Scientific Reasoning in Science Education: From Global Measures to Fine-Grained Descriptions of Students’ Competencies

A special issue of Education Sciences (ISSN 2227-7102). This special issue belongs to the section "STEM Education".

Deadline for manuscript submissions: closed (15 August 2021) | Viewed by 52214

Printed Edition Available!
A printed edition of this Special Issue is available here.

Special Issue Editors


E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Institute of Biology, Freie Universität Berlin, 14195 Berlin, Germany
Interests: scientific reasoning competencies; modeling competencies; biology teacher education; educational assessment Photo: n/a

E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Institute for Physics Education, Justus Liebig University Giessen, D-35394 Giessen, Germany
Interests: scientific reasoning competencies; inquiry-based instruction; classroom practice; modeling of competencies; learning processes; instructional design; physics subject-matter competencies

E-Mail Website
Guest Editor
Institute for Science Education, Leibniz Universität Hannover, Am Kleinen Felde 30, 30167 Hannover, Germany
Interests: scientific reasoning; nature of science / nature of scientific inquiry; inclusive science teaching; quality of instruction in subject specific and generic perspectives

Special Issue Information

Dear Colleagues,

In modern science- and technology-based societies, competencies that enable citizens to reason scientifically play a key role not only in science and technology-based careers but also for democratic co-determination (e.g., OECD, 2019). Developing these competencies is, hence, considered an important goal for science education in many countries around the globe (e.g., KMK, 2020; NRC, 2012).

Scientific reasoning competencies are defined as a complex construct that encompasses abilities such as identifying scientific problems, developing questions and hypotheses, categorizing and classifying entities, engaging in probabilistic reasoning, generating evidence through modeling, experimentation, etc., as well as communicating, evaluating, and scrutinizing claims (Lawson, 2004; NRC, 2012). These abilities require different forms of knowledge, such as content knowledge about the concepts of science, procedural knowledge about scientific methods, and epistemic knowledge of how such procedures warrant the claims that scientists advance (Osborne, 2014).

Research on scientific reasoning competencies is quite diverse. This diversity is—at least in part—caused by the manifold abilities that models of scientific reasoning comprise and the wide range of content, procedural, and epistemic knowledge that is deemed necessary to unfold these abilities. Differences exist, for instance, in the specific abilities that are addressed (e.g., applying the control-of-variables strategy: Reith & Nehring, 2020; handling of anomalous data: Chinn & Brewer, 2001; formulating questions and hypotheses: Vorholzer et al., 2016; 2020; developing and using models: Göhner & Krell, 2020). In addition, even studies that focus on similar abilities may use different theoretical frameworks and address different procedural and epistemic concepts (Vorholzer et al., 2016). Moreover, studies focus on a broad spectrum of respondents ranging from K-12 students (e.g., Koerber & Osterhaus, 2019; Mayer et al., 2014; Nehring et al., 2015; Vorholzer et al., 2016) to pre-service (e.g., Khan & Krell, 2019) and in-service teachers (e.g., Krell & Krüger, 2016).

Empirical research that focuses on scientific reasoning competencies typically describe the addressed competencies with a rather large grain-size: On the one hand, most studies offer a clear conceptual description of the addressed competencies, while the specific abilities, as well as the corresponding procedural and epistemic knowledge, often remain unclear (Vorholzer et al., 2016). For instance, a study may report that it focuses on students’ competencies to develop scientific investigations without stating whether that entails just knowledge of the control of variables strategy, or also knowledge of strategies, such as repeating measurements or measuring with large quantities. On the other hand, empirical studies often report aggregated measures, for instance in the form of a global scientific reasoning competency measure, a global measure of students’ procedural understanding of the control of variables strategy or a global measure of their epistemic understanding (e.g., naïve vs. sophisticated). This grain-size is completely sufficient when the goal of a study is, for instance, to investigate the effectiveness of a specific instructional intervention or to analyze the dimensionality of a competency model. Studies that utilize this grain-size have provided many vital insights regarding the modeling, assessment, and ways of fostering scientific reasoning competencies. However, goals such as designing instructions that match students’ current understanding and specific learning needs require more detailed insights. Therefore, from an educational point of view, insights into which procedural and epistemic knowledge students have and which they lack, respectively, and which abilities they have mastered and which they have not, is of vital importance for instructional practice. Such insights also provide manifold opportunities for further research, for instance in the development of students’ scientific reasoning competencies and the corresponding learning processes.

