Exploring the Relationship Between Preference and Production as Indicators of L2 Sociophonetic Competence
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Research Background
2.1. L2 Sociophonetic Competence
2.1.1. Production
2.1.2. Perception
2.1.3. Perception–Production Link
2.2. Spanish Intervocalic /d/ Deletion
2.2.1. L1 Spanish
2.2.2. L2 Spanish
3. Present Study
- With what frequency do (a subset of) the L2 learners of Spanish from Solon and Kanwit (2022) produce a deleted intervocalic /d/?
- What factors constrain production of the deleted variant for these learners?
- To what extent are differences observed (in frequency and constraining factors) as proficiency increases?
- What is the relationship between preference for and production of deleted intervocalic /d/ across individual learners and as proficiency increases?
3.1. Materials and Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Tasks
3.1.3. Coding
3.1.4. Analysis
4. Results
4.1. Production
4.2. Preference–Production
5. Discussion
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- (1)
- You are in an airport waiting for a plane. A person sits beside you and begins to speak with you. (S)he is from the Dominican Republic and doesn’t know much about university life in the United States. You tell him/her about your daily life and your impressions of university life according to your current experience.
- Describe your daily life or what you do routinely.
- What do you do on weekends?
- What do you think of the university? What do you like? What don’t you like?
- What do professors expect from students?
- What do students expect from professors?
- In general, what worries do university students have?
- What doubts do they have with respect to university life?
- How do you feel at the beginning of a semester?
- How do you feel at the end of a semester?
1 | Although months abroad was not a significant predictor of deletion for our 17 non-categorical learners in the mixed-effects regression (p = 0.065 when considered with the other predictors of the otherwise best-fit model), we will see that it did reveal a significant correlation with deletion in our Pearson test of all 21 learners in Section 4.2. (The correlation was also significant if the sample was limited to the subset of 17 learners.) That time abroad approached but did not achieve significance in the regression likely relates to a combination of causes, including the relatively small number of participants, the high level of individual variability and the role of the individual as a random effect, and the greater predictiveness of EIT score, which, as we will see in Section 4.2, correlated with time abroad. Recall, nevertheless, that time abroad and EIT score did not significantly interact in the regression, as no interactions were significant. |
2 | As previously mentioned, although we collected data on learners’ study abroad destinations, we considered time abroad holistically in the present study. Future examination of the role of time abroad in specific contexts (e.g., to regions where the variable phenomenon is known to be prevalent versus those where it is not) will provide further insight into the role of this experiential predictor. |
3 | Although we foreground correlations that included rates of production and/or selection of deleted /d/ in the current section, we performed one other Spearman test for the characteristics mentioned in the section. The test for EIT score and months abroad revealed a significant, positive correlation, r(19) = 0.54 [95% CI: 0.14, 0.79], p = 0.012. This supports patterns that can be gleaned from Table 3 and Table 5: learners with higher EIT scores had generally spent longer periods abroad. |
4 | Nevertheless, recall that the individual lexical item was entered as a random effect in the current and prior study’s regression models, which helps to not overly attribute the role of certain categories of predictors (e.g., high frequency, preceding -o as vowel) to (a) particular lexical item(s) (see Chapter 5 of Tagliamonte, 2012). |
References
- Adamson, D., & Regan, V. (1991). The acquisition of community speech norms by Asian immigrants learning English as a second language: A preliminary study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alba, O. (1999). Elisión de la/d/intervocálica postónica en el español dominicano. In A. Morales, J. Cardona, H. López Morales, & E. Forastieri (Eds.), Estudios de la lingüística hispánica (pp. 3–21). Homenaje a María Vaquero. [Google Scholar]
- Alvord, S. M., & Christiansen, D. E. (2012). Factors influencing the acquisition of Spanish voiced stop spirantization during an extended stay abroad. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 5, 239–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, W., & Smith, L. (2010). The impact of L2 dialect on L2 learning: Leaning Quebecois versus European French. Canadian Modern Language Review, 66, 711–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedinghaus, R., & Sedó, B. (2014). Intervocalic/d/deletion in Málaga: Frequency effects and linguistic factors. IULC Working Papers, 14(2), 62–79. [Google Scholar]
- Blas Arroyo, J. L. (2006). Hasta aquí hemos llega(d)o: ¿Un caso de variación morfológica? Factores estructurales y estilísticos en el español. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 25(2), 1–35. [Google Scholar]
- Bowden, H. W. (2016). Assessing second-language oral proficiency for research: The Spanish elicited imitation task. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 647–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canale, M., & Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied Linguistics, 1, 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caravedo, R. (1990). Sociolingüística del español de Lima. Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú. [Google Scholar]
- Cedergren, H. (1973). The interplay of social and linguistic factors in Panama [Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University]. [Google Scholar]
- Chappell, W., & Kanwit, M. (2022). Do learners connect sociophonetic variation with regional and social characteristics? The case of L2 perception of Spanish aspiration. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44, 185–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalola, A., & Bullock, B. E. (2017). On sociophonetic competence: Phrase-final vowel devoicing in native and advanced L2 speakers of French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 769–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, M. (2016). Corpus del español: Web/Dialects. Available online: http://www.corpusdelespanol.org/web-dial/ (accessed on 15 April 2019).
