Next Article in Journal
A Systematic Review of Positional Plagiocephaly Prevention Methods for Patients in Development
Next Article in Special Issue
Three-Dimensional Dubins-Path-Guided Continuous Curvature Path Smoothing
Previous Article in Journal
Biological Monitoring via Urine Samples to Assess Healthcare Workers’ Exposure to Hazardous Drugs: A Scoping Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Heat Conduction Plate Layout Optimization Using Physics-Driven Convolutional Neural Networks
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Technical Device for Determining the Predispositions of Students—Air Traffic Controllers and Pilots during Multitasking Training

Faculty of Aeronautics, Technical University of Kosice, Rampova 7, 041 21 Kosice, Slovakia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2022, 12(21), 11171; https://doi.org/10.3390/app122111171
Submission received: 11 October 2022 / Revised: 24 October 2022 / Accepted: 2 November 2022 / Published: 4 November 2022

Abstract

:

Featured Application

Authors are encouraged to provide a concise description of the specific application or a potential application of the work. This section is not mandatory.

Abstract

The specific professions of aviation personnel include the professions of the pilot and air traffic controller. These occupations are specific in that while performing their work, they must be able to simultaneously operate the devices in the handling area and in the pedipulation area, supplemented by acoustic sensations in the form of correspondence between flying and ground stations. The performance requirements of pilots and air traffic controllers place high demands on their health, psychological condition, attention, and concentration, due to being in constant pursuit of minimization of erroneous decisions, otherwise defined as the human factor in aviation. This article is focused on the development and testing of a technical device for measuring the relative error rate of students in multitasking tasks in preparation for employment. The main result is a designed technical device with hardware (HW) and software (SW) parts. An experimental method was used to measure the qualitative and quantitative performance indicators of the test subjects. The results of the experiment were observed and evaluated based on the analytical-synthetic method based on critical thinking. By comparing and abstracting the measured data, the reference values of the performance indicators of the tested subjects were determined. The selection of the final sample of subjects consisted of two phases. In the first phase, questionnaires were evaluated, and in the second phase, reaction time measurements during multitasking tasks using technical equipment were evaluated. Based on the measurements, an error ratio was defined, which could be graphically represented. The testing proved the full functionality of the designed technical equipment for these purposes in aviation education.