This Special Issue will provide a more fine-grained perspective on scientific reasoning competencies, illuminating specific abilities as well as the corresponding knowledge that constitute scientific reasoning, and the extent to which students of different age groups have these abilities and this knowledge. We invite studies from science education, educational psychology, and related fields that are concerned with the modeling and assessment of scientific reasoning competencies for all age groups. We are particularly interested in studies that focus on procedural and epistemic components of scientific reasoning competencies. We also invite contributions with a focus on content knowledge that have a strong link to scientific reasoning competencies (e.g., investigations of the relationship between content knowledge and the ability to reason in a given scientific context).

All contributions to this Special Issue are expected to provide an overview of their theoretical framework and fine-grained description of their specific operationalization of scientific reasoning competencies. In addition, we are looking for studies that provide detailed accounts of students’ abilities and their understanding of procedural and epistemic concepts that go beyond global measures. Contributions can be but are not limited to one of the following:

  1. Theoretical contributions: How are or should scientific reasoning competencies be conceptualized and modeled?

Articles that adopt a fine-grained perspective on the knowledge and the abilities that constitute scientific reasoning competency, their structure, or their interrelationship.

  1. Empirical contributions: What do we know about students’ scientific reasoning competencies?

Qualitative, quantitative, or mixed methods studies that provide detailed accounts of the scientific reasoning competencies and students’ understanding of corresponding (procedural or epistemic) concepts. Empirical contributions may also investigate interindividual or intraindividual differences, as well as relationships between scientific reasoning competencies and external factors. Re-analyses of existing studies that examine data on a finer grain size (e.g., on item level) are very welcome.

  1. Methodological contributions: How to capture and assess scientific reasoning competencies on a fine-grained level?

Empirical or theoretical contributions that explicitly focus on novel or established methodological approaches to capturing and assessing scientific reasoning competencies on a fine-grained level as well as the challenges and chances that come along with them.

Interested contributors are asked to send a short structured abstract (cf., https://www.mdpi.com/journal/education/instructions) to one of the editors or the Editorial Office ([email protected]).

References:

Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (2001). Models of data: A theory of how people evaluate data. Cognition and Instruction, 19(3), 323–393.

Göhner, M. & Krell, M. (2020). Preservice science teachers’ strategies in scientific reasoning: The case of modeling. Research in Science Education. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-020-09945-7

Khan, S. & Krell, M. (2019). Scientific reasoning competencies: A case of preservice teacher education. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics and Technology Education, 19, 446–464.

KMK [Sekretariat der Ständigen Konferenz der Kultusminister der Länder in der BRD] (Eds.). (2020). Bildungsstandards im Fach Biologie für die Allgemeine Hochschulreife. https://www.kmk.org/fileadmin/Dateien/veroeffentlichungen_beschluesse/2020/2020_06_18-BildungsstandardsAHR_Biologie.pdf

Koerber, S. & Osterhaus, C. (2019). Individual differences in early scientific thinking: Assessment, cognitive influences, and their relevance for science learning. Journal of Cognition and Development, 20(4), 510–533.

Krell, M., & Krüger, D. (2016). Testing models: A key aspect to promote teaching activities related to models and modelling in biology lessons? Journal of Biological Education, 50, 160–173.

Lawson, A. (2004). The nature and development of scientific reasoning: A synthetic view. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 2(3), 307–338.

Mayer, D., Sodian, B., Koerber, S. & Schwippert, K. (2014). Scientific reasoning in elementary school children: Assessment and relations with cognitive abilities. Learning and Instruction, 29, 43–55.

Nehring, A., Nowak, K. H., Upmeier zu Belzen, A. & Tiemann, R. (2015). Predicting students’ skills in the context of scientific inquiry with cognitive, motivational, and sociodemographic variables. International Journal of Science Education, 37, 1343–1363.

NGSS Lead States (Eds.). (2013). Next Generation Science Standards: For states, by states. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

National Research Council (2012). A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

OECD (2019). Conceptual learning framework: Learning Compass 2030. https://www.oecd.org/education/2030-project/teaching-and-learning/learning/learning-compass-2030/OECD_Learning_Compass_2030_concept_note.pdf

Osborne, J. (2014). Scientific practices and inquiry in the science classroom. In N. G. Lederman & S. K. Abell (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education. Volume 2 (pp. 579–599). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

Reith, M., & Nehring, A. (2020). Scientific reasoning and views on the nature of scientific inquiry: testing a new framework to understand and model epistemic cognition in science. International Journal of Science Education. doi:10.1080/09500693.2020.1834168

Vorholzer, A., von Aufschnaiter, C., & Kirschner, S. (2016). Entwicklung und Erprobung eines Tests zur Erfassung des Verständnisses experimenteller Denk- und Arbeitsweisen. Zeitschrift für Didaktik der Naturwissenschaften, 22(1), 25–41.

Vorholzer, A., von Aufschnaiter, C. & Boone, W. (2020). Fostering upper secondary students’ ability to engage in practices of scientific investigation: A comparative analysis of an explicit and an implicit instructional approach. Research in Science Education, 50, 333–359.

Dr. Moritz Krell
Prof. Dr. Andreas Vorholzer
Prof. Dr. Andreas Nehring
Guest Editors

Manuscript Submission Information

Manuscripts should be submitted online at www.mdpi.com by registering and logging in to this website. Once you are registered, click here to go to the submission form. Manuscripts can be submitted until the deadline. All submissions that pass pre-check are peer-reviewed. Accepted papers will be published continuously in the journal (as soon as accepted) and will be listed together on the special issue website. Research articles, review articles as well as short communications are invited. For planned papers, a title and short abstract (about 100 words) can be sent to the Editorial Office for announcement on this website.

Submitted manuscripts should not have been published previously, nor be under consideration for publication elsewhere (except conference proceedings papers). All manuscripts are thoroughly refereed through a double-blind peer-review process. A guide for authors and other relevant information for submission of manuscripts is available on the Instructions for Authors page. Education Sciences is an international peer-reviewed open access monthly journal published by MDPI.

Please visit the Instructions for Authors page before submitting a manuscript. The Article Processing Charge (APC) for publication in this open access journal is 1800 CHF (Swiss Francs). Submitted papers should be well formatted and use good English. Authors may use MDPI's English editing service prior to publication or during author revisions.

Keywords

  • science education
  • scientific reasoning
  • competencies
  • assessment
  • 21st-century skills

Published Papers (16 papers)

Order results
Result details
Select all
Export citation of selected articles as:

Editorial

Jump to: Research, Review

8 pages, 496 KiB  
Editorial
Scientific Reasoning in Science Education: From Global Measures to Fine-Grained Descriptions of Students’ Competencies
by Moritz Krell, Andreas Vorholzer and Andreas Nehring
Educ. Sci. 2022, 12(2), 97; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12020097 - 30 Jan 2022
Cited by 7 | Viewed by 3360
Abstract
In modern science- and technology-based societies, competencies that allow citizens to reason scientifically play a key role for science- and technology-based careers as well as for democratic co-determination (e [...] Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Research

Jump to: Editorial, Review

20 pages, 298 KiB  
Article
Models as Epistemic Artifacts for Scientific Reasoning in Science Education Research
by Marvin Rost and Tarja Knuuttila
Educ. Sci. 2022, 12(4), 276; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12040276 - 13 Apr 2022
Cited by 6 | Viewed by 3304
Abstract
Models are at the core of scientific reasoning and science education. They are especially crucial in scientific and educational contexts where the primary objects of study are unobservables. While empirical science education researchers apply philosophical arguments in their discussions of models and modeling, [...] Read more.
Models are at the core of scientific reasoning and science education. They are especially crucial in scientific and educational contexts where the primary objects of study are unobservables. While empirical science education researchers apply philosophical arguments in their discussions of models and modeling, we in turn look at exemplary empirical studies through the lense of philosophy of science. The studied cases tend to identify modeling with representation, while simultaneously approaching models as tools. We argue that such a dual approach is inconsistent, and suggest considering models as epistemic artifacts instead. The artifactual approach offers many epistemic benefits. The access to unobservable target systems becomes less mysterious when models are not approached as more or less accurate representations, but rather as tools constructed to answer theoretical and empirical questions. Such a question-oriented approach contributes to a more consistent theoretical understanding of modeling and interpretation of the results of empirical research. Full article
20 pages, 925 KiB  
Article
Describing the Development of the Assessment of Biological Reasoning (ABR)
by Jennifer Schellinger, Patrick J. Enderle, Kari Roberts, Sam Skrob-Martin, Danielle Rhemer and Sherry A. Southerland
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(11), 669; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11110669 - 21 Oct 2021
Cited by 2 | Viewed by 1798
Abstract
Assessments of scientific reasoning that capture the intertwining aspects of conceptual, procedural and epistemic knowledge are often associated with intensive qualitative analyses of student responses to open-ended questions, work products, interviews, discourse and classroom observations. While such analyses provide evaluations of students’ reasoning [...] Read more.
Assessments of scientific reasoning that capture the intertwining aspects of conceptual, procedural and epistemic knowledge are often associated with intensive qualitative analyses of student responses to open-ended questions, work products, interviews, discourse and classroom observations. While such analyses provide evaluations of students’ reasoning skills, they are not scalable. The purpose of this study is to develop a three-tiered multiple-choice assessment to measure students’ reasoning about biological phenomena and to understand the affordances and limitations of such an assessment. To validate the assessment and to understand what the assessment measures, qualitative and quantitative data were collected and analyzed, including read-aloud, focus group interviews and analysis of large sample data sets. These data served to validate our three-tiered assessment called the Assessment of Biological Reasoning (ABR) consisting of 10 question sets focused on core biological concepts. Further examination of our data suggests that students’ reasoning is intertwined in such a way that procedural and epistemic knowledge is reliant on and given meaning by conceptual knowledge, an idea that pushes against the conceptualization that the latter forms of knowledge construction are more broadly applicable across disciplines. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

9 pages, 376 KiB  
Article
Patterns of Scientific Reasoning Skills among Pre-Service Science Teachers: A Latent Class Analysis
by Samia Khan and Moritz Krell
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 647; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100647 - 15 Oct 2021
Cited by 7 | Viewed by 1714
Abstract
We investigated the scientific reasoning competencies of pre-service science teachers (PSTs) using a multiple-choice assessment. This assessment targeted seven reasoning skills commonly associated with scientific investigation and scientific modeling. The sample consisted of 112 PSTs enrolled in a secondary teacher education program. A [...] Read more.
We investigated the scientific reasoning competencies of pre-service science teachers (PSTs) using a multiple-choice assessment. This assessment targeted seven reasoning skills commonly associated with scientific investigation and scientific modeling. The sample consisted of 112 PSTs enrolled in a secondary teacher education program. A latent class (LC) analysis was conducted to evaluate if there are subgroups with distinct patterns of reasoning skills. The analysis revealed two subgroups, where LC1 (73% of the PSTs) had a statistically higher probability of solving reasoning tasks than LC2. Specific patterns of reasoning emerged within each subgroup. Within LC1, tasks involving analyzing data and drawing conclusions were answered correctly more often than tasks involving formulating research questions and generating hypotheses. Related to modeling, tasks on testing models were solved more often than those requiring judgment on the purpose of models. This study illustrates the benefits of applying person-centered statistical analyses, such as LC analysis, to identify subgroups with distinct patterns of scientific reasoning skills in a larger sample. The findings also suggest that highlighting specific skills in teacher education, such as: formulating research questions, generating hypotheses, and judging the purposes of models, would better enhance the full complement of PSTs’ scientific reasoning competencies. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

21 pages, 6423 KiB  
Article
Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective
by Sabine Meister and Annette Upmeier zu Belzen
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 639; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100639 - 14 Oct 2021
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 2071
Abstract
In this study, we investigated participants’ reactions to supportive and anomalous data in the context of population dynamics. Based on previous findings on conceptions about ecosystems and responses to anomalous data, we assumed a tendency to confirm the initial prediction after dealing with [...] Read more.
In this study, we investigated participants’ reactions to supportive and anomalous data in the context of population dynamics. Based on previous findings on conceptions about ecosystems and responses to anomalous data, we assumed a tendency to confirm the initial prediction after dealing with contradicting data. Our aim was to integrate a product-based analysis, operationalized as prediction group changes with process-based analyses of individual data-based scientific reasoning processes to gain a deeper insight into the ongoing cognitive processes. Based on a theoretical framework describing a data-based scientific reasoning process, we developed an instrument assessing initial and subsequent predictions, confidence change toward these predictions, and the subprocesses data appraisal, data explanation, and data interpretation. We analyzed the data of twenty pre-service biology teachers applying a mixed-methods approach. Our results show that participants tend to maintain their initial prediction fully or change to predictions associated with a mix of different conceptions. Maintenance was observed even if most participants were able to use sophisticated conceptual knowledge during their processes of data-based scientific reasoning. Furthermore, our findings implicate the role of confidence changes and the influences of test wiseness. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

32 pages, 446 KiB  
Article
Professional Knowledge and Self-Efficacy Expectations of Pre-Service Teachers Regarding Scientific Reasoning and Diagnostics
by Dagmar Hilfert-Rüppell, Monique Meier, Daniel Horn and Kerstin Höner
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 629; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100629 - 11 Oct 2021
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 2722
Abstract
Understanding and knowledge of scientific reasoning skills is a key ability of pre-service teachers. In a written survey (open response format), biology and chemistry pre-service teachers (n = 51) from two German universities claimed central decisions or actions school students have to [...] Read more.
Understanding and knowledge of scientific reasoning skills is a key ability of pre-service teachers. In a written survey (open response format), biology and chemistry pre-service teachers (n = 51) from two German universities claimed central decisions or actions school students have to perform in scientific reasoning in the open inquiry instruction of an experiment. The participants’ answers were assessed in a quality content analysis using a rubric system generated from a theoretical background. Instruments in a closed response format were used to measure attitudes towards the importance of diagnostics in teacher training and the domain-specific expectations of self-efficacy. The pre-service teacher lacked pedagogical (didactics) content knowledge about potential student difficulties and also exhibited a low level of content methodological (procedural) knowledge. There was no correlation between the knowledge of student difficulties and the approach to experimenting with expectations of self-efficacy for diagnosing student abilities regarding scientific reasoning. Self-efficacy expectations concerning their own abilities to successfully cope with general and experimental diagnostic activities were significantly lower than the attitude towards the importance of diagnostics in teacher training. The results are discussed with regard to practical implications as they imply that scientific reasoning should be promoted in university courses, emphasising the importance of understanding the science-specific procedures (knowing how) and epistemic constructs in scientific reasoning (knowing why). Full article
35 pages, 4033 KiB  
Article
High School Students’ Epistemic Cognition and Argumentation Practices during Small-Group Quality Talk Discussions in Science
by Liwei Wei, Carla M. Firetto, Rebekah F. Duke, Jeffrey A. Greene and P. Karen Murphy
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 616; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100616 - 8 Oct 2021
Cited by 5 | Viewed by 2925
Abstract
For high school students to develop scientific understanding and reasoning, it is essential that they engage in epistemic cognition and scientific argumentation. In the current study, we used the AIR model (i.e., Aims and values, epistemic Ideals, and Reliable processes) to examine high [...] Read more.
For high school students to develop scientific understanding and reasoning, it is essential that they engage in epistemic cognition and scientific argumentation. In the current study, we used the AIR model (i.e., Aims and values, epistemic Ideals, and Reliable processes) to examine high school students’ epistemic cognition and argumentation as evidenced in collaborative discourse in a science classroom. Specifically, we employed a qualitative case study approach to focus on four small-group discussions about scientific phenomena during the Quality Talk Science intervention (QTS), where students regularly received explicit instruction on asking authentic questions and engaging in argumentation. In total, five categories of epistemic ideals and five categories of reliable processes were identified. Students demonstrated more instances of normative epistemic ideals and argumentative responses in the discussions after they received a revised scientific model for discussion and explicit instruction on argumentation. Concomitantly, there were fewer instances of students making decisions based on process of elimination to determine a correct scientific claim. With respect to the relationship of epistemic cognition to authentic questioning and argumentation, the use of epistemic ideals seemed to be associated with the initiation of authentic questions and students’ argumentation appeared to involve the use of epistemic ideals. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

15 pages, 2414 KiB  
Article
A Novel Modelling Process in Chemistry: Merging Biological and Mathematical Perspectives to Develop Modelling Competences
by Vanessa Lang, Christine Eckert, Franziska Perels, Christopher W. M. Kay and Johann Seibert
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 611; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100611 - 3 Oct 2021
Cited by 1 | Viewed by 1973
Abstract
Models are essential in science and therefore in scientific literacy. Therefore, pupils need to attain competency in the appropriate use of models. This so-called model–methodical competence distinguishes between model competence (the conceptual part) and modelling competence (the procedural part), wherefrom a definition follows [...] Read more.
Models are essential in science and therefore in scientific literacy. Therefore, pupils need to attain competency in the appropriate use of models. This so-called model–methodical competence distinguishes between model competence (the conceptual part) and modelling competence (the procedural part), wherefrom a definition follows a general overview of the concept of models in this article. Based on this, modelling processes enable the promotion of the modelling competence. In this context, two established approaches mainly applied in other disciplines (biology and mathematics) and a survey among chemistry teachers and employees of chemistry education departments (N = 98) form the starting point for developing a chemistry modelling process. The article concludes with a description of the developed modelling process, which by its design, provides an opportunity to develop students’ modelling competence. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

19 pages, 2916 KiB  
Article
Elementary Students’ Reasoning in Drawn Explanations Based on a Scientific Theory
by Valeria M. Cabello, Patricia M. Moreira and Paulina Griñó Morales
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(10), 581; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100581 - 26 Sep 2021
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 2455
Abstract
Constructing explanations of scientific phenomena is a high-leverage practice that promotes student understanding. In the context of this study, we acknowledge that children are used to receiving explanations from teachers. However, they are rarely encouraged to construct explanations about the causes and consequences [...] Read more.
Constructing explanations of scientific phenomena is a high-leverage practice that promotes student understanding. In the context of this study, we acknowledge that children are used to receiving explanations from teachers. However, they are rarely encouraged to construct explanations about the causes and consequences of phenomena. We modified a strategy to elicit and analyze primary students’ reasoning based on scientific theory as a methodological advance in learning and cognition. The participants were fourth-graders of middle socioeconomic status in Chile’s geographical zone with high seismic risk. They drew explanations about the causes and consequences of earthquakes during a learning unit of eighteen hours oriented toward explanation-construction based on the Tectonic Plates Theory. A constant comparative method was applied to analyze drawings and characterize students’ reasoning used in pictorial representations, following the first coding step of the qualitative Grounded Theory approach. The results show the students expressed progressive levels of reasoning. However, several participants expressed explanations based on the phenomena causes even at an early stage of formal learning. More sophisticated reasoning regarding the scientific theory underpinning earthquakes was found at the end of the learning unit. We discuss approaching elementary students’ scientific reasoning in explanations based on theory, connected with context-based science education. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

20 pages, 651 KiB  
Article
Preservice Biology Teachers’ Scientific Reasoning Skills and Beliefs about Nature of Science: How Do They Develop and Is There a Mutual Relationship during the Development?
by Daniela Mahler, Denise Bock and Till Bruckermann
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 558; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090558 - 18 Sep 2021
Cited by 7 | Viewed by 3084 | Correction
Abstract
Scientific reasoning (SR) skills and nature of science (NOS) beliefs represent important characteristics of biology teachers’ professional competence. In particular, teacher education at university is formative for the professionalization of future teachers and is thus the focus of the current study. Our study [...] Read more.
Scientific reasoning (SR) skills and nature of science (NOS) beliefs represent important characteristics of biology teachers’ professional competence. In particular, teacher education at university is formative for the professionalization of future teachers and is thus the focus of the current study. Our study aimed to examine the development of SR skills and NOS beliefs and their mutual relationship during teacher education. We applied paper-and-pencil tests to measure SR skills and NOS beliefs of 299 preservice biology teachers from 25 universities in Germany. The results of linear mixed models and planned comparisons revealed that both SR skills and NOS beliefs develop over the course of the study. Nevertheless, the development of SR skills and multiple aspects of NOS beliefs proceeds in different trajectories. Cross-lagged models showed a complex picture concerning the mutual relationship between SR skills and NOS beliefs during their development (both positive and negative). The current study contributes to the existing research because it is based on longitudinal data and allows—in contrast to cross-sectional research—conclusions about the development of SR skills and NOS beliefs. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

19 pages, 922 KiB  
Article
Reasoning on Controversial Science Issues in Science Education and Science Communication
by Anna Beniermann, Laurens Mecklenburg and Annette Upmeier zu Belzen
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 522; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090522 - 8 Sep 2021
Cited by 11 | Viewed by 5396
Abstract
The ability to make evidence-based decisions, and hence to reason on questions concerning scientific and societal aspects, is a crucial goal in science education and science communication. However, science denial poses a constant challenge for society and education. Controversial science issues (CSI) encompass [...] Read more.
The ability to make evidence-based decisions, and hence to reason on questions concerning scientific and societal aspects, is a crucial goal in science education and science communication. However, science denial poses a constant challenge for society and education. Controversial science issues (CSI) encompass scientific knowledge rejected by the public as well as socioscientific issues, i.e., societal issues grounded in science that are frequently applied to science education. Generating evidence-based justifications for claims is central in scientific and informal reasoning. This study aims to describe attitudes and their justifications within the argumentations of a random online sample (N = 398) when reasoning informally on selected CSI. Following a deductive-inductive approach and qualitative content analysis of written open-ended answers, we identified five types of justifications based on a fine-grained category system. The results suggest a topic-specificity of justifications referring to specific scientific data, while justifications appealing to authorities tend to be common across topics. Subjective, and therefore normative, justifications were slightly related to conspiracy ideation and a general rejection of the scientific consensus. The category system could be applied to other CSI topics to help clarify the relation between scientific and informal reasoning in science education and communication. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

23 pages, 1739 KiB  
Article
Measuring and Fostering Preservice Chemistry Teachers’ Scientific Reasoning Competency
by Besim Enes Bicak, Cornelia Eleonore Borchert and Kerstin Höner
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 496; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090496 - 3 Sep 2021
Cited by 11 | Viewed by 3037
Abstract
Developing scientific reasoning (SR) is a central goal of science-teacher education worldwide. On a fine-grained level, SR competency can be subdivided into at least six skills: formulating research questions, generating hypotheses, planning experiments, observing and measuring, preparing data for [...] Read more.
Developing scientific reasoning (SR) is a central goal of science-teacher education worldwide. On a fine-grained level, SR competency can be subdivided into at least six skills: formulating research questions, generating hypotheses, planning experiments, observing and measuring, preparing data for analysis, and drawing conclusions. In a study focusing on preservice chemistry teachers, an organic chemistry lab course was redesigned using problem-solving experiments and SR video lessons to foster SR skills. To evaluate the intervention, a self-assessment questionnaire was developed, and a performance-based instrument involving an experimental problem-solving task was adapted to the target group of undergraduates. The treatment was evaluated in a pre-post design with control group (cook-book experiments, no SR video lessons) and alternative treatment group (problem-solving experiments, unrelated video lessons). Interrater reliability was excellent (ρ from 0.915 to 1.000; ICC (A1)). Data analysis shows that the adapted instrument is suitable for university students. First insights from the pilot study indicate that the cook-book lab (control group) only fosters students’ skill in observing and measuring, while both treatment groups show an increase in generating hypotheses and planning experiments. No pretest-posttest differences were found in self-assessed SR skills in the treatment groups. Instruments and data are presented and discussed. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

11 pages, 2808 KiB  
Article
Modeling as Scientific Reasoning—The Role of Abductive Reasoning for Modeling Competence
by Annette Upmeier zu Belzen, Paul Engelschalt and Dirk Krüger
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 495; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090495 - 3 Sep 2021
Cited by 23 | Viewed by 5423
Abstract
While the hypothetico-deductive approach, which includes inductive and deductive reasoning, is largely recognized in scientific reasoning, there is not much focus on abductive reasoning. Abductive reasoning describes the theory-based attempt of explaining a phenomenon by a cause. By integrating abductive reasoning into a [...] Read more.
While the hypothetico-deductive approach, which includes inductive and deductive reasoning, is largely recognized in scientific reasoning, there is not much focus on abductive reasoning. Abductive reasoning describes the theory-based attempt of explaining a phenomenon by a cause. By integrating abductive reasoning into a framework for modeling competence, we strengthen the idea of modeling being a key practice of science. The framework for modeling competence theoretically describes competence levels structuring the modeling process into model construction and model application. The aim of this theoretical paper is to extend the framework for modeling competence by including abductive reasoning, with impact on the whole modeling process. Abductive reasoning can be understood as knowledge expanding in the process of model construction. In combination with deductive reasoning in model application, such inferences might enrich modeling processes. Abductive reasoning to explain a phenomenon from the best fitting guess is important for model construction and may foster the deduction of hypotheses from the model and further testing them empirically. Recent studies and examples of learners’ performance in modeling processes support abductive reasoning being a part of modeling competence within scientific reasoning. The extended framework can be used for teaching and learning to foster scientific reasoning competences within modeling processes. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