- Del Saz, M. (2019). Native and nonnative perception of western Andalusian Spanish/s/aspiration in quiet and noise. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(4), 673–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Introno, F., & Sosa, J. M. (1986). Elisión de la/d/en el español de Caracas: Aspectos sociolingüísticos e implicaciones teóricas. In R. Núñez Cedeño, I. Páez, & J. Guitart (Eds.), Estudios sobre la fonología de español del caribe (pp. 135–163). Casa de Bello. [Google Scholar]
- Díaz-Campos, M., & Gradoville, M. (2011). An analysis of frequency as a factor contributing to the diffusion of variable phenomena: Evidence from Spanish data. In L. A. Ortiz-López (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the 13th hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 224–238). Cascadilla. [Google Scholar]
- Díaz-Campos, M., Fafulas, S., & Gradoville, M. (2011). Going retro: An analysis of the interplay between socioeconomic class and age in Caracas Spanish. In J. Michnowicz, & R. Dodsworth (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 5th workshop on Spanish sociolinguistics (pp. 65–78). Cascadilla. [Google Scholar]
- Erker, D., & Guy, G. R. (2012). The role of lexical frequency in syntactic variability: Variable subject personal pronoun expression in Spanish. Language, 88, 526–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Escalante, C. (2018a). The acquisition of a sociolinguistic variable while volunteering abroad: S-weakening among L2 and heritage speakers in coastal Ecuador [Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California]. [Google Scholar]
- Escalante, C. (2018b). ¡ Ya pué [h]! Perception of coda-/s/weakening among L2 and heritage speakers in coastal Ecuador. EuroAmerican Journal of Applied Linguistics and Languages, 5(1), 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Escalante, C. (2021). Individual differences in dialectal accommodation: Case studies of heritage speakers volunteering in coastal Ecuador. In R. Pozzi, T. Quan, & C. Escalante (Eds.), Heritage speakers of Spanish and study abroad (pp. 77–97). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Face, T. L., & Menke, M. R. (2009). Acquisition of the Spanish voiced spirants by second language learners. In J. Collentine, M. García, B. Lafford, & F. Marcos Marín (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 11th hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 39–52). Cascadilla. [Google Scholar]
- Geeslin, K. L., & Gudmestad, A. (2008). The acquisition of variation in second-language Spanish: An agenda for integrating studies of the L2 sound system. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5(2), 137–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geeslin, K. L., & Long, A. Y. (2014). Sociolinguistics and second language acquisition: Learning to use language in context. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Geeslin, K. L., & Schmidt, L. B. (2018). Study abroad and L2 learner attitudes. In C. Sanz, & A. Morales-Front (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of study abroad research and practice (pp. 387–405). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Geeslin, K. L., Gudmestad, A., Kanwit, M., Linford, B., Long, A. Y., Schmidt, L., & Solon, M. (2018). Sociolinguistic competence and the acquisition of speaking. In M. R. Alonso Alonso (Ed.), Speaking in a second language (pp. 1–25). John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- George, A. (2014). Study abroad in Central Spain: The development of regional phonological features. Foreign Language Annals, 47, 97–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grammon, D. (2024). Ideology, indexicality, and the second language development of sociolinguistic perception during study abroad. L2 Journal, 16(1), 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hernández-Campoy, J. M., & Jiménez-Cano, J. M. (2003). Broadcasting standardisation: An análisis of the linguistic normalization process in Murcian Spanish. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 7(3), 321–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howard, M., Lemée, I., & Regan, V. (2006). The L2 acquisition of a phonological variable: The case of/l/deletion in French. Journal of French Language Studies, 16, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanwit, M., & Solon, M. (Eds.). (2023). Communicative competence in a second language: Theory, method, and applications. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy Terry, K. (2017). Contact, context, and collocation: The emergence of sociostylistic variation in L2 French learners during study abroad. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 553–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kennedy Terry, K. (2022). Sociolinguistic variation in L2 French: What schwa deletion patterns reveal about language acquisition during study abroad. In R. Bayley, D. R. Preston, & X. Li (Eds.), Variation in second and heritage languages: Crosslinguistic perspectives (pp. 279–310). John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Knouse, S. M. (2012). The acquisition of dialectal phonemes in a study abroad context: The case of the Castilian theta. Foreign Language Annals, 45, 512–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lord, G. (2010). The combined effects of immersion and instruction on second language pronunciation. Foreign Language Annals, 43, 488–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López Morales, H. (1983). Estratificación social del español de San Juan de Puerto Rico. Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. [Google Scholar]
- Moya Corral, J. A., & García Wiedemann, E. J. (2009). La elisión de/d/intervocálica en el español culto de Granada: Factores lingüísticos. Pragmalingüística, 17, 92–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nagle, C. L., & Baese-Berk, M. M. (2022). Advancing the state of the art in L2 speech perception-production research: Revisiting theoretical assumptions and methodological practices. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44(2), 580–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortega, L., Iwashita, N., Norris, J. M., & Rabie, S. (2002, October 3–6). An investigation of elicited imitation tasks in crosslinguistic SLA research. Second Language Research Forum, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Pfenninger, S. E., & Festman, J. (2021). Psycholinguistics. In T. Gregerson, & S. Mercer (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of the psychology of language learning and teaching (pp. 74–86). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Pope, J. (2023). Individual differences in the adoption of dialectal features during study abroad. In S. Zahler, A. Y. Long, & B. Linford (Eds.), Study abroad and the second language acquisition of sociolinguistic variation in Spanish (pp. 147–173). John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Pozzi, R. (2022). Acquiring sociolinguistic competence during study abroad: U.S. students in Buenos Aires. In R. Bayley, D. R. Preston, & X. Li (Eds.), Variation in second and heritage languages: Crosslinguistic perspectives (pp. 199–222). John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Pozzi, R., & Bayley, R. (2021). The development of a regional phonological feature during a semester abroad in Argentina. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43, 109–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raish, M. (2015). The acquisition of an Egyptian phonological variant by U.S. students in Cairo. Foreign Language Annals, 48, 267–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Regan, V., Howard, M., & Lemée, I. (2009). The acquisition of sociolinguistic competence in a study abroad context. Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar]
- Rehner, K. (2002). The development of aspects of linguistic and discourse competence by advanced second language learners of French [Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto]. [Google Scholar]
- Ringer-Hilfinger, K. (2012). Learner acquisition of dialect variation in a study abroad context: The case of the Spanish [θ]. Foreign Language Annals, 45, 430–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samper Padilla, J. A. (1990). Estudio sociolingüístico del español de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. Imprenta Pérez Galdós. [Google Scholar]
