.; Roff, D.A. Testing hypotheses of adaptive variation in cricket ovipositor lengths. Oecologia 1993, 93, 263–267. [Google Scholar]
  70. Carrière, Y.; Masaki, S.; Roff, D.A. The coadaptation of female morphology and offspring size: A comparative analysis in crickets. Oecologia 1997, 110, 197–204. [Google Scholar]
  71. Harrison, R.G.; Arnold, J. A narrow hybrid zone between closeley related cricket species. Evolution 1982, 36, 535–552. [Google Scholar]
  72. Mousseau, T.A.; Roff, D.A. Genetic and environmental contributions to geographic variation in the ovipositor length of a cricket. Ecology 1995, 76, 1473–1482. [Google Scholar]
  73. Réale, D.; Roff, D.A. Quantitative genetics of oviposition behaviour and interactions among oviposition traits in the sand cricket. Anim. Behav. 2002, 64, 397–406. [Google Scholar]
  74. Alexander, R.D. The taxonomy of the field crickets of the eastern United States (Orthoptera: Gryllidae: Acheta). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 1957, 50, 584–602. [Google Scholar]
  75. Gray, D.A.; Barnfield, P.; Seifried, M.; Richards, M.H. Molecular divergence between Gryllus rubens and Gryllus texensis, sister species of field crickets (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). Can. Entomol. 2006, 138, 305–313. [Google Scholar]
  76. Harrison, R.G.; Bogdanowicz, S.M. Mitochondrial DNA phylogeny of North American field crickets: Perspectives on the evolution of life cycles, songs, and habitat associations. J. Evol. Biol. 1995, 8, 209–232. [Google Scholar]
  77. Gray, D.A.; Huang, H.; Knowles, L.L. Molecular evidence of a peripatric origin for two sympatric species of field crickets (Gryllus rubens and G. texensis) revealed from coalescent simulations and population genetic tests. Mol. Ecol. 2008, 17, 3836–3855. [Google Scholar]
  78. Kaneshiro, K.Y. Sexual isolation, speciation and the direction of evolution. Evolution 1980, 34, 437–444. [Google Scholar]
  79. Gray, D.A.; Cade, W.H. Quantitative genetics of sexual selection in the field cricket, Gryllus integer. Evolution 1999, 53, 848–854. [Google Scholar]
  80. Wiley, C.; Shaw, K.L. Multiple genetic linkages between female preference and male signal in rapidly speciating Hawaiian crickets. Evolution 2010, 64, 2238–2245. [Google Scholar]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gray, D.A. Speciation, Divergence, and the Origin of Gryllus rubens: Behavior, Morphology, and Molecules. Insects 2011, 2, 195-209. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects2020195

AMA Style

Gray DA. Speciation, Divergence, and the Origin of Gryllus rubens: Behavior, Morphology, and Molecules. Insects. 2011; 2(2):195-209. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects2020195

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gray, David A. 2011. "Speciation, Divergence, and the Origin of Gryllus rubens: Behavior, Morphology, and Molecules" Insects 2, no. 2: 195-209. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects2020195

APA Style

Gray, D. A. (2011). Speciation, Divergence, and the Origin of Gryllus rubens: Behavior, Morphology, and Molecules. Insects, 2(2), 195-209. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects2020195

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop