Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Working Memory Components in Bilingual Children with DLD
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Methodology
3. Results
3.1. Overall Results on Working Memory Measures
3.2. Specific Measures of VSTM
4. Discussion
4.1. No Differences Were Found between Monolingual and Bilingual Children with DLD with Respect to the Working Memory Components VSTM and CE
4.2. Children with TD Outperformed Children with DLD on All Measures of Working Memory with the Highest Effect Size Obtained for NWR
4.3. Limitations of the Study
4.4. Implications for Practice and Research
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Note that such latter CE functions (i.e., shifting, inhibition and updating of information) have also been discussed as specific facets of basal executive functions (EF) (cf. Miyake et al. 2000). |
2 | Note that verbal memory tasks proved to be more difficult than non-verbal tasks (for instance, visual-spatial) in children with DLD (cf. Vugs et al. 2013 for review of non-verbal memory skills and for studies including bilingual children cf. e.g., Engel de Abreu et al. 2014). |
References
- Adesope, Olusola, Tracy Lavin, Terri Thompson, and Charles Ungerleider. 2010. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Cognitive Correlates of Bilingualism. Review of Educational Research 80: 207–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Archibald, Lisa M. D. 2018. The reciprocal influences of working memory and linguistic knowledge on language performance: Considerations for the assessment of children with developmental language disorder. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49: 424–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Archibald, Lisa M. D., and Susan E. Gathercole. 2007. Nonword repetition and serial recall: Equivalent measures of verbal short-term memory? Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 587–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arizmendi, Genesis D., Mary Alt, Shelley Gray, Tiffany P. Hogan, Samuel Green, and Nelson Cowan. 2018. Do bilingual children have an executive function advantage? Results from inhibition, shifting, and updating Tasks. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49: 356–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Armon-Lotem, Sharon. 2012. Introduction: Bilingual children with SLI—The nature of the problem. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15: 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Armon-Lotem, Sharon, Jan de Jong, and Natalia Meir. 2015. Assessing Multilingual Children Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Communication Disorders across Languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan. 2003. Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders 36: 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan, and Graham Hitch. 1974. Working Memory. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Gordon H. Bower. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Cambridge: Academic Press, vol. 8, pp. 47–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, Alan, Graham Hitch, and Richard Allen. 2021. A Multicomponent Model of Working Memory. In Working Memory. Edited by Alan Baddeley, Graham Hitch and Richard Allen. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 10–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2011. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 65: 229–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Fergus I. M. Craik, Raymond Klein, and Mythili Viswanathan. 2004. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging 19: 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bishop, Dorothy V. M., Margaret J. Snowling, Paul A. Thompson, and Trisha Greenhalgh. 2017. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 58: 1068–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blom, Elma, Aylin C. Küntay, Marielle Messer, Josje Verhagen, and Paul Leseman. 2014. The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 128: 105–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blom, Elma, and Tessel Boerma. 2017. Effects of language impairment and bilingualism across domains. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7: 277–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boerma, Tessel, and Elma Blom. 2020. Effects of developmental language disorder and bilingualism on children’s executive functioning: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 107: 103782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buac, Milijana, Megan Gross, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2016. Predictors of processing-based task performance in bilingual and monolingual children. Journal of Communication Disorders 62: 12–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Calder, Samuel D., Mary Claessen, Suze Leitão, and Susan Ebbels. 2022. A profile of expressive inflectional morphology in early school-age children with developmental language disorder. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 36: 341–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chiat, Shula. 2015. Nonword repetition. In Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment. Edited by Sharon Armon-Lotem, Jan de Jong and Natalia Meir. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 125–50. [Google Scholar]
- Chiat, Shula, and Kamila Polišenská. 2016. A framework for crosslinguistic nonword repetition Tests: Effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 59: 1179–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Almeida, Laetitia, Sandrine Ferré, Marie-Anne Barthez, and Christophe Dos Santos. 2019. What do monolingual and bilingual children with and without SLI produce when phonology is too complex? First Language 39: 158–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delage, Hélène, and Ulrich Hans Frauenfelder. 2020. Relationship between working memory and complex syntax in children with Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Child Language 47: 600–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delage, Hélène, Emily Stanford, and Stephanie Durrleman. 2021. Working memory training enhances complex syntax in children with Developmental Language Disorder. Applied Psycholinguistics 42: 1341–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engel de Abreu, Pascale M. J. 2011. Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory 19: 529–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engel de Abreu, Pascale M. J., Anabela Cruz-Santos, and Marina L. Puglisi. 2014. Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 49: 736–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Engel, Pascale Marguerite Josiane, Flávia Santos, and Susan Elizabeth Gathercole. 2008. Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 51: 1580–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Frizelle, Pauline, and Paul Fletcher. 2015. The role of memory in processing relative clauses in children with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 24: 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, Susan Elizabeth, and Tracy Packiam Alloway. 2006. Practitioner review: Short-term and working memory impairments in neurodevelopmental disorders: Diagnosis and remedial support. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47: 4–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gathercole, Susan Elizabeth, Susan J. Pickering, Benjamin Ambridge, and Hannah Wearing. 2004. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology 40: 177–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Graf Estes, Katharine, Julia L. Evans, and Nicole M. Else-Quest. 2007. Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without Specific Language Impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50: 177–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grimm, Angela. 2022. The use of the LITMUS quasi-universal nonword repetition task to identify DLD in monolingual and early second language learners aged 8 to 10. Languages 7: 218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grimm, Angela, and Julia Hübner. forthcoming. Nonword repetition by bilingual learners of German. The role of language-specific complexity. In Bilingualism and Specific Language Impairment. Edited by Christophe dos Santos and Laetitia de Almeida. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
- Grimm, Angela, and Petra Schulz. 2021. Phonology and sentential semantics: Markers of SLI in bilingual children at age 6? In Language Impairment in Multilingual Settings: LITMUS in Action across Europe. Edited by Sharon Armon-Lotem and Kleanthes K. Grohmann. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 263–300. [Google Scholar]
- Grundy, John G., and Kalinka Timmer. 2017. Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research 33: 325–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hamann, Cornelia. 2017. Phonological working memory and language development: What are the measures and what do they measure? Applied Psycholinguistics 38: 1313–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hasselhorn, Marcus, Ruth Schumann-Hengsteler, Julia Gronauer, Dietmar Grube, Claudia Mähler, Inga Schmid, Katja Seitz-Stein, and Christof Zoelch. 2012. Arbeitsgedächtnistestbatterie für Kinder von 5 bis 12 Jahren—AGTB 5–12. Göttingen: Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Henry, Luca A., and Nicola Botting. 2017. Working memory and developmental language impairments. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 33: 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henry, Lucy A., Emma Christopher, Shula Chiat, and David J. Messer. 2022. A Short and Engaging Adaptive Working-Memory Intervention for Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Effects on Language and Working Memory. Brain Sciences 12: 642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hilchey, Matthew D., and Raymond M. Klein. 2011. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18: 625–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulk, Aafke, and Natascha Müller. 2000. Crosslinguistic influence at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3: 227–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulk, Aafke, and Sharon Unsworth. 2010. Bilingualism as a kind of therapy? Aplied Psycholinguistics 31: 297–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jakubowicz, Celia, and Lea Nash. 2001. Functional categories and syntactic operations in (Ab)normal language acquisition. Brain and Language 77: 321–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klem, Marianne, Monica Melby-Lervåg, Bente Hagtvet, Solveig-Alma Halaas Lyster, Jan-Eric Gustafsson, and Charles Hulme. 2015. Sentence repetition is a measure of children’s language skills rather than working memory limitations. Developmental Science 18: 146–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Korkman, Marit, Maria Stenroos, Annika Mickos, Martin Westman, Pia Ekholm, and Roger Byring. 2012. Does simultaneous bilingualism aggravate chidlren’s specific language problems? Acta Paediatrica 101: 946–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krizman, Jennifer, Erika Skoe, Viorica Marian, and Nina Kraus. 2014. Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and Language 128: 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, Laurence B. 2014. Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marini, Andrea, Nadezda Eliseeva, and Franco Fabbro. 2019. Impact of early second-language acquisition on the development of first language and verbal short-term and working memory. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 22: 165–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marshall, Chloë. 2020. Investigating the relationship between syntactic and short-term/working memory impairments in children with developmental disorders is not a straightforward endeavour. First Language 40: 491–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meir, Natalia. 2017. Effects of specific language impairment (SLI) and bilingualism on verbal short-term memory. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7: 301–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, Monica, and Charles Hulme. 2013. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology 49: 270–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mishra, Ramesh Kumar, and Jubin Abutalebi, eds. 2020. Cognitive Consequences of Bilingualism. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science 4. Available online: https://link.springer.com/journal/41809/volumes-and-issues/4-2 (accessed on 30 May 2022).