1. Introduction

At the Department of Flight Training of the Faculty of Aeronautics of the Technical University of Kosice (TUKE), together with representatives of other departments of the Faculty of Aeronautics and institutions outside TUKE, the project “Application of self-regulatory methods in the training of flight crews” was implemented. The project dealt with human performance in air transport in the profession of the pilot and its impact on the safety of air transport. This is a very topical issue, as there is currently a constant increase in requirements for aviation professions, not least for the mental and physical activity of aviation personnel [1]. This implies the need to identify individual factors affecting aircrew, such as fatigue [2], discomfort, maneuvering under certain loads, or flights in adverse weather conditions, which in some way cause stress and thus affect the psychophysiological parameters of aircrew and consequently the quality and safety of flights [3]. Despite statistical evaluations of air transport, as the safest mode of transporting passengers and cargo, current air accidents and the increase in air traffic with their capacity problems are causing significant global interest in aviation safety, including human performance [4,5]. Some authors in their studies are devoted to the forecasting and flow of shipments, evaluating safety in transport. The aim of these studies is to predict the short-term and long-term prediction of traffic flow [6]. Most studies in this area deal with the mental strain of aviation personnel, which, given its disproportionate size, can lead to imperfect perception, insufficient attention, or an insufficient level of information processing. These stressors negatively affect air safety, in which statistical indicators point to the fact that the human factor currently plays a key role in most air accidents [7]. One of the first studies in this area was conducted in 1977 at an airbase in Arizona, in which researchers focused on the origin and effect of stress in pilot training [8]. Their research was based on research studies that showed the effect of stress on changes in the effectiveness of pilot behavior, thus leading to an increased possibility of making wrong decisions. Similar studies have shown the origin and presence of stress when flying, respectively, during simulated flights [9,10]. However, other studies have focused on evaluating the impact of stress and its factors concerning professional activities. Various factors of life have been examined, such as relationships, family problems, lifestyle, etc. [11]. The findings pointed to the need to intensively organize control or stress management programs, which result in the improved operational efficiency of aviation personnel [12]. An interesting study by Y. Lin et al. describes the training of air traffic controllers using computerized pseudo-pilots, including speech and language recognition, thereby trying to eliminate factors affecting flight safety [13]. In a study by Wilson and Fisher, several physiological parameters were measured in pilots, which aimed to define the load in individual flight segments [14]. The aim was to find suitable physiological parameters and their combination for determining the condition (pilot load rate) concerning flight segments [15,16]. Most current studies have used several parameters to assess mental and physical stress, such as heart rate, respiratory rate, eye click, myopotentials, temperature, blood pressure, etc. [17,18]. However, the evaluation of the parameters realized in the previous studies was based on different methodologies of data collection and evaluation using different types of sensors [19]. Topics are found in both military and civilian aviation training; for example, in the work of the Advisory Group for Aerospace Research and Development (AGARD) on assessment of pilot workload [20]. French pilots are trained in modern avionics and learning to scan views into and out of the cockpit, for both their decision-making processes and errors, are under investigation [21]. We can study, from a selected literature review, the Eye-Tracking Measures in Aviation [22] or flight tests for a proof of concept and to compare the reaction times of pilots looking out the cabin and pilots looking at the display [23]. Work on the design of a portable measuring device for measuring the reaction time of the upper limbs and lower limbs to visual stimuli is also inspiring [24,25]; for example, reaction time measurement devices for flight crew testing [26], especially for 24 h flights. We can study the prediction of a pilot’s reaction time based on EEG signals [27] or based on visual perception [28].
In the years 2012 to 2015, the project “Research of pilot training methods using flight simulators”, co-financed by European Union resources within the Operational Program Research and Development, which was assigned the code 26220220161, was successfully implemented at the Faculty of Aeronautics. Within the project was gained new knowledge in the field of the impact of changes in the display of flight and navigation data on pilot performance and air safety. The subject of the research was to compare the performance of pilots in changing the display of information in the cockpit from the classic display to the glass cockpit. Within the research, the performance of the pilots was measured using the accuracy of piloting techniques and load levels. Today, although the issue of load assessment in the aviation professions seems to be largely overhauled, we do not record established methods for assessing physical and mental stress, and especially in real air traffic, there are no methods for eliminating load and increasing performance that would apply to various types of professions providing air transport. The previous scientific work was the motivation for continuing to investigate the issue.
For the needs of the project, it was necessary to create a device, which is presented in this article. The device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks simultaneously evaluates inputs from the manipulation, pedipulation, and acoustic environments in real time. With the device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks, it is possible to determine the critical point of the measured subject’s psychological readiness for the performance of a specific occupation. The newly created device integrates newly created software and available hardware devices that can be used in the conditions of the Slovak Republic.
The specific professions of aviation personnel include the professions of pilot and air traffic controller. These occupations are specific in that, while performing their work, they must be able to simultaneously operate equipment in the handling area and in the pedipulation area, supplemented by acoustic sensations in the form of correspondence between flying and ground stations. The performance requirements of pilots and air traffic controllers place high demands on their health, psychological state, attention, concentration, due to being in constant pursuit of minimization of erroneous decisions. When measuring the reaction time for multitasking tasks, which are the output of the designed device, it is possible to monitor the performance of the individual and the deviations in the monitored indicators from the reference values during repeated tests. Such monitoring of performance parameters could help practical training of instructors, such as through individual counseling, thus enabling an individual approach in the theoretical as well as practical training of pilots and air traffic controllers.
This facility is unique in the territory of the Slovak Republic. There are similar devices for measuring reaction time in the world, but they are not made specifically for the needs of monitoring the correctness and speed of reaction during the performance of the profession of pilots and air traffic controllers. The device designed by us, unlike other devices, monitors the speed and correctness of the response in the manipulation space and pedipulation space simultaneously, with acoustic sensations. This covers the entire spectrum of activities of the professions of pilots and air traffic controllers.
The device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks used in this work is the subject of the Industrial Property Office of the Slovak Republic as a utility model SK 8986 Y1 and is the subject of a patent application PP 50007-2020. The main goal of the paper was to design and test a technical device for measuring the relative error rate of students—air traffic controllers and pilots—during multitasking training in aviation education.
We studied two research questions:
Research question 1: Is it possible to produce a technical device for measuring the relative error rate of the test subject using the measurement of reaction time when solving multitasking tasks?
Research question 2: Will the testing of the technical device demonstrate the ability to compare the relative error rate of the tested subjects?
The main result is a designed technical device enabling the measurement of the relative error rate of the tested persons when solving multitasking tasks, which is made up of the relevant HW and SW parts. This paper emphasizes an innovative approach to testing and evaluating student performance in preparation for the aviation profession. The technical solution is protected by copyright on the registered utility model, logged at the Industrial Property Office (the Section 6). Experimental testing of the technical device under the conditions provides at the Faculty of Aeronautics demonstrated the full functionality of the designed device, with use in aviation education.
The paper is structured as follows: In the next part, the HW and SW of the technical device are presented. The third part presents the results of the validation experiment, which was primarily focused on testing the device. Finally, first experiences with the performance of the proposed technical device are discussed, focusing on the new perspectives provided by this scientific work.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Methods and Methodology

For the processing of the article and the processing of the issue, an analysis of available studies and scientific works was carried out using the analytical-synthetic method based on critical thinking. During the selection of a sample of students, a questionnaire method was used to determine the entry requirements for conducting the experiment. In the experimental phase, a verification device was used to determine the predisposition of aviation personnel based on the measurement of reaction time in multitasking tasks. When evaluating the qualitative and quantitative indicators of the measured data, the methods of observation, induction, deduction, comparison, abstraction, and generalization were used. The detailed procedure of the experimental testing is shown in Figure 1.
The device, which was used to measure the reaction time during multitasking tasks, was developed at the Faculty of Aeronautics, TUKE, and is the subject of the utility model PUV 50011-2020, which was published in the Gazette and Registered at the Industrial Property Office of the Slovak Republic. The device can process acoustic commands and tasks from the handling and pedipulation area in real time. The device was developed to determine the predisposition of individuals to perform the profession of the pilot and air traffic controller. The verification device monitors the speed of the test sample, the correct motor function, and the response to visual and auditory stimuli.

2.2. Testing Device Hardware

The device used to test the chosen subjects is shown in Figure 2. The device itself is durable and easily transportable. The device is made of durable materials, such as aluminum and hardened plastic, which ensures the protection of the delicate electronics inside the device. The device in its disassembled state is placed in a transport case, which ensures easy handling and transfer of the device. The dimensions for inclusion in transport condition (Figure 3) are 35 × 25 × 17 cm. It consists of four main parts: the main module, with the integrated microcontroller (MCU) module and buttons placed in front of the tested subject, and from the foot pedals, a module that connects to the main module, as well as one from the headphones. The main module is connected to the USB port of a PC or a laptop that has the software application running and from this port it is also powered. The USB connection provides a virtual comport connection with a baud rate of 921,600 baud/s, 8-bit word, and one stop bit without parity.
The MCU uses an embedded module with NXP LPC1768 processor that is based on the ARM Cortex-M3 architecture and is running at 96 MHz. It has 32 KB RAM and 512 KB FLASH memories, which are sufficient for the tasks programmed in the C programming language that realize within the testing system. All the buttons, both the ones for visual (3 buttons) and for audio (10 buttons) tasks, and both pedals are connected to the MCU with pull-up resistors to define the initial state and suppress the possible interference from ambient electromagnetic fields.
There are no timers used within the MCU; it simply reacts as fast as possible to in-puts and sends them to the PC or laptop; however, the button groups are logically handled as buses, to avoid accepting the situation in which more buttons from the same group are simultaneously pressed. Each button has its corresponding ASCII character from “a” to “o”, which is sent if it is pressed, and to avoid sending multiple characters, the button needs to be released and again pressed to send another character; thus, the buttons are evaluated after a state change is detected. The safety time margin, when the buttons are non-responsive while the task changes from one type to another, was set to 0.2 s. When testing the device, it was found that the minimum response speed was measured at 0.3 s, and the device testing also showed that the buttons oscillated for up to 0.1 s, causing errors in the measurements. For this reason, the software set a minimum task time of 0.2 s, and the problems with the oscillation of the probes were thereby eliminated.
The main result of the work is a technical device for measuring the relative error rate of students—air traffic controllers and pilots—during multitasking training, which consists of HW and SW parts. The basis of the measuring device is a suitably located input–output unit, which is connected in connection with the handling control unit. The manipulation control unit also includes buttons L, C, and R, which are used to enter tasks from movements entered in the manipulation area. Other signals entering the manipulation control unit are imported from the numeric keypad, containing buttons 0–9, which are used to enter the tasks of the acoustic commands. The tasks resulting from the pedipulation area enter the control unit from the left pedipulation unit and the right pedipulation unit. The verification device was designed to measure the reaction time in response to a randomly generated task. The technical solution of the device is shown on Figure 4.

2.3. Testing Device Software

The application software for the testing device was written in the C++ programming language in the QtCreator IDE with help of the QtFramework libraries. The main window of the application is shown in Figure 5. The window is divided into three main regions, excluding the region for the control buttons information text region and three graphic regions.
The settings dialog window is shown in Figure 6. Parameters that can be set include the working directory, in which the output files are created, and the base name for the created files. Considering the testing process, the number of testing cycles can be set, and since the number of tasks in one cycle is 6, this setting can be adjusted only as multiples of 6. The last adjustable setting is the COM port name to properly choose the connected testing device MCU.
There are 3 timers/counters used in the application: the first measures the time to complete the task, the second ensures moving to next task after 20 s, and the third is responsible to play a chosen audio sound repetitively, each being 2 s, up to 9 times in one testing cycle.
The three graphic regions serve to draw small squares while the red square, if it is drawn in a region, has a special meaning (see the Results section). The tasks generated for testing one subject are created randomly within the sets of 6 into a complete sequence of tasks in an array. Other sub-arrays used in the application include the arrays filled during this process with randomly generated colors (yellow, green, blue) and with identification of one from the three sectors of each graphic region randomly chosen for the approaches if needed. All the randomization processes are seeded with an unsigned int value of the current time and is handled at the start of the testing, so the generation process does not interfere with the testing process.
The example output file of the testing is shown in Figure 7. As it can be seen, it is a formatted text file that has chosen statistical parameters in the head of the file at lines beginning with the “#” character. The statistical parameters include the complete time taken to finish the testing, average time for one task, total number of tasks, total number of errors, and the error ratio parameter. The last six lines indicate the values to create a histogram, with task type number on the x-axis and the errors corresponding to each task type on the y-axis.
After the head there are four columns, which form from left to right they are the number of the task within the testing, the task type number, time in seconds, and the number of errors for the given task.

3. Results and Experimental Testing of Device

3.1. Testing Process

The device generates six random tasks, where the ratio of the individual types of tasks is determined as follows:
  • Tasks 1–3 are focused on visual perceptions and stimuli to which the test subject responds in the manipulation area. The main goal of these tasks is to reveal the predisposition of the tested subject to work in a specific profession and to determine the speed and correctness of the reactions to visual perceptions and stimuli at the standard load of reactions in the handling area. The secondary goal of these tasks is to determine the ability of the test subject to respond to visual stimuli with different wavelengths; i.e., whether the test subject has corrected color vision.
  • Tasks 4–5 are focused on visual perceptions and stimuli to which the test subject responds in the pedipulation area. The main goal of these tasks is to reveal the predisposition of the tested subject to work in a specific profession and to determine the speed and correctness of responses to visual perceptions and stimuli at the standard load of reactions in the pedipulation area. The secondary objective of this task is to determine the ability of the test subject to engage opposite hemispheres of the brain.
  • Task 6 is focused on acoustic perceptions and stimuli to which the test subject responds in the manipulation area. The main goal of this task is to reveal the predispositions of the tested subject to work in a specific profession and to determine the speed and correctness of reactions to acoustic perceptions and stimuli of reactions in the handling area. The secondary objective of this task is to determine the ability of the test subject to respond to multiple types of tasks simultaneously.
Another research part of the project aimed at testing the technical equipment took place in two stages. When creating the test sample, it was necessary to ensure the representation of women. All volunteers interested in participating in the project had to fill out a questionnaire (identification data, age, gender, experience with aircraft piloting—real and simulator). The initial requirement for inclusion in the test sample was zero experience with piloting the aircraft. In the next part of the selection, those interested in participating in the project passed a stress test and tested their performance using a device for measuring reaction time. This selection aimed to exclude from the experiment those subjects who exhibit higher emotional lability, low ability to concentrate, and reduced ability to perform several activities simultaneously under increased load, which should ensure greater uniformity of the test sample and increase the objectivity of experimental results. From various entrance tests, which help to determine the health and mental fitness of an individual to perform a given profession, we focused on monitoring and measuring the reaction time of selected subjects within the entrance tests. Current devices used to measure reaction time are widespread throughout the world, especially in the military, in the departments of general psychology and sports. Within the reaction time measuring device, simple tasks for measuring the reaction time can be used, for example, “Go/No Go” tasks to explore higher brain centers, or more complex tasks to measure critical response time to explore cognitive processing skills. Reaction time is genetically the most conditioned, which means that it can be least developed through training. Reaction time belongs to the structure of the motor skills. We can divide it into simple, when an individual responds to one stimulus, or complex, when there can be several stimuli to respond to. The reaction time consists of several characteristics. The most important characteristic is its level represented by the response time itself. The higher the reaction time, the shorter the response time.
After starting the test, three sectors with a size of 300 × 600 pixels are displayed on the display unit. In each of the sectors, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) with a size of 10 × 10 pixels are displayed. A pair of dots of the same color are 150 pixels apart. With the start of each new six tasks, the system automatically triggers a beep at regular 2-second intervals. The number of beeps must be counted in parallel in addition to solving other tasks. After entering the correct number of beeps, the system considers the beep cycle complete and begins generating beeps again as the six new tasks begin. The display unit of the device for measuring the reaction time is shown on Figure 8.
The verification device system will generate a new task only after entering the correct answer. The test subject thus has real-time feedback. If the correct answer is not entered within 20 s, the system generates another answer and writes the letter N (unfulfilled task) to the file instead of the time.
Within one testing, the system generates 50 six-task errands, and the tested subject must correctly answer the 300 individual tasks focused on visual and acoustic perceptions. The output of testing is a data file containing the following:
  • Total time to complete 300 tasks.
  • Average time per task.
  • Total number of errors.
  • Frequency of errors in Tasks 1–6.
Task 1—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, in Sector 1, one pair of points of the same color has a distance reduced to 30 pixels (two points of the same color converge). The correct answer for Task 1 is to press the L button in the manipulation area.
Task 2—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, in Sector 2, one pair of dots of the same color has a distance reduced to 30 pixels (two dots of the same color converge). The correct answer for Task 2 is to press the C key in the manipulation area.
Task 3—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, in Sector 3, one pair of points of the same color has a distance reduced to 30 pixels (two points of the same color converge). The correct answer for Task 3 is to press the R button in the manipulation area.
Task 4—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, one red dot appears in Sector 1. The correct answer for Task 4 is to press the right foot in the pedipulation area.
Task 5—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, one red dot appears in Sector 3. The correct answer for Task 5 is to press the left foot in the pedipulation area.
Task 6—in Sector 1, Sector 2, and Sector 3, three pairs of dots (yellow, green, blue) 150 pixels apart are displayed. However, one red dot appears in Sector 2. The correct answer for Task 6 is to press the number 0–9 in the manipulation area, which belongs to the number of beeps. The maximum number of beeps in the software is limited to 9. Examples of tasks are shown on Figure 9.

3.2. Evaluation of the Second Phase of Testing

Sixty-seven subjects participated in the testing. During testing, the total time required to perform a set of 300 randomly generated tasks and the number of incorrect answers during testing were monitored. The limit for inclusion in the project was set for a total time equal to a maximum of 600 s, which is a limit of 2 s for one task and the maximum number of errors was set at 50. These values were proposed based on expert estimation and practical experience in the trial testing process and can inform further research. The time limit out of the 67 subjects tested was met by 56 subjects. A total of 42 subjects met the limit for the maximum number of errors. A total of 38 subjects met both conditions. The measurement results are shown in the Table 1.

3.3. Selection of the Final Sample for the Project

During controlled flights on the aircraft-to-ground radio connection. Pilots are controlled using the “Hearback/Readback” system. This means that each instruction from the air traffic controller must be repeated correctly. Only after the correct repetition of the instruction can they start to follow the instruction. In case of an incorrect command from the air traffic controller, or incorrect repetition of the instruction by the pilot, the instruction “Correction” follows and the whole instruction is sent anew both by the air traffic controller and the pilot. Therefore, in the error rate, the weight of one incorrect answer is expressed as four times the average time per roll. The error ratio, therefore, has the following final form:
E R = A T · ( N T + 4 · N E )
where:
  • ER = error ratio;
  • AT = average time for one task in seconds;
  • NT = number of tasks;
  • NE = number of errors.
For the limit values of the total time (600 s) and the total number of errors (50), the limit value of the error ratio would be ER = 2.(300 + 4.50) = 1000. A total of 43 subjects met this filter. Based on the error rate, a ranking of the best entities that could be worked on in the project was compiled. As the number of reported students far outweighed the need for a test sample of the project, we were able to tighten the error rate criteria during the measurement of reaction time by 20% to the level of 800 points, from which we received a final sample of 30 subjects + one substitute. The final ranking is shown in the Table 2.

4. Discussion

In evaluating the data, emphasis was placed on the total time required to perform tests on the device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks, on the total number of errors during testing in individual subjects and on the number of errors in individual types of tasks.
Time to complete the test is shown on Figure 10 and Histogram of time to complete the test is shown on Figure 11.
From the graphs showing the total time required to complete the testing, it can be observed that most of the tested subjects were able to perform the tasks in the time limit of 440 s to 600 s, i.e., 48 subjects, which is approximately 71.5% tested; 10 subjects were not able to meet the time limit, and 9 subjects passed the test under 440 s.
Cumulative number of errors is shown on Figure 12 and histogram of the cumulative number of errors during the test is shown on Figure 13.
From the graphs of the total number of errors during the test, each test subject made at least one error. There was no case without error. Less than 45 errors were made by 40 subjects, which is approximately 59.7% of the number of subjects tested. When comparing the times and number of errors in subjects with the longest test time, a correlation is not clear; Subjects 7, 41, 45, 46, 54, 66, and 67 needed the longest test times, but a significant correlation was observed only for Subjects 54 and 67.
Subjects with the lowest number of errors (4, 5, 10, 18, 40, and 49) reached times of less than 500 s, except for Subject 49, who reached a time of approximately 578 s, but this subject made the fewest errors (4) of all subjects. Looking at the overall test group, it can be observed that most of the tested subjects fall into two groups—fast-reacting, but with a higher error rate, and slower reacting, with a lower error rate.
Figure 14 shows the number of errors based on the type of tasks – per subject. For better readability of picture, different types of tasks are marked with different colors. The Figure 15 shows percentage fraction of errors based on type of tasks – all subjects together.
From the graph (graphs) of the frequency of errors for individual tasks, the biggest problem was the test subjects with a task focused on parallelism; i.e., determining the correct number of sound stimuli when up to more than 58% of errors were in this task. This is one of the most important tasks in testing because even during real air traffic, this task is performed by the pilot in parallel. During the flight activity itself (monitoring of altitude, course, speed, engine speed), a pilot must watch in parallel (non-stop) the appropriate radio frequency, follow the instructions of air traffic control, and those of the instructor. From the visualization of the number of errors in the type of tasks “per subject”, specific cases can be observed where the error rate for the “audio” parallel task was minimal, but the error rate in other tasks was increased. The same phenomenon can be observed in these test subjects as in the group that made quite a few mistakes in the “audio” task, thus showing weak multitasking skill. Subjects who made mistakes in a task of any type cannot respond properly to multiple stimuli at the same time, as they have trouble engaging both hemispheres of the brain, which can have fatal consequences in real air traffic.
As described in Section 3.3., during controlled flights on the aircraft–ground radio link, the pilots are controlled using the “Hearback/Readback” system. This means that every instruction from the air traffic controller must be repeated correctly. Only after correct repetition of the instruction can they begin to follow the instruction. In the event of an incorrect command from the air traffic controller, or an incorrect repetition of the instruction by the pilot, the instruction “Correction” follows, and the entire instruction is sent anew to both the air traffic controller and the pilot. Therefore, in the error rate, the weight of one wrong answer is expressed as four times the average time per hour, which has a fundamental impact on the final shape of the relative error rate. By calculating the relative error rate of the tested persons when solving multitasking tasks in training, the result of our scientific work differs from the results of the works discussed in the literature search, primarily those of [25,26,27,28].