16 pages, 1054 KiB  
Article
Analyzing Cognitive Demands of a Scientific Reasoning Test Using the Linear Logistic Test Model (LLTM)
by Moritz Krell, Samia Khan and Jan van Driel
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 472; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090472 - 27 Aug 2021
Cited by 7 | Viewed by 2354
Abstract
The development and evaluation of valid assessments of scientific reasoning are an integral part of research in science education. In the present study, we used the linear logistic test model (LLTM) to analyze how item features related to text complexity and the presence [...] Read more.
The development and evaluation of valid assessments of scientific reasoning are an integral part of research in science education. In the present study, we used the linear logistic test model (LLTM) to analyze how item features related to text complexity and the presence of visual representations influence the overall item difficulty of an established, multiple-choice, scientific reasoning competencies assessment instrument. This study used data from n = 243 pre-service science teachers from Australia, Canada, and the UK. The findings revealed that text complexity and the presence of visual representations increased item difficulty and, in total, contributed to 32% of the variance in item difficulty. These findings suggest that the multiple-choice items contain the following cognitive demands: encoding, processing, and combining of textually presented information from different parts of the items and encoding, processing, and combining information that is presented in both the text and images. The present study adds to our knowledge of which cognitive demands are imposed upon by multiple-choice assessment instruments and whether these demands are relevant for the construct under investigation—in this case, scientific reasoning competencies. The findings are discussed and related to the relevant science education literature. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

13 pages, 267 KiB  
Article
Individual Differences in Children’s Scientific Reasoning
by Erika Schlatter, Ard W. Lazonder, Inge Molenaar and Noortje Janssen
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(9), 471; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11090471 - 27 Aug 2021
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 2407
Abstract
Scientific reasoning is an important skill that encompasses hypothesizing, experimenting, inferencing, evaluating data and drawing conclusions. Previous research found consistent inter- and intra-individual differences in children’s ability to perform these component skills, which are still largely unaccounted for. This study examined these differences [...] Read more.
Scientific reasoning is an important skill that encompasses hypothesizing, experimenting, inferencing, evaluating data and drawing conclusions. Previous research found consistent inter- and intra-individual differences in children’s ability to perform these component skills, which are still largely unaccounted for. This study examined these differences and the role of three predictors: reading comprehension, numerical ability and problem-solving skills. A sample of 160 upper-primary schoolchildren completed a practical scientific reasoning task that gauged their command of the five component skills and did not require them to read. In addition, children took standardized tests of reading comprehension and numerical ability and completed the Tower of Hanoi task to measure their problem-solving skills. As expected, children differed substantially from one another. Generally, scores were highest for experimenting, lowest for evaluating data and drawing conclusions and intermediate for hypothesizing and inferencing. Reading comprehension was the only predictor that explained individual variation in scientific reasoning as a whole and in all component skills except hypothesizing. These results suggest that researchers and science teachers should take differences between children and across component skills into account. Moreover, even though reading comprehension is considered a robust predictor of scientific reasoning, it does not account for the variation in all component skills. Full article

Review

Jump to: Editorial, Research

19 pages, 360 KiB  
Review
A Model of Scientific Data Reasoning
by Amy M. Masnick and Bradley J. Morris
Educ. Sci. 2022, 12(2), 71; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12020071 - 20 Jan 2022
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 3792
Abstract
Data reasoning is an essential component of scientific reasoning, as a component of evidence evaluation. In this paper, we outline a model of scientific data reasoning that describes how data sensemaking underlies data reasoning. Data sensemaking, a relatively automatic process rooted in perceptual [...] Read more.
Data reasoning is an essential component of scientific reasoning, as a component of evidence evaluation. In this paper, we outline a model of scientific data reasoning that describes how data sensemaking underlies data reasoning. Data sensemaking, a relatively automatic process rooted in perceptual mechanisms that summarize large quantities of information in the environment, begins early in development, and is refined with experience, knowledge, and improved strategy use. Summarizing data highlights set properties such as central tendency and variability, and these properties are used to draw inferences from data. However, both data sensemaking and data reasoning are subject to cognitive biases or heuristics that can lead to flawed conclusions. The tools of scientific reasoning, including external representations, scientific hypothesis testing, and drawing probabilistic conclusions, can help reduce the likelihood of such flaws and help improve data reasoning. Although data sensemaking and data reasoning are not supplanted by scientific data reasoning, scientific reasoning skills can be leveraged to improve learning about science and reasoning with data. Full article
Back to TopTop