- Miyake, Akira, Naomi P. Friedman, Michael J. Emerson, Alexander H. Witzki, Amy Howerter, and Tor D. Wager. 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41: 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Montgomery, James W., Julia L. Evans, Jamison D. Fargo, Sarah Schwartz, and Ronald B. Gillam. 2018. Structural relationship between cognitive processing and syntactic sentence comprehension in children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 61: 2950–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Müller, Natascha, Katja Cantone, Tanja Kupisch, and Katrin Schmitz. 2002. Zum Spracheneinfluss im bilingualen Erstspracherwerb: Italienisch-Deutsch. Linguistische Berichte 190: 157–206. [Google Scholar]
- Orgassa, Antje, and Fred Weerman. 2008. Dutch gender in specific language impairment and second language acquisition. Second Language Research 24: 333–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2010. The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics 31: 227–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Johanne, Martha Crago, Fred Genesee, and Mabel Rice. 2003. French-English bilingual children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46: 113–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penke, Martina, and Eva Wimmer. 2020. Verbal short-term memory and sentence comprehension in German children and adolescents with Down syndrome: Beware of the task. First Language 40: 367–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penke, Martina, and Monika Rothweiler. 2018. Comparing specific language impairment and hearing impairment: Different profiles in German verbal agreement morphology. Language Acquisition 25: 39–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peristeri, Eleni, Eleni Baldimtsi, Stephanie Durrleman, and Ianthi Maria Tsimpli. 2019. Bilingualism effects in children with Developmental Language Disorder: Metalinguistic awareness, executive functions, and false-belief reasoning. In Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development. Edited by Megan M. Brown and Brady Dailey. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 523–36. Available online: http://www.lingref.com/bucld/43/BUCLD43-41.pdf (accessed on 30 May 2022).
- Petermann, Franz. 2017. WISC-V: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th ed. German version by Franz Petermann. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Pratt, Amy S., Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2021. Sentence repetition with bilinguals with and without DLD: Differential effects of memory, vocabulary, and exposure. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 24: 305–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Röhm, Alexander. 2020. Sprache, Arbeitsgedächtnis und mathematische Kompetenz von Schulkindern mit SES. Lernen Und Lernstörungen 9: 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherger, Anna-Lena. 2016. Kasuserwerb bilingual deutsch-italienischer Kinder: Vorübergehend verzögernder Spracheneinfluss [The acquisition of case marking in German-Italian children: Temporarily delayed cross-linguistic influence]. Linguistische Berichte 246: 195–239. [Google Scholar]
- Scherger, Anna-Lena. 2018. German dative case marking in monolingual and simultaneous bilingual children with and without SLI. Journal of Communication Disorders 75: 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherger, Anna-Lena. 2020. Erprobung von LITMUS-Screenings für SES bei Mehrsprachigkeit—Morphosyntax und phonologische Komplexität [Testing LITMUS diagnostic tools for DLD in bilingualism—Morphosyntax and phonological complexity]. Forschung Sprache 2: 10–25. [Google Scholar]
- Scherger, Anna-Lena. 2022. The role of age and timing in bilingual assessment: Non-word repetition, subject-verb agreement and case marking in L1 and eL2 children with and without SLI. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 36: 54–74. [Google Scholar]
- Stanford, Emily, Stephanie Durrleman, and Hélène Delage. 2019. The effect of working memory training on a clinical marker of French-speaking children with Developmental Language Disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 28: 1388–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steenge, Judit. 2006. Bilingual Children with Specific Language Impairment: Additionally Disadvantaged? Nijmegen: EAC, Research Centre on Atypical Communication. Available online: https://repository.ubn.ru.nl/bitstream/handle/2066/29807/29807.pdf (accessed on 30 May 2022).
- Talli, Ionna, and Stavroula Stavrakaki. 2020. Short-term memory, working memory and linguistic abilities in bilingual children with Developmental Language Disorder. First Language 40: 437–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thordardottir, Elin, and Myrto Brandeker. 2013. The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders 46: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuller, Laurice. 2015. Clinical use of parental questionnaires in multilingual contexts. In Assessing Multilingual Children Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment (Communication Disorders across Languages: Vol. 13). Edited by Sharon Armon-Lotem, Jan de Jong and Natalia Meir. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, pp. 299–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuller, Laurice, Cornelia Hamann, Solveig Chilla, Sandrine Ferré, Eléonore Morin, Philippe Prévost, Christophe dos Santos, Lina Abed Ibrahim, and Racha Zebib. 2018. Identifying language impairment in bilingual children in France and in Germany. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 53: 888–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van den Noort, Maurits, Esli Struys, and Peggy Bosch, eds. 2019. Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage-Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve. Behavioral Sciences 9. Available online: https://www.mdpi.com/books/book/2015-individual-variation-and-the-bilingual-advantage-factors-that-modulate-the-effect-of-bilingualism (accessed on 30 May 2022).