5. Conclusions

The purpose of the article was to present a device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks and to verify the functionality of its connection. The device, which was used to measure reaction time during multitasking tasks, was developed at the TUKE Aviation Faculty and is the subject of utility model PUV 50011-2020, which was published in the Gazette and Register of the Slovak Industrial Property Office, which ensures its uniqueness in the conditions of the Slovak Republic. The device designed by us monitors the speed and correctness of the response in the manipulation space and pedipulation space simultaneously, with acoustic sensations. This covers the entire spectrum of activities of the professions of pilot and air traffic controller.
The methodology of the experimental research was based on the implementation of testing in two phases. In the first phase, a questionnaire method was used to eliminate students who did not meet the conditions of the experimental research. In the second phase, testing took place on the verification device to determine the predispositions of the adepts to perform the aviation profession.
During testing, the total time required to perform a set of 300 randomly generated tasks and the number of incorrect answers during testing were monitored. The limit for inclusion in the project was set for a total time equal to a maximum of 600 s, which is a limit of 2 s for 1 task and the maximum number of errors was set at 50. The time limit out of the 67 tested subjects was met by 56 subjects. A total of 42 subjects met the limit for the maximum number of errors. A total of 38 subjects met both conditions.
After evaluating the questionnaires, and measuring the reaction time for multitasking tasks, a final test sample was determined, which participated in the project activities. In the aviation profession, the right decision is more important than the speed of the decision. During testing on the verification device, more emphasis was placed on the correctness of the response than on the speed of the response.
The so-called error ratio was determined for the objective assessment of the current mental preparedness. The error rate expresses the dependence of incorrect answers, reaction time, and the total number of tasks. In the error rate, the weight of one incorrect answer is expressed as four times the average time per task. Based on the error rate, a ranking of the best subjects was compiled. Based on the ranking, 31 entities were involved in the project activities, with which it was possible to work in the project.
The creation of the device was based on commonly used materials and equipment in aircraft cockpits and at air traffic control stations, such as display units in the form of a monitor, buttons on a computer keyboard, control elements of the aircraft cabin in the form of buttons, and pedal systems commonly used in aircraft cabins and at air traffic control stations. To make the experiment as reliable as possible, the device simulates the actions of pilots and air traffic controllers in real operation.
By measuring the reaction time of the monitored sample, the correctness of the connection and operation of the device was confirmed. The device for measuring the reaction time in multitasking tasks, which is used to select the final sample of adepts, can be used to stabilize methods for assessing physical and mental stress in real and simulated air traffic to eliminate the load and increase performance. The results of the measurements may apply to different types of professions providing air transport.