- Samper, & Padilla, J. A. (1996). El debilitamiento de-/d/-en la norma culta de las Palmas de Gran Canaria. In M. Arjona Iglesias, J. López Chávez, A. Enríquez Ovando, G. C. López Lara, & M. A. Novella Gómez (Eds.), Actas del X Congreso Internacional de la Asociación de Lingüística y Filología de la América Latina (pp. 791–796). Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, L. B. (2009). The effect of dialect familiarity via a study abroad experience on L2 comprehension of Spanish. In J. Collentine, M. García, B. Lafford, & F. M. Marín (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 11th hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 143–154). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, L. B. (2018). L2 development of perceptual categorization of dialectal sounds: A study in Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 847–882. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, L. B. (2022). L2 development of dialect awareness in Spanish. Hispania, 105(2), 267–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, L. B., & Geeslin, K. L. (2022). Acquisition of linguistic competence: Development of sociolinguistic evaluations of regional varieties in a second language. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics, 35(1), 206–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoonmaker-Gates, E. (2017). Regional variation in the language classroom and beyond: Mapping learners’ developing dialectal competence. Foreign Language Annals, 50, 177–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, L. C., & Baker, W. (2011). L2 dialect acquisition of German vowels: The case of Northern German and Austrian dialects. Poznán Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 47, 120–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solon, M., & Kanwit, M. (2022). New methods for tracking development of sociophonetic competence: Exploring a preference task for Spanish/d/deletion. Applied Linguistics, 43(4), 805–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solon, M., Linford, B., & Geeslin, K. L. (2018). Acquisition of sociophonetic variation: Intervocalic/d/reduction in native and non-native Spanish. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada, 31(1), 309–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solon, M., Park, H., Henderson, C., & Dehghan-Chaleshtori, M. (2019). Revisiting the Spanish elicited imitation task: A tool for assessing advanced language learners? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 1027–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tagliamonte, S. A. (2012). Variationist sociolinguistics: Change, observation, interpretation. Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Trimble, J. C. (2014). The intelligibility of Spanish dialects from the L2 learner’s perspective: The importance of phonological variation and dialect familiarity. International Journal of the Linguistic Association of the Southwest, 33(2), 31–57. [Google Scholar]
- Uritescu, D., Mougeon, R., & Handouleh, Y. (2002). Le comportement du schwa dans le français parlé par les élèves des programmes d’immersion française. In C. Tatilon, & A. Baudot (Eds.), La Linguistique fonctionnelle au tournant du siècle. Actes du Vingt-quatrième Colloque international de linguistique fonctionnelle (pp. 335–346). Éditions du GREF. [Google Scholar]
- Uruburu Bidaurrázaga, A. (1994). El tratamiento del fonema/a/en posición intervocálica en la lengua hablada en Córdoba (España). La Linguistique, 30, 85–104. [Google Scholar]
- Zampini, M. (1994). The role of native language transfer and task formality in the acquisition of Spanish spirantization. Hispania, 77, 470–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Regional Variety of Spanish | Source | Rate of /d/ Deletion |
---|---|---|
Caracas, Venezuela | Díaz-Campos and Gradoville (2011) | 10% |
Málaga, Spain | Bedinghaus and Sedó (2014) | 15% |
Panama City, Panama | Cedergren (1973) | 20% |
San Juan, Puerto Rico | López Morales (1983) | 21% |
Lima, Peru | Caravedo (1990) | 23% |
Santiago, Dominican Republic | Alba (1999) | 23% |
Córdoba, Spain | Uruburu Bidaurrázaga (1994) | 33% |
Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Spain | Samper Padilla (1990) | 38% |
Group (by Proficiency Level) | n | Realized /d/ | Deleted /d/ | Total | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
All participants | k | % | k | % | k | % | |
Low | 13 | 314 | 75.5 | 102 | 24.5 | 416 | 100 |
Mid | 13 | 283 | 68.5 | 130 | 31.5 | 413 | 100 |
High | 24 | 513 | 67.0 | 253 | 33.0 | 766 | 100 |
Categorical participants removed | |||||||
Low | 12 | 282 | 73.4 | 102 | 26.6 | 384 | 100 |
Mid | 11 | 220 | 62.9 | 130 | 37.1 | 350 | 100 |
High | 21 | 449 | 67.0 | 221 | 33.0 | 670 | 100 |
Learner Group | Speaker Gender | Preceding Vowel | Grammatical Category | Lexical Frequency |
---|---|---|---|---|
Low | (man) | ** (/i/) | * (verbs, adjectives) | (frequent) |
Mid | * (man) | (/a/) | (adjectives, participles) | (frequent) |
High | *** (man) | *** (/a/) | * (adjectives, participles) | (not frequent) |
Participant | Group from Solon and Kanwit (2022) | EIT Score | Age | Institutional Level | Years Studying Spanish | Months Abroad |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Jen | Low | 37 | 18 | 2 | 8 | 0.25 |
Anna | Low | 46 | 21 | 3 | 10 | 0 |
Emma | Low | 52 | 23 | 3 | 2 | 0 |
Kara | Low | 59 | 20 | 2 | 11 | 0.5 |
Ashley | Low | 62 | 77 | 2 | 8 | 14 |
Jeff | Mid | 81 | 21 | 4 | 9 | 1 |
Lauren | Mid | 87 | 19 | 3 | 5 | 0.75 |
Ty | Mid | 93 | 32 | 4 | 5 | 22.5 |
Stacy | Mid | 99 | 24 | 5 | 6 | 0 |
Joe | High | 104 | 22 | 5 | 7 | 1 |
Maura | High | 109 | 28 | 5 | 15 | 24 |
Tiff | High | 110 | 28 | 5 | 14 | 20 |
Maddie | High | 112 | 30 | 5 | 19 | 10 |
Lainey | High | 112 | 28 | 5 | 11 | 14 |
Tim | High | 112 | 28 | 5 | 15 | 15 |
Alexa | High | 114 | 28 | 5 | 12 | 4 |
Kenzie | High | 114 | 33 | 5 | 19 | 21 |
Mark | High | 114 | 31 | 5 | 18 | 26 |
Steph | High | 116 | 27 | 5 | 15 | 4.5 |
Jade | High | 119 | 27 | 5 | 14 | 18 |
Zadie | High | 120 | 32 | 5 | 11 | 9 |
Group M (SD) | 93.91 (26.78) | 28.43 (12.02) | 4.19 (1.17) | 11.14 (4.80) | 9.79 (9.35) |
Variable | Estimate | 95% CI | SE | z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −3.43 | [−4.13, −2.73] | 0.36 | −9.59 | <0.001 |
EIT Score (continuous) | 0.05 | [0.02, 0.08] | 0.01 | 3.49 | <0.001 |
Lexical Frequency | |||||
Frequent | reference | ||||
Not frequent | −1.16 | [−1.86, −0.46] | 0.36 | −3.25 | 0.001 |
Preceding Vowel | |||||
/a/ | reference | ||||
/o/ | 0.95 | [−0.06, 1.96] | 0.52 | 1.85 | 0.065 |
/e/ | −0.24 | [−1.21, 0.73] | 0.50 | −0.49 | 0.624 |
/u/ | −1.67 | [−2.81, −0.53] | 0.58 | −2.87 | 0.004 |
/i/ | −2.17 | [−3.05, −1.28] | 0.45 | −4.80 | <0.001 |
Grammatical Category | |||||
Adjective | reference | ||||
Noun | 0.16 | [−0.67, 0.99] | 0.42 | 0.38 | 0.705 |
Participle | −0.33 | [−1.49, 0.82] | 0.59 | −0.57 | 0.572 |
Verb | −0.38 | [−1.47, 0.71] | 0.56 | −0.69 | 0.493 |
Pronoun | −1.10 | [−1.81, −0.38] | 0.37 | −3.00 | 0.003 |
Adverb | −1.70 | [−3.55, 0.16] | 0.94 | −1.80 | 0.073 |
Random Effects | Variance | SD | N | ||
Participant | 0.54 | 0.74 | 17 | ||
Word | 1.04 | 1.08 | 323 |
Group | EIT Score | Participant | Deletion Rate, Production | Deletion Rate, Selection | Months Abroad |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low | 37 | Jen | 0.00% | 31.25% | 0.25 |
Low | 46 | Anna | 0.00% | 15.63% | 0 |
Low | 52 | Emma | 2.82% | 0.00% | 0 |
Low | 59 | Kara | 0.00% | 37.50% | 0.5 |
Low | 62 | Ashley | 0.00% | 12.50% | 14 |
Mid | 81 | Jeff | 1.23% | 0.00% | 1 |
Mid | 87 | Lauren | 8.33% | 46.88% | 0.75 |
Mid | 93 | Ty | 6.25% | 0.00% | 22.5 |
Mid | 99 | Stacy | 4.26% | 34.38% | 0 |
High | 104 | Joe | 3.28% | 21.88% | 1 |
High | 109 | Maura | 18.02% | 25.00% | 24 |
High | 110 | Tiff | 29.49% | 31.25% | 20 |
High | 112 | Maddie | 2.44% | 40.63% | 10 |
High | 112 | Lainey | 22.68% | 34.38% | 14 |
High | 112 | Tim | 13.91% | 28.13% | 15 |
High | 114 | Alexa | 11.76% | 18.75% | 4 |
High | 114 | Kenzie | 8.75% | 0.00% | 21 |
High | 114 | Mark | 12.24% | 50.00% | 26 |
High | 116 | Steph | 24.11% | 50.00% | 4.5 |
High | 119 | Jade | 13.40% | 31.25% | 18 |
High | 120 | Zadie | 12.75% | 37.50% | 9 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Solon, M.; Kanwit, M. Exploring the Relationship Between Preference and Production as Indicators of L2 Sociophonetic Competence. Languages 2025, 10, 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10040065
Solon M, Kanwit M. Exploring the Relationship Between Preference and Production as Indicators of L2 Sociophonetic Competence. Languages. 2025; 10(4):65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10040065
Chicago/Turabian StyleSolon, Megan, and Matthew Kanwit. 2025. "Exploring the Relationship Between Preference and Production as Indicators of L2 Sociophonetic Competence" Languages 10, no. 4: 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10040065
APA StyleSolon, M., & Kanwit, M. (2025). Exploring the Relationship Between Preference and Production as Indicators of L2 Sociophonetic Competence. Languages, 10(4), 65. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10040065