- Vugs, Brigitte, Juliane Cuperus, Marc Hendriks, and Ludo Verhoeven. 2013. Visuospatial working memory in specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34: 2586–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wimmer, Eva, Bernadette Witecy, and Martina Penke. 2021. Frag mal, wer…! Produktion von w-Fragen bei Kindern, Jugendlichen und Erwachsenen mit Down-Syndrom: [Ask, Who…! Production of wh-Questions in Children, Adolescents and Adults with Down Syndrome]. Forschung Sprache 9: 132–50. Available online: https://www.forschung-sprache.eu/fileadmin/user_upload/Dateien/Heftausgaben/2021-2/5-70-2021-02-12.pdf (accessed on 30 May 2022).
- Zebib, Racha, Laurice Tuller, Cornelia Hamann, Lina Abed Ibrahim, and Philippe Prévost. 2020. Syntactic complexity and verbal working memory in bilingual children with and without Developmental Language Disorder. First Language 40: 461–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziethe, Anke, Ulrich Eysholdt, and Michael Doellinger. 2013. Sentence repetition and digit span: Potential markers of bilingual children with suspected SLI? Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology 38: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Groups | Monolingual (Mo)/ Bilingual (Bi) | Number (Sex) | Mean Age (Range) | Age of Onset | Length of Exposure | Amount of L2 Exposure at Home |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
TD | MoTD | N = 11 (N = 6 female) | 7;1 (6;1–8;6) | -- | -- | -- |
BiTD | N = 10 (N = 4 female) | 7;10 (6;11–8;6) | 2;11 (2;10–4;3) | 54.2 (45–66) | 2.63 (SD = 0.6) | |
DLD | MoDLD | N = 8 (N = 2 female) | 7;8 (6;1–8;5) | -- | -- | -- |
BiDLD | N = 13 (N = 7 female) | 7;6 (6;3–8;3) | 3;1 (2;0–4;0) | 53.1 (45–60) | 2.29 (SD = 1.0) |
Word Length | Nonword Examples |
---|---|
monosyllabic (N = 2) | pli, kip |
bisyllabic (N = 13) | pukif, fluka, klifak, kafip |
trisyllabic (N = 17) | sklipafu, kuflapi, kufiski, pifakup |
TD | DLD | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
MoTD | BiTD | MoDLD | BiDLD | |
NWR | ||||
M | 75.6% | 81.0% | 30.9% | 35.8% |
SD | 9.1 | 18.1 | 18.6 | 22.9 |
range | 59.4–93.8 | 34.4–100 | 3.1–53.1 | 3.1–68.8 |
DS forward | ||||
M | 4.27 | 4.30 | 3.88 | 3.23 |
SD | 0.62 | 0.64 | 0.33 | 0.70 |
range | 3–5 | 3–4 | 3–4 | 2–4 |
DS backward | ||||
M | 2.91 | 2.78 | 2.63 | 2.00 |
SD | 0.79 | 0.42 | 0.86 | 0.96 |
range | 2–5 | 2–3 | 2–4 | 0–3 |
Groups | Mono-/Bisyllabic | Trisyllabic | Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test |
---|---|---|---|
MoTD | |||
M | 80.0% | 70.1% | W = 6.0 |
SD | 12.7 | 16.3 | p = 0.008 |
range | 46.7–93.3 | 41.2–94.1 | |
BiTD | |||
M | 86.0% | 75.6% | W = 9.0 |
SD | 11.7 | 25.6 | p = 0.045 |
range | 60.0–100 | 17.6–100 | |
MoDLD | |||
M | 34.2% | 27.2% | W = 7.0 |
SD | 15.1 | 22.8 | p = 0.938 |
range | 13.3–53.3 | 0.0–64.7 | |
BiDLD | |||
M | 41.0% | 33.9% | W = 20.5 |
SD | 20.1 | 24.0 | p = 0.959 |
range | 13.3–73.3 | 0.0–70.6 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wimmer, E.; Scherger, A.-L. Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference? Languages 2022, 7, 287. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040287
Wimmer E, Scherger A-L. Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference? Languages. 2022; 7(4):287. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040287
Chicago/Turabian StyleWimmer, Eva, and Anna-Lena Scherger. 2022. "Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference?" Languages 7, no. 4: 287. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040287
APA StyleWimmer, E., & Scherger, A. -L. (2022). Working Memory Skills in DLD: Does Bilingualism Make a Difference? Languages, 7(4), 287. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040287