6. Patents

The device for measuring reaction time in multitasking tasks used in this work is registered at the Industrial Property Office of the Slovak Republic as a utility model, SK 8986 Y1, and is the subject of a patent application, PP 50007-2020.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.A.; methodology, M.A. and P.L.; software, M.A. and P.L.; validation, P.L.; formal analysis, M.A. and P.L.; investigation, M.A. and P.L.; resources, M.A.; data curation, M.A.; writing—original draft preparation, M.A. and P.L.; writing—review and editing, M.A. and P.L.; supervision, M.A.; project administration, M.A.; funding acquisition, M.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The APC was funded by the Cultural and Educational Grant Agency of the Ministry of Education, Science, Research and Sports of the Slovak Republic.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Cultural and Educational Grant Agency of the Ministry of Education, Science, Research and Sports of the Slovak Republic, No. 051-TUKE-4/2021-Integrated laboratory for digital aviation education in selected subjects of flight training.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bonner, M.A.; Wilson, G.F. Heart Rate Measures of Flight Test and Evaluation. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 2002, 12, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Brookhuis, K.; De Waard, D. Monitoring drivers’ mental workload in driving simulators using physiological measures. Accid. Anal. 2010, 42, 898–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hanakova, L.; Socha, V.; Socha, L.; Szabo, S.; Kozuba, J.; Lališ, A.; Vittek, P.; Kraus, J.; Rozenberg, R.; Kaľavský, P.; et al. Determining importance of physiological parameters and methods of their evaluation for classification of pilots psychophysiological condition. In Proceedings of the 2017 International Conference on Military Technologies (ICMT), Brno, Czech Republic, 31 May–2 June 2017. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hewson, D.J.; McNair, P.J.; Marshall, R.N. Aircraft control forces and EMG activity: Comparison of novice and experienced pilots during simulated rolls. loops and turns. Aviat. Area Environ. Med. 2000, 71, 798–805. [Google Scholar]
  5. Hewson, D.J.; McNair, P.J.; Marshall, R.N. Aircraft control forces and EMG activity in a C-130 Hercules during strength-critical maneuvers. Aviat. Area Environ. Med. 2001, 72, 165–169. [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen, X.; Wu, S.; Shi, C.; Huang, Y.; Yang, Y.; Ke, R.; Zhao, J. Sensing Data Supported Traffic Flow Prediction via Denoising Schemes and ANN: A Comparison. IEEE Sens. J. 2020, 20, 14317–14328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kaľavský, P.; Rozenberg, R.; Mikula, B.; Zgodavová, Z. Pilots’ performance in changing from analogue to glass cockpits. In Proceedings of the 22nd International Scientific Conference, Transport Means 2018. Trakai, Lithuania, 3–5 October 2018; Kaunas University of Technology: Kaunas, Lithuania, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  8. Leo Jeeva, S.P.; Chandramohan, V. Stressors and stress coping strategies among Civil pilots: A pilot study. Indian J. Aeroarea Med. 2008, 52, 60–64. [Google Scholar]
  9. Kaľavský, P.; Kimličková, M.; Socha, L.; Socha, V. Research on pilot training methods using flight simulators—Analysis of simulator training procedures for pilots. Acta Avion. 2014, 16, 37–40. [Google Scholar]
  10. Kaľavský, P.; Socha, L.; Socha, V. Measuring pilot performance under real flight conditions. In Proceedings of the Nové Trendy v Civilním Letectví 2014:Sborník Příspěvků z Mezinárodní Conference, Herbertov, Czech Republic, 18–20 May 2014. [Google Scholar]
  11. Kelemen, M.; Antoško, M.; Szabo, S.; Socha, L.; Jevčák, J.; Choma, L.; Tobisová, A. Experimental verification of psychophysiological performance of a selected flight personnel and SW: Presurvey for transport safety. Int. Sci. J. 2019, 14, 145–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Krahenbuhl, G.; Marett., J.R.; King., N.W. Stress and Performance in T-37 Pilot Training; Defense Technical Information Center: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1977; ISBN A041734.
  13. Lin, Y.; Wu, Y.; Guo, D.; Zhang, P.; Yin, C.; Yang, B.; Zhang, J. A Deep Learning Framework of Autonomous Pilot Agent for Air Traffic Controller Training. IEEE Trans. Hum. Mach. Syst. 2021, 51, 442–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Veltman, J.A.; Gaillard, A.W. Physiological indices of workload in a simulated flight task. Biol. Psychol. 1996, 42, 323–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Némethové, H.; Neétrék, D.; Szabo, P.; Bal, F.; Kal’avské, P.; Antoéko, M. The identification of scientific issue and the circadian rhythm research concept for ATCO work performance. In Proceedings of the 2020 New Trends in Aviation Development (NTAD), Starý Smokovec, Slovakia, 17–18 September 2020. [Google Scholar]
  16. Patel, M.; Lal, S.K.L.; Kavanagh, D.; Rossiter, P. Applying neural network analysis on heart rate variability data to assess driver fatigue. Expert Syst. Appl. 2011, 38, 7235–7242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kušnírek, S.; Socha, V.; Hanáková, L.; Lališ, A.; Socha, L.; Antoško, M.; Kozuba, J. Reaction time measurement device for flight crew testing. In New Trends in Civil Aviation; CRC Press: London, UK, 2018; pp. 21–24. [Google Scholar]
  18. Socha, L.; Kaľavský, P.; Socha, V.; Gazda, J. Pilot performance and changes in the flight data presentation. Acta Avion. 2014, 16, 53–56. [Google Scholar]
  19. Socha, V.; Schlenker, J.; Kaľavský, P.; Kutílek, P.; Socha, L.; Szabo, S.; Smrčka, P. Effect of the change of flight, navigation and motor data visualization on psychophysiological state of pilots. In Proceedings of the SAMI 2015: IEEE 13th International Symposium on Applied Machine Intelligence and Informatics, Herľany, Slovakia, 22–24 January 2015. [Google Scholar]
  20. NATO AGARD. Assessing Pilot Workload; AGARDograph No. 233; Hardford House: London, UK; pp. 1–190.
  21. Dubois, E.; Blättler, C.; Camachon, C.; Hurter, C. Eye Movements Data Processing for Ab Initio Military Pilot Training. In Intelligent Decision Technologies; IDT 2017. Smart Innovation Systems and Technologies; Neves-Silva, R., Jain, L., Howlett., R., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; Volume 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Peißl, S.; Wickens, C.D.; Baruah, R. Eye-Tracking Measures in Aviation: A Selective Literature Review. Int. J. Aerosp. Psychol. 2018, 28, 98–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Jennings, C.W.; Powell, J.D. Flight test measurement of pilot reaction times to runway incursions: Out-the-window vs. synthetic vision. In Proceedings of the 21st Digital Avionics Systems Conference, Irvine, CA, USA, 27–31 October 2002. [Google Scholar]
  24. Šimerka, J. Designe of the Reaction Time Measurement Device. Prague: CVUT. FD. 2020. Available online: https://dspace.cvut.cz/handle/10467/90476 (accessed on 1 August 2022).
  25. Socha, V.; Hanáková, L.; Stojić, S.; Kušmírek, S.; Socha, L.; Antoško, M. A fatigue influence on pilot’s reaction ability during 24 hours flight simulation: A case series study. In Proceedings of the 19th International Conference on New Trends in Civil Aviation 2017 (NTCA 2017), Prague, Czech Republic, 7–8 December 2017. [Google Scholar]
  26. Socha, V.; Kušmírek, S.; Hanáková, L.; Olexa, P.; Šimerka, J. Portable Reaction Time Measurement Device: Prototype Proposal and Validation. In Proceedings of the 2020 New Trends in Civil Aviation (NTCA), Prague, Czech Republic, 23–24 November 2020. [Google Scholar]
  27. Binias, B.; Myszor, D.; Palus, H.; Cyran, K.A. Prediction of Pilot’s Reaction Time Based on EEG Signals. Front. Neuroinform. 2020, 14, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Abbasi-Kesbi, R.; Memarzadeh-Tehran, H.; Deen, M.J. Technique to estimate human reaction time based on visual perception. Healthc. Technol. Lett. 2017, 4, 73–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Algorithm of the testing process.
Figure 1. Algorithm of the testing process.
Applsci 12 11171 g001
Figure 2. Photo of the testing device.
Figure 2. Photo of the testing device.
Applsci 12 11171 g002
Figure 3. The testing device in transport conditions.
Figure 3. The testing device in transport conditions.
Applsci 12 11171 g003
Figure 4. The technical solution of the device for measuring the reaction time and the relative error rate in multitasking tasks.
Figure 4. The technical solution of the device for measuring the reaction time and the relative error rate in multitasking tasks.
Applsci 12 11171 g004
Figure 5. Main window of the application of the testing device.
Figure 5. Main window of the application of the testing device.
Applsci 12 11171 g005
Figure 6. Settings window of the application for the testing device.
Figure 6. Settings window of the application for the testing device.
Applsci 12 11171 g006
Figure 7. Output file example.
Figure 7. Output file example.
Applsci 12 11171 g007
Figure 8. The display unit of the device for measuring the reaction time and the relative error rate in multitasking tasks.
Figure 8. The display unit of the device for measuring the reaction time and the relative error rate in multitasking tasks.
Applsci 12 11171 g008
Figure 9. Kind of tasks during testing.
Figure 9. Kind of tasks during testing.
Applsci 12 11171 g009
Figure 10. Time to complete the test.
Figure 10. Time to complete the test.
Applsci 12 11171 g010
Figure 11. Histogram of time to complete the test.
Figure 11. Histogram of time to complete the test.
Applsci 12 11171 g011
Figure 12. Cumulative number of errors during the test.
Figure 12. Cumulative number of errors during the test.
Applsci 12 11171 g012
Figure 13. Histogram of the cumulative number of errors during the test.
Figure 13. Histogram of the cumulative number of errors during the test.
Applsci 12 11171 g013
Figure 14. Number of errors based on the type of tasks—per subject.
Figure 14. Number of errors based on the type of tasks—per subject.
Applsci 12 11171 g014
Figure 15. Percentage fraction of errors based on the type of tasks—all subject together.
Figure 15. Percentage fraction of errors based on the type of tasks—all subject together.
Applsci 12 11171 g015
Table 1. Reaction time measurement results.
Table 1. Reaction time measurement results.
Id. NumberTotal Time (s)Number of ErrorsId. NumberTotal Time (s)Number of Errors
1519.9158135318.02316
2487.0753436429.54729
3547.5729537452.07119
4490.9381138555.68190
5487.3721439543.03233
6577.6243740425.29612
7762.9463941814.40924
8507.4436842595.353130
9420.8781943530.98481
10511.2771044412.19631
11562.8734045782.84217
12431.9823446711.697146
13514.5998047480.36541
14590.0166148543.56160
15598.24314449577.8454
16556.697750451.34449
17603.6766351613.20933
18487.6031052587.75645
19472.7048953569.49424
20418.2091754730.159215
21556.697755546.57317
22414.9432456559.43334
23564.3634557635.107172
24559.497358630.072104
25555.521759545.311119
26576.9671860433.76317
27501.1582261536.57134
28590.1324962470.84923
29489.2053863431.99842
30477.6072664579.46482
31488.8332265464.72845
32517.2615466698.72679
33592.022967890.012133
34571.14858
Table 2. The final sample of subjects.
Table 2. The final sample of subjects.
Id. NumberTotal Time
(s)
Total ErrorsError Ratio
(Count)
Id. NumberTotal Time
(s)
Total ErrorsError Ratio
(Count)
35318.02316385.86811562.87340863.072
40425.29612493.34451613.20933883.021
20418.20917513.00332517.26154889.689
9420.87819527.523564.36345902.981
60433.76317532.08352587.75645940.409
22414.94324547.72445782.84217960.286
18487.60310552.6178507.44368967.525
4490.93811562.94328590.13249975.686
37452.07119566.59648543.56160978.41
5487.37214578.34934571.148581012.84
10511.27710579.44719472.704891033.65
44412.19631582.5713514.599801063.51
36429.54729595.63814590.016611069.9
49577.8454608.66341814.409241075.02
62470.84923615.2421519.915811081.42
12431.98234627.81424559.49731104.06
31488.83322632.22443530.984811104.45
30477.60726643.17717603.676631110.76
27501.15822648.16416556.69771128.22
55546.57317670.46321556.69771128.22
63431.99842673.9177762.946391159.68
25555.5217681.43764579.464821213.01
2487.07534707.88338555.681901222.5
26576.96718715.4393547.572951241.16
29489.20538737.06959545.3111191410.54
47480.36541742.96566698.726791434.72
65464.72845743.56558630.0721041503.77
50451.34449746.22342595.3531301627.3
53569.49424751.73315598.2431441746.87
61536.57134779.81657635.1071722091.62
39543.03233781.96646711.6971462097.13
56559.43334813.04267890.0121332468.3
33592.0229820.93454730.1592152823.28
6577.62437862.585
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Antosko, M.; Lipovsky, P. A Technical Device for Determining the Predispositions of Students—Air Traffic Controllers and Pilots during Multitasking Training. Appl. Sci. 2022, 12, 11171. https://doi.org/10.3390/app122111171

AMA Style

Antosko M, Lipovsky P. A Technical Device for Determining the Predispositions of Students—Air Traffic Controllers and Pilots during Multitasking Training. Applied Sciences. 2022; 12(21):11171. https://doi.org/10.3390/app122111171

Chicago/Turabian Style

Antosko, Matej, and Pavol Lipovsky. 2022. "A Technical Device for Determining the Predispositions of Students—Air Traffic Controllers and Pilots during Multitasking Training" Applied Sciences 12, no. 21: 11171. https://doi.org/10.3390/app122111171

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop